Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vvkck Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T21:57:59.882Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Introduction

The New Science of Learning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

R. Keith Sawyer
Affiliation:
Washington University
R. Keith Sawyer
Affiliation:
Washington University, St Louis
Get access

Summary

By the twentieth century, all major industrialized countries offered formal schooling to all of their children. When these schools took shape in the nineteenth and twentieth centuries, scientists didn't know very much about how people learn. Even by the 1920s, when schools began to become the large bureaucratic institutions that we know today, there still was no sustained study of how people learn. As a result, the schools we have today were designed around commonsense assumptions that had never been tested scientifically:

  • Knowledge is a collection of facts about the world and procedures for how to solve problems. Facts are statements like “The earth is tilted on its axis by 23.45 degrees” and procedures are step-by-step instructions like how to do multidigit addition by carrying to the next column.

  • The goal of schooling is to get these facts and procedures into the student's head. People are considered to be educated when they possess a large collection of these facts and procedures.

  • Teachers know these facts and procedures, and their job is to transmit them to students.

  • Simpler facts and procedures should be learned first, followed by progressively more complex facts and procedures. The definitions of “simplicity” and “complexity” and the proper sequencing of material were determined either by teachers, by textbook authors, or by asking expert adults like mathematicians, scientists, or historians – not by studying how children actually learn.

  • The way to determine the success of schooling is to test students to see how many of these facts and procedures they have acquired.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baron, J. (1985). Rationality and intelligence. New York: Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bereiter, C. (2002). Education and mind in the knowledge age. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bobrow, D. G., & Collins, A. (1975). Representation and understanding: Studies in cognitive science. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bransford, J. D., Brown, A. L., & Cocking, R. R. (Eds.). (2000). How people learn: Brain, mind, experience, and school. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Burtis, P. J., Bereiter, C., Scardamalia, M., & Tetroe, J. (1983). The development of planning in writing. In Kroll, B. M. & Wells, G. (Eds.), Explorations in the development of writing: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 153–174). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge: Harvard.Google Scholar
Collins, A., & Brown, J. S. (1988). The computer as a tool for learning through reflection. In Mandl, H. & Lesgold, A. (Eds.), Learning issues for intelligent tutoring systems (pp. 1–18). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuban, L. (2001). Oversold and underused: Computers in the classroom. Cambridge, MA: Harvard.Google Scholar
Drucker, P. F. (1993). Post-capitalist society. New York: HarperBusiness.Google Scholar
Dunbar, K., & Klahr, D. (1989). Developmental differences in scientific discovery strategies. In Klahr, D. & Kotovsky, K. (Eds.), Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 109–143). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Flower, L., & Hayes, J. R. (1980). The cognition of discovery: Defining a rhetorical problem. College Composition and Communication, 31, 21–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heath, C., & Luff, P. (1991). Collaborative activity and technological design: Task coordination in the London Underground control rooms. Paper presented at the Proceedings of ECSCW '91.CrossRef
Hughes, J. A., Shapiro, D. Z., Sharrock, W. W., Anderson, R. J., & Gibbons, S. C. (1988). The automation of air traffic control (Final Report SERC/ESRC Grant no. GR/D/86257). Lancaster, UK: Department of Sociology, Lancaster University.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (Eds.). (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. New York: Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D. (1989). Children and adults as intuitive scientists. Psychological Review, 96, 674–689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, D. (1990). Introduction. In Kuhn, D. (Ed.), Developmental perspectives on teaching and learning thinking skills (pp. 1–8). Basel: Karger.Google Scholar
Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics, and culture in everyday life. New York: Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liebowitz, J. (Ed.). (1998). The handbook of applied expert systems. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.Google Scholar
McGuire, J. E. (1992). Scientific change: Perspectives and proposals. In Salmon, M., Earman, J., Glymour, C., Lennox, J., Machamer, P., McGuire, J., Norton, J., Salmon, W., & Schaffner, K. (Eds.), Introduction to the philosophy of science (pp. 132–178). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
National Center for History in the Schools. (1996). National standards for history. Los Angeles, CA: National Center for History in the Schools.
National Research Council. (1996). National science education standards. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Palincsar, A. S., & Brown, A. L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension fostering and comprehension monitoring. Cognition and Instruction, 1(2), 117–175.Google Scholar
Papert, S. (1980). Mindstorms: Children, computers, and powerful ideas. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Papert, S. (1993). The children's machine: rethinking school in the age of the computer. New York: BasicBooks.Google Scholar
Parsad, B., & Jones, J. (2005). Internet access in U. S. public schools and classrooms: 1994–2003 (NCES 2005-015). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B. (1998). Cognition as a collaborative process. In Kuhn, D. & Siegler, R. S. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, 5th edition, Volume 2: Cognition, perception, and language (pp. 679–744). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Salomon, G. (Ed.). (1993). Distributed cognitions: Psychological and educational considerations. New York: Cambridge.Google Scholar
Saxe, G. B. (1991). Culture and cognitive development: Studies in mathematical understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schauble, L. (1990). Belief revision in children: The role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 31–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scribner, S., & Cole, M. (1973). Cognitive consequences of formal and informal education. Science, 182(4112), 553–559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shavelson, R. J., & Towne, L. (2002). Scientific research in education. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Siegler, R. S. (1998). Children's thinking. (Third ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1954/1968). The science of learning and the art of teaching. In Skinner, B. F. (Ed.), The technology of teaching (pp. 9–28). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. (Original work published in 1954 in the Harvard Educational Review, Vol. 24, No. 2, pp. 86–97).Google Scholar
Sleeman, D., & Brown, J. S. (Eds.). (1982). Intelligent tutoring systems. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Suchman, L. A. (1987). Plans and situated actions: The problem of human-machine communication. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Suppe, F. (1974). The search for philosophic understanding of scientific theories. In Suppe, F. (Ed.), The structure of scientific theories (pp. 3–241). Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Voss, J. F., Perkins, D. N., & Segal, J. W. (Eds.). (1991). Informal reasoning and education. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wenger, E. (1987). Artificial intelligence and tutoring systems: Computational and cognitive approaches to the communication of knowledge. San Francisco, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Introduction
  • Edited by R. Keith Sawyer, Washington University, St Louis
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of the Learning Sciences
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511816833.002
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Introduction
  • Edited by R. Keith Sawyer, Washington University, St Louis
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of the Learning Sciences
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511816833.002
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Introduction
  • Edited by R. Keith Sawyer, Washington University, St Louis
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of the Learning Sciences
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511816833.002
Available formats
×