Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-9pm4c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T08:54:30.625Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

32 - The Counterfactual Imagination: The Impact of Alternatives to Reality on Morality

from Part IV - Novel Combinatorial Forms of the Imagination

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2020

Anna Abraham
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Get access

Summary

People often create counterfactual alternatives to reality about how things could have been different “if only …” The creation of alternatives has widespread effects in daily mental life: It enables people to explain the past, to prepare for the future, and to experience emotions such as regret and guilt. In this chapter I consider the profound impact of the counterfactual imagination on everyday thought by focusing on its impact on moral judgments such as blame and responsibility. Insights from the study of imagination and morality shed light on the mechanisms underlying the counterfactual imagination: simulation processes that construct an alternative to reality by mutating aspects of the mental representation of reality. The relationship between the imagination and morality is two-way: On the one hand, counterfactuals affect moral judgments because people rely on them to identify causes of an outcome and to probe a person’s intentions and knowledge. On the other hand, moral thoughts affect the imagination because moral norms provide important “fault lines,” “junctures” in the mental representation of reality that affect the accessibility of imagined alternatives. The two-way relationship corroborates the idea that the imagination depends on cognitive processes shared with reasoning.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alicke, M. D., Buckingham, J., Zell, E., and Davis, T. (2008). Culpable Control and Counterfactual Reasoning in the Psychology of Blame. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 13711381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, S. R., Riggs, K. J., and Gorniak, S. L. (2009). Relating Developments in Children’s Counterfactual Thinking and Executive Functions. Thinking and Reasoning, 15, 337354.Google Scholar
Beck, S. R., Robinson, E. J., Carroll, D. J., and Apperly, I. A. (2006). Children’s Thinking about Counterfactuals and Future Hypotheticals as Possibilities. Child Development, 77, 413426.Google Scholar
Branscombe, N. R., Owen, S., Garstka, T. A., and Coleman, J. (1996). Rape and Accident Counterfactuals: Who Might Have Done Otherwise and Would It Have Changed the Outcome? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26(12), 10421067.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bucciarelli, M., Khemlani, S., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2008). The Psychology of Moral Reasoning. Judgment and Decision Making, 3(2), 121139.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. M. J. (2005). The Rational Imagination: How People Create Alternatives to Reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. M. J.(2007). Précis of The Rational Imagination: How People Create Alternatives to Reality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(5–6), 439453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. M. J.(2016). Counterfactual Thoughts. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 135157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. M. J.(2017). Counterfactual Thinking: From Logic to Morality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(4), 314322.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. M. J., and Egan, S. M. (2004). Counterfactual and Prefactual Conditionals. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(2), 113.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. M. J., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2009). “If” and the Problems of Conditional Reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(7), 282287.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. M. J., Segura, S., Culhane, R., Tasso, A., and Berrocal, P. (2000). The Temporality Effect in Counterfactual Thinking about What Might Have Been. Memory & Cognition, 28(2), 264281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. M. J., and Tasso, A. (1999). Deductive Reasoning with Factual, Possible, and Counterfactual Conditionals. Memory & Cognition, 27, 726740.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. M. J., and Timmons, S. (2018). Moral Hindsight for Good Actions and the Effects of Imagined Alternatives to Reality. Cognition, 178, 8291.Google Scholar
Chituc, V., Henne, P., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., and De Brigard, F. (2016). Blame, not Ability, Impacts Moral “Ought” Judgments for Impossible Actions: Toward an Empirical Refutation of “Ought” Implies “Can”. Cognition, 150, 2025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cushman, F. (2008). Crime and Punishment: Distinguishing the Roles of Causal and Intentional Analyses in Moral Judgment. Cognition, 108(2), 353380.Google Scholar
Cushman, F.(2013). Action, Outcome, and Value: A Dual-System Framework for Morality. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17(3), 273292.Google Scholar
Davis, C. G., Lehman, D. R.,Wortman, C. B., Silver, R. C., and Thompson, S. C. (1995). The Undoing of Traumatic Life Events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21,109124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, J., and Byrne, R. M. J. (2011). “If Only” Counterfactual Thoughts about Exceptional Actions. Memory & Cognition, 39(7), 13171331.Google Scholar
Effron, D. A., Miller, D. T., and Monin, B. (2012). Inventing Racist Roads Not Taken: The Licensing Effect of Immoral Counterfactual Behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(6), 916932.Google Scholar
Egan, S., and Byrne, R. M. J. (2012). Inferences from Counterfactual Threats and Promises. Experimental Psychology, 59(4), 227235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Espino, O., and Byrne, R. M. J. (2013). The Compatibility Heuristic in Non Categorical Hypothetical Reasoning: Inferences Between Conditionals And Disjunctions. Cognitive Psychology , 67(3), 98129.Google Scholar
Espino, O., and Byrne, R. M. J.(2018). Thinking about the Opposite of What Is Said: Counterfactual Conditionals and Symbolic or Alternate Simulations of Negation. Cognitive Science, 42(8), 24592501.Google Scholar
Ferrante, D., Girotto, V., Straga, M., and Walsh, C. (2013). Improving the Past and the Future: A Temporal Asymmetry in Hypothetical Thinking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1), 2327.Google Scholar
Fillenbaum, S. (1974). Information Amplified: Memory for Counterfactual Conditionals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102(1), 44.Google Scholar
Fleischhut, N., Meder, B., and Gigerenzer, G. (2017). Moral Hindsight. Experimental Psychology, 64, 110123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French Civil Aviation Safety Investigation Authority (2016). Final Report. www.bea.aero/uploads/tx_elydbrapports/BEA2015-0125.en-LR.pdf.Google Scholar
Frosch, C., and Byrne, R. M. J. (2012). Causal Conditionals and Counterfactuals. Acta Psychologica, 141(1), 5466.Google Scholar
Gilbert, E. A., Tenney, E. R., Holland, C. R., and Spellman, B. A. (2015). Counterfactuals, Control, and Causation Why Knowledgeable People Get Blamed More. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(5), 643658.Google Scholar
Girotto, V., Legrenzi, P., and Rizzo, A. (1991). Event Controllability in Counterfactual Thinking. Acta Psychologica, 78, 111133.Google Scholar
Gomez Beldarrain, M., Garcia-Monco, J. C., Astigarraga, E., Gonzalez, A., and Grafman, J. (2005). Only Spontaneous Counterfactual Thinking Is Impaired in Patients with Prefrontal Cortex Lesions. Cognition and Brain Research, 24(3), 723726.Google Scholar
Guajardo, N. R., Parker, J., and Turley-Ames, K. (2009). Associations among False Belief Understanding, Counterfactual Reasoning, and Executive Function. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27(3), 681702.Google Scholar
Gubbins, E., and Byrne, R. M. J. (2014). Dual Processes of Emotion and Reason in Judgments about Moral Dilemmas. Thinking and Reasoning, 20(2), 245268.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. (2000). The Work of the Imagination. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., and Byrne, R. M. J. (1995). A Model Point of View. Thinking and Reasoning, 1(4), 339350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., and Byrne, R. M. J.(2002) Conditionals: A Theory of Meaning, Pragmatics, and Inference. Psychological Review, 109, 646678.Google Scholar
Juhos, C., Quelhas, A. C., and Byrne, R. M. J. (2015). Reasoning about Intentions: Counterexamples to Reasons for Actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 41(1), 5576.Google ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D, and Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm Theory: Comparing Reality to its Alternatives. Psychological Review, 93(2), 136153.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D, and Tversky, A. (1982). The Simulation Heuristic. In Kahneman, D, Slovic, P, and Tversky, A (eds.), Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, 201208.Google Scholar
Khemlani, S., Johnson-Laird, P. N., and Byrne, R. M. J. (2018). Facts and Possibilities: A Model-Based Theory of Sentential Reasoning. Cognitive Science, 42(6), 18871924.Google Scholar
Lagnado, D. A., and Channon, S. (2008). Judgments of Cause and Blame: The Effects of Intentionality and Foreseeability. Cognition, 108, 754770.Google Scholar
Lagnado, D. A., Gerstenberg, T., and Zultan, R. I. (2013). Causal Responsibility and Counterfactuals. Cognitive Science, 37(6), 10361073.Google Scholar
Lench, H. C., Domsky, D., Smallman, R., and Darbor, K. E. (2015). Beliefs in Moral Luck: When and Why Blame Hinges on Luck. British Journal of Psychology, 106 (2), 272287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, D. (1973). Counterfactuals. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Macrae, C. N., Milne, A. B., and Griffiths, R. J. (1993). Counterfactual Thinking and the Perception of Criminal Behaviour. British Journal of Psychology, 84(2), 221226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malle, B. F., Guglielmo, S., and Monroe, A. E. (2014). A Theory of Blame. Psychological Inquiry, 25(2), 147186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandel, D. R., and Lehman, D. R. (1996). Counterfactual Thinking and Ascriptions of Cause and Preventability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(3), 450463.Google Scholar
Markman, K. D., McMullen, M. N, and Elizaga, R. A. (2008). Counterfactual Thinking, Persistence, and Performance: A Test of the Reflection and Evaluation Model. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(2), 421428.Google Scholar
Markman, K. D., Mizoguchi, N., and McMullen, M. N. (2008). “It Would Have Been Worse under Saddam”: Implications of Counterfactual Thinking for Beliefs Regarding the Ethical Treatment of Prisoners of War. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(3), 650654.Google Scholar
Martin, J. W., and Cushman, F. A. (2016a). The Adaptive Logic of Moral Luck. In Sytsma, J and Buckwalter, W (eds.), The Blackwell Companion to Experimental Philosophy. Oxford, UK: Wiley Blackwell, 190202.Google Scholar
Martin, J. W., and Cushman, F. A.(2016b). Why We Forgive What Can’t Be Controlled. Cognition, 147, 133143.Google Scholar
Martinez, J. (2002) Flight 587’s Last Moments. New York Daily News. October 30.Google Scholar
McCloy, R., and Byrne, R. M. J. (2002). Semifactual “Even If” Thinking. Thinking and Reasoning, 8(1), 4167.Google Scholar
McCloy, R., and Byrne, R. M. J.(2000). Counterfactual Thinking about Controllable Events. Memory & Cognition, 28(6), 10711078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCormack, T., O’Connor, E., Beck, S., and Feeney, A. (2016). The Development of Regret and Relief about the Outcomes of Risky Decisions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 148, 119.Google Scholar
McCrea, S. M. (2008). Self-Handicapping, Excuse Making, and Counterfactual Thinking: Consequences for Self-Esteem and Future Motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 274292.Google Scholar
McEleney, A., and Byrne, R. M. J. (2006). Spontaneous Causal and Counterfactual Thoughts. Thinking and Reasoning, 12, 235255.Google Scholar
Migliore, S., Curcio, G., Mancini, F., and Cappa, S. F. (2014). Counterfactual Thinking in Moral Judgment: An Experimental Study. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 451.Google Scholar
Murray, D., and Lombrozo, T. (2016). Effects of Manipulation on Attributions of Causation, Free Will, and Moral Responsibility. Cognitive Science, 41(2), 447481.Google Scholar
National Transportation Safety Board (2004). NTSB Says Pilots Excessive Rudder Pedal Inputs Led to Crash of American Flight 587. Press Release. www.ntsb.gov/news/press-releases/Pages/NTSB_Says_Pilots_Excessive_Rudder_Pedal_Inputs_Led_to_Crash_of_American_Flight_587;_Airbus_Rudder_System_Design_Amp;_Eleme.aspx.Google Scholar
N’gbala, A., and Branscombe, N. R. (1995). Mental Simulation and Causal Attribution: When Simulating an Event Does Not Affect Fault Assignment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 31, 139162.Google Scholar
Parkinson, M., and Byrne, R. M. J. (2017). Counterfactual and Semifactual Thoughts in Moral Judgments about Failed Attempts to Harm. Thinking and Reasoning, 23(4), 409448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parkinson, M., and Byrne, R. M. J.(2018). Judgments of Moral Responsibility and Wrongness for Intentional and Accidental Harm and Purity Violations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(3), 779789.Google ScholarPubMed
Phillips, J., and Cushman, F. (2017). Morality constrains the default representation of what is possible. PNAS, 114(18), 46494654.Google Scholar
Phillips, J., Luguri, J. B., and Knobe, J. (2015). Unifying Morality’s Influence on Non-Moral Judgments: The Relevance of Alternative Possibilities. Cognition, 145, 3042.Google Scholar
Pighin, S., Byrne, R. M. J., Ferrante, D., Gonzalez, M., and Girotto, V. (2011). Counterfactual Thoughts about Experienced, Observed, and Narrated Events. Thinking and Reasoning, 17(2), 197211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rasga, C., Quelhas, A. C., and Byrne, R. M. J. (2016). Children’s Reasoning about Other’s Intentions: False-Belief and Counterfactual Conditional Inferences. Cognitive Development, 40, 4659.Google Scholar
Rasga, C., Quelhas, A. C., and Byrne, R. M. J.(2017). How Children with Autism Reason about Other’s Intentions: False-Belief and Counterfactual Inferences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(6), 18061817.Google Scholar
Rim, S., and Summerville, A. (2014). How Far to the Road Not Taken? The Effect of Psychological Distance on Counterfactual Direction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(3), 391401.Google Scholar
Roese, N. J., and Epstude, K. (2017). The Functional Theory of Counterfactual Thinking: New Evidence, New Challenges, New Insights. In Olson, J. M. (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology: Vol. 56. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 179.Google Scholar
Santamaría, C., Espino, O., and Byrne, R.M.J. (2005). Counterfactual and Semifactual Conditionals Prime Alternative Possibilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 31, 11491154.Google Scholar
Segura, S., Fernandez-Berrocal, P., and Byrne, R. M. J. (2002). Temporal and Causal Order Effects in Thinking about What Might Have Been. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 55(4), 12951305.Google Scholar
Smallman, R., and McCulloch, K. C. (2012). Learning from Yesterday’s Mistakes to Fix Tomorrow’s Problems: When Functional Counterfactual Thinking and Psychological Distance Collide. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42(3), 383390.Google Scholar
Stalnaker, R. C. (1968). A Theory of Conditionals. In Rescher, N (ed.), Studies in Logical Theory. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Tasso, A., Sarlo, M., and Lotto, L. (2017). Emotions Associated with Counterfactual Comparisons Drive Decision-Making in Footbridge-Type Moral Dilemmas. Motivation and Emotion, 41(3), 410418.Google Scholar
Thompson, V., and Byrne, R. M. J. (2002). Reasoning Counterfactually: Making Inferences about Things That Didn’t Happen. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 28, 11541170.Google Scholar
Timmons, S., and Byrne, R. M. J. (2018). Moral Fatigue: The Effects of Cognitive Fatigue on Moral Reasoning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(4), 943954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timmons, S., Gubbins, E., Almeida, T. & Byrne, R.M.J. (2019). Imagined Alternatives to Episodic Memories of Morally Good Acts. Journal of Positive Psychology. DOI: 10.1080/17439760.2019.1689410CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wald, M. L (2004). Investigators Fault Co-Pilot’s Actions in Crash That Killed 265. New York Times. October 26. www.nytimes.com/2004/10/26/nyregion/investigators-fault-copilots-actions-in-crash-that-killed-265.html?_r=0.Google Scholar
van Hoeck, N., Begtas, E., Steen, J., et al. (2014). False Belief and Counterfactual Reasoning in a Social Environment. NeuroImage, 90, 315325.Google Scholar
Walsh, C. R., and Byrne, R. M. J. (2007). How People Think “If Only …” About Reasons for Actions. Thinking and Reasoning, 13(4), 461483.Google Scholar
Walsh, C. R., and Byrne, R. M. J.(2004). Counterfactual Thinking: The Temporal Order Effect. Memory & Cognition, 32, 369378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, L., and Phillips, J. (2011). The Paradox of Moral Focus. Cognition, 119(2), 166178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, L., and Saxe, R. (2011). When Ignorance Is No Excuse: Different Roles for Intent across Moral Domains. Cognition, 120(2), 202214.Google Scholar
Young, L., and Tsoi, L. (2013). When Mental States Matter, When They Don’t, and What That Means for Morality. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7(8), 585604.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×