Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-jbqgn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-17T17:35:09.005Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

22 - Training to be Creative: The Interplay between Cognition, Skill Learning, and Motivation

from Part VI - Reasoning and Intelligence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 January 2018

Rex E. Jung
Affiliation:
University of New Mexico
Oshin Vartanian
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abushanab, B., & Bishara, A. J. (2013). Memory and metacognition for piano melodies: Illusory advantages of fixed-over random-order practice. Memory & Cognition, 41, 928937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity: A componential conceptualization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 357376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context: Update to “The Social Psychology of Creativity”. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Amabile, T. M., & Pillemer, J. (2012). Perspectives on the social psychology of creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 46, 315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barbot, B., Tan, M., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2013). The genetics of creativity: The generative and receptive sides of the creativity equation. In Vartanian, O., Bristol, A. S., & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.), The neuroscience of creativity (pp. 7193). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkowitz, A. L., & Ansari, D. (2010). Expertise-related deactivation of the right temporoparietal junction during musical improvisation. NeuroImage, 49, 712719.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bilalić, M., McLeod, , , P., & Gobet, F. (2007). Does chess need intelligence? – A study with young chess players. Intelligence, 35, 457470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bilalić, M., Smallbone, , McLeod, K., , P., & Gobet, F. (2009). Why are (the best) women so good at chess? Participation rates and gender differences in intellectual domains. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 276, 11611165.Google ScholarPubMed
Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2011). Making things hard on yourself, but in a good way: Creating desirable difficulties to enhance learning. In Gernsbacher, M. A., Pew, R. W., & Hough, L. M. (Eds.), Psychology and the real world: Essays illustrating fundamental contributions to society (pp. 5664). Duffield: Worth.Google Scholar
Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings. In Metcalfe, J., & Shimamura, A. (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 185205). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development, 78, 246263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, B. S. (1985). Generalizations about talent development. In Bloom, B. S. (Ed.), Developing talent in young people (pp. 507549). New York, NY: Ballantine Books.Google Scholar
Bonneville-Roussy, A., & Bouffard, T. (2015). When quantity is not enough: Disentangling the roles of practice time, self-regulation and deliberate practice in musical achievement. Psychology of Music, 43, 686704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryan, W. L., & Harter, N. (1897). Studies in the physiology and psychology of the telegraphic language. Psychological Review, 4, 2753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryan, W.L., & Harter, N. (1899). Studies on the telegraphic language: The acquisition of a hierarchy of habits. Psychological Review, 6, 345375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charness, N., Krampe, R., & Mayr, U. (1996). The role of practice and coaching in entrepreneurial skill domains: An international comparison of life-span chess skill acquisition. In Ericsson, K. A. (Ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports, and games (pp. 5180). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skill and working memory. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 16, pp. 158). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 5581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clapham, M. M., & Schuster, D. H. (1992). Can engineering students be trained to think more creatively. Journal of Creative Behavior, 26, 165171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claxton, A. F., Pannells, T. C., & Rhoads, P. A. (2005). Developmental trends in the creativity of school-age children. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 327335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colquitt, J. A., LePine, J. A., & Noe, R. A. (2000). Toward an integrative theory of training motivation: A meta-analytic path analysis of 20 years of research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N. (2005). Working memory capacity. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
De Dreu, C. K., Nijstad, B. A., Baas, M., Wolsink, I., & Roskes, M. (2012). Working memory benefits creative insight, musical improvisation, and original ideation through maintained task-focused attention. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 656669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Groot, A. D. (1946). Het denken van den schaker. Amsterdam: Noord Hollandsche.Google Scholar
de Groot, A. D. (1965). Thought and choice in chess. The Hague: Mouton Publishers.Google Scholar
Djakow, I., Petrowski, N., & Rudik, P. (1927). Psychologie des Schachspiels. Berlin: de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U., & May, A. (2004). Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature, 427, 311312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dweck, C. (2015). Growth mindset, revisited. Education Week, 35, 20.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dweck, C. S., Mangels, J. A., & Good, C. (2004). Motivational effects on attention, cognition, and performance. In Dai, D. Y., & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), Motivation, emotion, and cognition: Integrative perspectives on intellectual functioning and development (Part II, pp. 4155). Abingdon: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ericsson, A., & Pool, R. (2016). Peak: Secrets from the new science of expertise. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2004). Deliberate practice and the acquisition and maintenance of expert performance in medicine and related domains. Academic Medicine, 79, S70S81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Feltovich, P. J., & Hoffman, R. R. (Eds.). (2006). The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Chase, W. G., & Faloon, S. (1980). Acquisition of a memory skill. Science, 208, 11811182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Etkin, J. (2016). The hidden cost of personal quantification. Journal of Consumer Research, 42, 967984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Norton, A., & Schlaug, G. (2008). Practicing a musical instrument in childhood is associated with enhanced verbal ability and nonverbal reasoning. PLoS ONE, 3, e3566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galamian, I. (1962). Principles of violin playing & teaching. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Galton, F. (1865). Heredity, talent and character. Macmillan’s Magazine, 12, 157166, 318327.Google Scholar
Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (1996). Templates in chess memory: A mechanism for recalling several boards. Cognitive Psychology, 31, 140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gobet, F., & Simon, H. (1998). Expert chess memory: Revisiting the chunking hypothesis. Memory, 6, 225255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haimovitz, K., Wormington, S. V., & Corpus, J. H. (2011). Dangerous mindsets: How beliefs about intelligence predict motivational change. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 747752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hennessey, B. A. (1996). Teaching for creative development: A social-psychological approach. In Colangelo, N. & Davis, G. (Eds.), Handbook of gifted education (2nd ed., pp. 282291). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Herholz, S. C., & Zatorre, R. J. (2012). Musical training as a framework for brain plasticity: Behavior, function, and structure. Neuron, 76, 486502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, R. W. (2009). Individual differences in expertise development over decades in a complex intellectual domain. Memory & Cognition, 37, 194209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howard, R. W. (2011). Does high-level intellectual performance depend on practice alone? Debunking the Polgar sisters case. Cognitive Development, 26, 196202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans, A. C., & Schlaug, G. (2009). The effects of musical training on structural brain development. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 182186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacoby, L. L., Bjork, R. A., & Kelley, C. M. (1994). Illusions of comprehension, competence, and remembering. In Druckman, D. & Bjork, R A. (Eds.), Learning, remembering, believing: Enhancing human performance (pp. 5780). Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Jauk, E., Benedek, M., Dunst, B., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). The relationship between intelligence and creativity: New support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection. Intelligence, 41, 212221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, F. S. (1958). The phantom plateau. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, K. H. (2011). The creativity crisis: The decrease in creative thinking scores on the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 285295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klingberg, T. (2012). Is working memory capacity fixed? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1, 194196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Limb, C. J., & Braun, A. R. (2008). Neural substrates of spontaneous musical performance: An fMRI study of jazz improvisation. PLoS ONE, 3, e1679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, C. H. J., Chiang, M. C., Wu, A. D., Iacoboni, M., Udompholkul, P., Yazdanshenas, O., & Knowlton, B. J. (2012). Age related differences in the neural substrates of motor sequence learning after interleaved and repetitive practice. NeuroImage, 62, 20072020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, C. H. J., Knowlton, B. J., Chiang, M. C., Iacoboni, M., Udompholkul, P., & Wu, A. D. (2011). Brain–behavior correlates of optimizing learning through interleaved practice. NeuroImage, 56, 17581772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ma, W. J., Husain, M., & Bays, P. M. (2014). Changing concepts of working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 347356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mangels, J. A., Butterfield, B., Lamb, J., Good, C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Why do beliefs about intelligence influence learning success? A social cognitive neuroscience model. Social, Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 7586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McPherson, G. E., & McCormick, J. (1999 ). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of musical practice. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 141, 98102.Google Scholar
McPherson, G. E., & McCormick, J. (2006). Self-efficacy and music performance. Psychology of Music, 34, 322336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meador, K. S. (1994). The effects of synectics training on gifted and non-gifted kindergarten students. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 18, 5573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 8197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can undermine children’s motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 3352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mumford, M. D., Mobley, M. I., Reiter-Palmon, R., Uhlman, C. E., & Doares, L. M. (1991). Process analytic models of creative capacities. Creativity Research Journal, 4, 91122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinho, A. L., de Manzano, Ö., Fransson, P., Eriksson, H., & Ullén, F. (2014). Connecting to create: Expertise in musical improvisation is associated with increased functional connectivity between premotor and prefrontal areas. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 61566163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pressing, J. (1988). Improvisation: Methods and models. In Sloboda, J. A. (Ed.), Generative processes in music: The psychology of performance, improvisation, and composition (pp. 129178). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Reder, L., & Klatzky, R. L. (1994). Transfer: Training for performance. In Druckman, D., & Bjork, R. A. (Eds.), Learning, remembering, believing: Enhancing human performance (pp. 2556). Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Reuter, M., Roth, S., Holve, K., & Hennig, J. (2006). Identification of first candidate genes for creativity: A pilot study. Brain Research, 1069, 190197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Runco, M. A., & Cayirdag, N. (2006). The development of children’s creativity. In Saracho, B., & Spodek, O. N (Eds.), Handbook of research on the education of young children (pp. 121131). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Jaeger, G. J. (2012). The standard definition of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 9296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saggar, M., Quintin, E. M., Bott, N. T., Kienitz, E., Chien, Y. H., Hong, D. W., … Reiss, A. L. (2016). Changes in brain activation associated with spontaneous improvisation and figural creativity after design-thinking-based training: A longitudinal fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, epub ahead of print.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saggar, M., Stankov, A., Schreiber, M., & Reiss, A.L. (2016). Finding the neural correlates of middle childhood “slump” in creativity. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
Scott, G., Leritz, L. E., & Mumford, M. D. (2004). The effectiveness of creativity training: A quantitative review. Creativity Research Journal, 16, 361388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shalley, C. E., & Gilson, L. L. (2004). What leaders need to know: A review of social and contextual factors that can foster or hinder creativity. The Leadership Quarterly, 15, 3353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Berg, C., Martin, C., & O’Connor, A. (2009). Openness to experience, plasticity, and creativity: Exploring lower-order, higher-order, and interactive effects. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 10871090.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Winerstein, B. P., Willse, J. T., Barona, C. M., Cram, J. T., Hess, K. I., … Richard, C. A. (2008). Assessing creativity with divergent thinking tasks: Exploring the reliability and validity of new subjective scoring methods. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2, 6885.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, H. A., & Gilmartin, K. (1973). A simulation of memory for chess positions. Cognitive Psychology, 5(1), 2946.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1997). Creative productivity: A predictive and explanatory model of career trajectories and landmarks. Psychological Review, 104, 6689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, G. F. (1998). Idea generation techniques: A formulary of active ingredients. Journal of Creative Behavior, 32, 107134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1991). An investment theory of creativity and its development. Human Development, 34(1), 131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (1968). A longitudinal examination of the fourth grade slump in creativity. Gifted Child Quarterly, 12, 195199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (1972). Can we teach children to think creatively? The Journal of Creative Behavior, 6, 114143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Unterrainer, J. M., Kaller, C. P., Halsband, U., & Rahm, B. (2006). Planning abilities and chess: A comparison of and non-chess players on the Tower of London. British Journal of Psychology, 97, 299311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woollett, K., & Maguire, E. A. (2011). Acquiring “the Knowledge” of London’s layout drives structural brain changes. Current Biology, 21, 21092114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×