Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-gtxcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T11:22:22.269Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

20 - Imagining and Experiencing the Self on Cognitive Maps

from Part III - Intentionality-Based Forms of the Imagination

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2020

Anna Abraham
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Get access

Summary

The recent years have brought into the forefront of cognitive neuroscience a mechanistic representation of the world in the cognitive system – cognitive maps. A “zoo” of different cells transforms the immediate experience of wandering in the environment into a maplike representation, written again and again in specific brain structures. Here we claim that another component is crucial for forming and interpreting these maps – the experiential and imaginary self. Through the concept of mental travel we explain how the self may be projected prior to the use of cognitive maps and how the world is referred to the self through the cognitive map. Biases and influences of the self may further affect the maps formation and interpretation. The concept of mental orientation is then suggested to include the experiencing self and the way in which it relates to the environment, as represented on cognitive maps, not only in spatial navigation but also in the processing of time (memories and plans), people (social world), and even abstract concepts.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., and Schacter, D. L. (2007). Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future: Common and Distinct Neural Substrates During Event Construction and Elaboration. Neuropsychologia, 45(7), 13631377.Google Scholar
Anelli, F., Avanzi, S., Arzy, S., Mancuso, M., and Frassinetti, F. (2018a). Effects of Spatial Attention on Mental Time Travel in Patients with Neglect. CORTEX, 101, 192205.Google Scholar
Anelli, F., Ciaramelli, E., Arzy, S., and Frassinetti, F. (2016a). Prisms to Travel in Time: Investigation of Time-Space Association through Prismatic Adaptation Effect on Mental Time Travel. Cognition, 156, 15.Google Scholar
Anelli, F., Ciaramelli, E., Arzy, S., and Frassinetti, F. (2016b). Age-Related Effects on Future Mental Time Travel, Neural Plasticity, 1(8), 125.Google Scholar
Anelli, F., Peters-Founshtein, G., Shreibman, Y., et al. (2018b). Nature and Nurture Effects on the Spatiality of the Mental Time Line. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 11710.Google Scholar
Arzy, S., Adi-Japha, E., and Blanke, O. (2009a). The Mental Time Line: An Analogue of the Mental Number Line in the Mapping of Life Events. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(3), 781785.Google Scholar
Arzy, S., Bick, A., and Blanke, O. (2009b). Mental Time in Amnesia: Evidence from Bilateral Medial Temporal Damage Before and After Recovery. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26(6), 503510.Google Scholar
Arzy, S., Collette, S., Ionta, S., Fornari, E., and Blanke, O. (2009c). Subjective Mental Time: The Functional Architecture of Projecting the Self to Past and Future. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 30(10), 20092017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arzy, S., Molnar-Szakacs, I., and Blanke, O. (2008). Self in Time: Imagined Self-Location Influences Neural Activity Related to Mental Time Travel. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(25), 65026507.Google Scholar
Arzy, S., and Schacter, D. L. (2019). Self-Agency and Self-Ownership in Cognitive Mapping. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(6), 476487.Google Scholar
Atkinson, R. C., and Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human Memory: A Proposed System and its Control Processes. In Spence, K. W. and Spence, J. T. (eds.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory. Volume 2. New York, NY: Academic Press, 89195.Google Scholar
Banino, A., Barry, C., Uria, B., et al. (2018). Vector-Based Navigation Using Grid-Like Representations in Artificial Agents. Nature, 557(7705), 429433.Google Scholar
Banks, W. P., and Hill, D. K. (1974). The Apparent Magnitude of Number Scaled by Random Production. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102(2), 353376.Google Scholar
Bartsch, T., Dohring, J., Rohr, A., Jansen, O., and Deuschl, G. (2011). CA1 Neurons in the Human Hippocampus Are Critical for Autobiographical Memory, Mental Time Travel, and Autonoetic Consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(42), 1756217567.Google Scholar
Behrens, T. E. J., Muller, T. H., Whittington, J. C. R., et al. (2018). What is a Cognitive Map? Organizing Knowledge for Flexible Behavior. Neuron, 100(2), 490509.Google Scholar
Ben-Yakov, A., Honey, C. J., Lerner, Y., and Hasson, U. (2012). Loss of Reliable Temporal Structure in Event-Related Averaging of Naturalistic Stimuli. NeuroImage, 63(1), 501506.Google Scholar
Benoit, R. G., and Schacter, D. L. (2015). Specifying the Core Network Supporting Episodic Simulation and Episodic Memory by Activation Likelihood Estimation. Neuropsychologia, 75, 450457.Google Scholar
Berrios, G. E. (1982). Disorientation States and Psychiatry. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 23(5), 479491.Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The Brain’s Default Network: Anatomy, Function, and Relevance to Disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 138.Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L., and Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-Projection and the Brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 4957.Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L., and DiNicola, L. M. (2019). The Brain’s Default Network: Updated Anatomy, Physiology and Evolving Insights. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 20(10), 593608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cabeza, R., and St Jacques, P. (2007). Functional Neuroimaging of Autobiographical Memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(5), 219227.Google Scholar
Constantinescu, A. O., O’Reilly, J. X., and Behrens, T. E. (2016). Organizing Conceptual Knowledge in Humans with a Gridlike Code. Science, 1(6292), 19.Google Scholar
Coughlan, G., Laczó, J., Hort, J., Minihane, A. M., and Hornberger, M. (2018). Spatial Navigation Deficits – Overlooked Cognitive Marker for Preclinical Alzheimer Disease? Nature Reviews Neurology, 14(8), 111.Google Scholar
Craik, K. J. W. (1943). The nature of explanation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
Dafni-Merom, A., Peters-Founshtein, G., Kahana-Merhavi, S., & Arzy, S. (2019). A Unified Brain System of Orientation and Its Disruption in Alzheimer’s Disease. Annals of Clinical and Translational Neurology, 6(12), 24682478.Google Scholar
Danjo, T., Toyoizumi, T., and Fujisawa, S. (2018). Spatial Representations of Self and Other in the Hippocampus. Science, 218(January), 213218.Google Scholar
Davelaar, E. J., Goshen-Gottstein, Y., Ashkenazi, A., Haarmann, H. J., and Usher, M. (2005). The Demise of Short-Term Memory Revisited: Empirical and Computational Investigations of Recency Effects. Psychological Review, 112(1), 342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., and Giraux, P. (1993). The Mental Representation of Parity and Number Magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 122(3), 371396.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., and Cohen, L. (1995). Towards an Anatomical and Functional Model of Number Processing. Mathematical Cognition, 1(1), 83120.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Izard, V., and Pica, P. (2008). Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures. Science, 1217(May), 12171220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R., and Tsivkin, S. (1999). Sources of Mathematical Thinking. Science, 284(May), 970974.Google Scholar
DeVito, L. M., and Eichenbaum, H. (2011). Memory for the Order of Events in Specific Sequences: Contributions of the Hippocampus and Medial Prefrontal Cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(9), 31693175.Google Scholar
Doeller, C. F., Barry, C., and Burgess, N. (2010). Evidence for Grid Cells in a Human Memory Network. Nature, 463(7281), 657661.Google Scholar
Eichenbaum, H. (2014). Time Cells in the Hippocampus: A New Dimension for Mapping Memories. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(11), 732744.Google Scholar
Epstein, R. A., Parker, W. E., and Feiler, A. M. (2007). Where Am I Now? Distinct Roles for Parahippocampal and Retrosplenial Cortices in Place Recognition. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(23), 61416149.Google Scholar
Epstein, R. A., Patai, E. Z., Julian, J. B., and Spiers, H. J. (2017). The Cognitive Map in Humans: Spatial Navigation and Beyond. Nature Neuroscience, 20(11), 15041513.Google Scholar
Folkerts, S., Rutishauser, U., and Howard, M. W. (2018). Human Episodic Memory Retrieval Is Accompanied by a Neural Contiguity Effect. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(17), 42004211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M., Folstein, S., and McHugh, P. (1975). “Mini-Mental State”: A Practical Method for Grading the Cognitive State of Patients for the Clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189198.Google Scholar
Frith, U., and Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and Neurophysiology of Mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 459473.Google Scholar
Fuhrman, O., and Boroditsky, L. (2010). Cross-Cultural Differences in Mental Representations of Time: Evidence From an Implicit Nonlinguistic Task. Cognitive Science, 34(8), 14301451.Google Scholar
Garvert, M. M., Dolan, R. J., and Behrens, T. E. J. (2017). A Map of Abstract Relational Knowledge in the Human Hippocampal–Entorhinal Cortex. Elife, 6, 120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gauthier, B., and van Wassenhove, V. (2016). Cognitive Mapping in Mental Time Travel and Mental Space Navigation. Cognition, 154, 5568.Google Scholar
Gauthier, J. L., and Tank, D. W. (2018). A Dedicated Population for Reward Coding in the Hippocampus. Neuron, 99(1), 179193.e7.Google Scholar
Gershman, S. J., and Daw, N. D. (2017). Reinforcement Learning and Episodic Memory in Humans and Animals: An Integrative Framework. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 101128.Google Scholar
Göbel, S. M., Shaki, S., and Fischer, M. H. (2011). The Cultural Number Line: A Review of Cultural and Linguistic Influences on the Development of Number Processing. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(4), 543565.Google Scholar
Hafting, T., Fyhn, M., Molden, S., Moser, M. B., and Moser, E. I. (2005). Microstructure of a Spatial Map in the Entorhinal Cortex. Nature, 436(7052), 801806.Google Scholar
Hassabis, D., and Maguire, E. A. (2007). Deconstructing Episodic Memory with Construction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(7), 299306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, K. J., and Lourenco, S. F. (2011). Common Spatial Organization of Number and Emotional Expression: A Mental Magnitude Line. Brain and Cognition, 77(2), 315323.Google Scholar
Howard, M. W. (2014). Mathematical Learning Theory through Time. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 59(1), 1829.Google Scholar
Høydal, Ø. A., Skytøen, E. R., Moser, M., and Moser, E. I. (2018). Object-Vector Coding in the Medial Entorhinal Cortex. Nature, 568, 400404.Google Scholar
Huijbers, W., Pennartz, C. M. A., Cabeza, R., and Daselaar, S. M. (2011). The Hippocampus Is Coupled with the Default Network during Memory Retrieval but Not during Memory Encoding. PloS ONE, 6(4).Google Scholar
Ishihara, M., Keller, P. E., Rossetti, Y., and Prinz, W. (2008). Horizontal Spatial Representations of Time: Evidence for the STEARC Effect. Cortex, 44, 454461.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1980). Mental Models in Cognitive Science. Cognitive Science, 4(1), 71115.Google Scholar
Julian, J. B., Keinath, A. T., Frazzetta, G., and Epstein, R. A. (2018). Human Entorhinal Cortex Represents Visual Space using a Boundary-Anchored Grid. Nature Neuroscience, 21(2), 191194.Google Scholar
Kahana, M. J. (1996). Associative Retrieval Processes in Free Recall. Memory & Cognition, 24(1), 103109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraus, B. J., Brandon, M. P., Robinson, R. J., et al. (2015). During Running in Place, Grid Cells Integrate Elapsed Time and Distance Run. Neuron, 88(3), 578589.Google Scholar
Krupic, J., Bauza, M., Burton, S., Barry, C., and O’Keefe, J. (2015). Grid Cell Symmetry Is Shaped by Environmental Geometry. Nature, 518(7538), 232235.Google Scholar
Krupic, J., Bauza, M., Burton, S., and O’Keefe, J. (2018). Local Transformations of the Hippocampal Cognitive Map. Science, 359(6380), 11431146.Google Scholar
Kunz, L., Schroder, T. N., Lee, H., et al. (2015). Reduced Grid-Cell-Like Representations in Adults at Genetic Risk for Alzheimer’s Disease. Science, 350(6259), 430433.Google Scholar
Lever, C., Burton, S., Jeewajee, A., O’Keefe, J., and Burgess, N. (2009). Boundary Vector Cells in the Subiculum of the Hippocampal Formation. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(31), 97719777.Google Scholar
Liberman, N., and Trope, Y. (2008). The Psychology of Transcending the Here and Now. Science, 322(5905), 12011205.Google Scholar
MacDonald, C. J., Lepage, K. Q., Eden, U. T., and Eichenbaum, H. (2011). Hippocampal “Time Cells” Bridge the Gap in Memory for Discontiguous Events. Neuron, 71(4), 737749.Google Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Burgess, N., Donnett, J. G., et al. (1998). Knowing Where and Getting There: A Human Navigation Network. Science, 280(May), 921924.Google Scholar
Mason, M., Magee, J. C., and Fiske, S. T. (2014). Neural Substrates of Social Status Inference: Roles of Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Superior Temporal Sulcus. Journal Of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(5), 11311140.Google Scholar
Murdock, B. B., Jr. (1962). The Serial Position Effect of Free Recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 482488.Google Scholar
Murdock, В. B., and Metcalfe, J. (1978). Controlled Rehearsal in Single-Trial Free Recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 309324.Google Scholar
Murdock, B. B., and Okada, R. (1970). Interresponse Times in Single-Trial Free Recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86, 263267.Google Scholar
Nau, M., Navarro Schröder, T., Bellmund, J. L. S., and Doeller, C. F. (2018). Hexadirectional Coding of Visual Space in Human Entorhinal Cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 21(2), 188190.Google Scholar
O’Keefe, J., and Nadel, L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Omer, D. B., Maimon, S. R., Las, L., and Ulanovsky, N. (2018). Social Place-Cells in the Bat Hippocampus. Science, 224(January), 218224.Google Scholar
Parkinson, C., Kleinbaum, A. M., and Wheatley, T. (2017). Spontaneous Neural Encoding of Social Network Position. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(5), 17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peer, M., Lyon, R., and Arzy, S. (2014). Orientation and Disorientation: Lessons from Patients with Epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 41, 149157.Google Scholar
Peer, M., Prüss, H., Ben-Dayan, I., et al. (2017). Functional Connectivity of Large-Scale Brain Networks in Patients with Anti-NMDA Receptor Encephalitis: An Observational Study. Lancet Psychiatry, 4(10), 768774.Google Scholar
Peer, M., Salomon, R., Goldberg, I., Blanke, O., and Arzy, S. (2015). Brain System for Mental Orientation in Space, Time, and Person. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(35), 1107211077.Google Scholar
Peters-Founshtein, G., Peer, M., Rein, Y., et al. (2018). Mental-Orientation: A New Approach to Assessing Patients Across the Alzheimer’s Disease Spectrum. Neuropsychology, 32(6), 690699.Google Scholar
Raaijmakers, J. G., and Shiffrin, R. M. (1980). SAM: A Theory of Probabilistic Search of Associative Memory. In Bower, G (ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Volume 14. New York, NY: Academic Press, 207262.Google Scholar
Raichle, M. E., Macleod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., et al. (2001). A Default Mode of Brain Function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(2), 676682.Google Scholar
Rapoport, B. I., and Rapoport, S. (2015). Orientation to Person, Orientation to Self. Neurology, 85(23), 20722074.Google Scholar
Rappaport, H., Enrich, K., and Wilson, A. (1985). Relation between Ego Identity and Temporal Perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(6), 16091620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rugani, R., Vallortigara, G., Priftis, K., and Regolin, L. (2015). Number-Space Mapping in the Newborn Chick Resembles Humans’ Mental Number Line. Science, 347(6221), 534537.Google Scholar
Santiago, J., Lupáñez, J., Pérez, E., and Funes, M. J. (2007). Time (Also) Flies from Left to Right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 512516.Google Scholar
Sarel, A., Finkelstein, A., Las, L., and Ulanovsky, N. (2017). Vectorial Representation of Spatial Goals in the Hippocampus of Bats. Science, 355(6321), 176180.Google Scholar
Saxe, R., and Kanwisher, N. G. (2003). People Thinking about Thinking People: The Role of the Temporo-Parietal Junction in “Theory of Mind”. NeuroImage, 19, 18351842.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., and Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: The Prospective Brain. Nature Neuroscience, 8(9), 657661.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., et al. (2012). The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain. Neuron, 76(4), 677694.Google Scholar
Schmajuk, N. A., and Buhusi, C. V. (1997). Spatial and Temporal Cognitive Mapping: A Neural Network Approach. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(3), 109114.Google Scholar
Schurr, R., Nitzan, M., Eliahou, R., et al. (2018). Temporal Dissociation of Neocortical and Hippocampal Contributions to Mental Time Travel Using Intracranial Recordings in Humans. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 12(February), 112.Google Scholar
Shaki, S., and Fischer, M. H. (2014). Random Walks on the Mental Number Line. Experimental Brain Research, 232(1), 4349.Google Scholar
Siegler, R. S., and Booth, J. L. (2004). Development of Numerical Estimation in Young Children. Child Development, 75(2), 428444.Google Scholar
Simons, J. S., Garrison, J. R., and Johnson, M. K. (2017). Brain Mechanisms of Reality Monitoring. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(6), 462473.Google Scholar
Simony, E., Honey, C. J., Chen, J., et al. (2016). Dynamical Reconfiguration of the Default Mode Network during Narrative Comprehension. Nature Communications, 7, 12141. doi:10.1038/ncomms12141.Google Scholar
Sirotin, Y. B., Kimball, D. R., and Kahana, M. J. (2005). Going Beyond a Single List: Modeling the Effects of Prior Experience on Episodic Free Recall. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12(5), 787805.Google Scholar
Solstad, T., Boccara, C. N., Kropff, E., Moser, M., and Moser, E. I. (2008). Representation of Geometric Borders in the Entorhinal Cortex. Science, 322(5909), 18651868.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., and Grady, C. L. (2009). Patterns of Brain Activity Supporting Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, and Theory of Mind, and Their Relationship to the Default Mode Network. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(6), 11121123.Google Scholar
St. Jacques, P. L., Carpenter, A. C., Szpunar, K. K., and Schacter, D. L. (2018). Remembering and Imagining Alternative Versions of the Personal Past. Neuropsychologia, 110, 170179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
St. Jacques, P., Rubin, D. C., LaBar, K. S., and Cabeza, R. (2008). The Short and Long of it: Neural Correlates of Temporal-Order Memory for Autobiographical Events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(7), 13271341.Google Scholar
Strange, B. A., Witter, M. P., Lein, E.S., and Moser, E. I. (2014). Functional Organization of the Hippocampal Longitudinal Axis. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(10), 655669.Google Scholar
Szpunar, K. K., Watson, J. M., and McDermott, K. B. (2007). Neural Substrates of Envisioning the Future. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(2), 642647.Google Scholar
Taube, J. S., Muller, R. U., and Ranck, J. B. (1990). Head-Direction Cells Recorded from the Postsubiculum in Freely Moving Rats. I. Description and Quantitative Analysis. Journal of Neuroscience, 10(2), 420435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tavares, R. M., Mendelsohn, A., Grossman, Y., et al. (2015). A Map for Social Navigation in the Human Brain. Neuron, 87(1), 231243.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive Maps in Rats and Men. The Psychological Review, 55(4), 189208.Google Scholar
Torralbo, A., Santiago, J., and Lupiáñez, J. (2006). Flexible Conceptual Projection of Time onto Spatial Frames of Reference. Cognitive Science, 30(4), 745757.Google Scholar
Tversky, B.(1993). Cognitive Maps, Cognitive Collages, and Spatial Mental Models. In European Conference on Spatial Information Theory (pp. 1424). Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg.Google Scholar
Uddin, L. Q., Iacoboni, M., Lange, C., and Keenan, J. P. (2007). The Self and Social Cognition: The Role of Cortical Midline Structures and Mirror Neurons. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(4), 153157.Google Scholar
Vallesi, A., Binns, M. A., and Shallice, T. (2008). An Effect of Spatial-Temporal Association of Response Codes: Understanding the Cognitive Representations of Time. Cognition, 107(2), 501527.Google Scholar
Wallace, D. G., and Whishaw, I. Q. (2003). NMDA Lesions of Ammon’s Horn and the Dentate Gyrus Disrupt the Direct and Temporally Paced Homing Displayed by Rats Exploring a Novel Environment: Evidence for a Role of the Hippocampus in Dead Reckoning. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 18(3), 513523.Google Scholar
Wilson, R. C., Takahashi, Y. K., Schoenbaum, G., and Niv, Y. (2014). Orbitofrontal Cortex as a Cognitive Map of Task Space. Neuron, 81(2), 267278.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×