Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-gq7q9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-18T02:55:41.208Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

19 - Boredom

from Part IV - Curiosity and Boredom

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 February 2019

K. Ann Renninger
Affiliation:
Swarthmore College, Pennsylvania
Suzanne E. Hidi
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Get access

Summary

This chapter examines boredom – an emotion often described as one of the plagues of modern societies. In educational settings, boredom is also often experienced. The chapter first outlines how boredom is defined and operationalized including current approaches to differentiating specific types of boredom. We further review the extent to which boredom has been investigated in the research literature and how it has been assessed. Empirical evidence on the prevalence of boredom in students is outlined, and preliminary findings on the frequency of boredom experiences in teachers is highlighted. Theoretical considerations and empirical findings are subsequently addressed concerning the effects and causes of academic boredom, as are relevant conceptual frameworks and findings on how to most effectively cope with boredom in educational settings. Implications for the prevention and reduction of boredom in the classroom following from empirical literature are then discussed. Finally, we outline potential next steps in research on academic boredom.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Acee, T. W., Kim, H., Kim, H. J., Kim, J.-I., Chu, H. R., Kim, M., ... Wicker, F. W. (2010). Academic boredom in under- and over-challenging situations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35(1), 1727.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ahmed, W., van der Werf, G., Kuyper, H., & Minnaert, A. (2013). Emotions, self-regulated learning, achievement in mathematics: A growth curve analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 150–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, E. S., Keller, M. M., Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., & Taxer, J. L. (2015). Antecedents of teachers' emotions in the classroom: An intraindividual approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Book Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, R. F., Kale, A. R., & Cornell, K. R. (1990). Boredom as a limiting condition on the mere exposure effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 791800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chin, A., Markey, A., Bhargava, S., Kassam, K. S., & Loewenstein, G. (2017). Bored in the USA: Experience sampling and boredom in everyday life. Emotion, 17(2), 359–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. & Larson, R. (1984). Being adolescent. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Daniels, L. M., Stupnisky, R. H., Pekrun, R., Haynes, T. L., Perry, R. P., & Newall, N. E. (2009). A longitudinal analysis of achievement goals: From affective antecedents to emotional effects and achievement outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 948–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniels, L. M., Tze, V. M. C., & Goetz, T. (2015). Examining boredom: Different causes for different coping profiles. Learning and Individual Differences, 37, 255–61.Google Scholar
Daschmann, E. C., Goetz, T., & Stupnisky, R. H. (2011). Testing the predictors of boredom at school: Development and validation of the Precursors to Boredom Scales. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 421–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dicintio, M. J. & Gee, S. (1999). Control is the key: Unlocking the motivation of at-risks students. Psychology in the Schools, 36, 231–7.3.0.CO;2-#>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duda, J. L., Fox, K. R., Biddle, S. J., & Armstrong, N. (1992). Children's achievement goals and beliefs about success in sport. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 62, 313–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durik, A. M. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2007). Different strokes for different folks: How individual interest moderates the effects of situational factors on task interest. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 597610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. (1984). Expression and the nature of emotion. In Scherer, K. S. & Ekman, P. (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 319–43). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fahlman, S. A., Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Flora, D. B., & Eastwood, J. D. (2013). Development and validation of the multidimensional state boredom scale. Assessment, 20(1), 6885.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farmer, R. & Sundberg, N. D. (1986). Boredom proneness: The development and correlates of a new scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farrell, E., Peguero, G., Lindsey, R., & White, R. (1988). Giving voice to high school students: Pressure and boredom, ya know what I'm sayin'? American Educational Research Journal, 25(4), 489502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenichel, O. (1951). On the psychology of boredom. In Rapaport, D. (Ed.), Organization and pathology of thought: Selected sources (pp. 349–61). New York, NY: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Fisher, C. D. (1993). Boredom at work: A neglected concept. Human Relations, 46(3), 395417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., Dicke, A.-L., & Goetz, T. (2012). Beyond quantitative decline: Conceptual shifts in adolescents' development of interest in mathematics. Developmental Psychology, 48(4), 1069–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., & Goetz, T. (2007). Perceived learning environment and students' emotional experiences: A multilevel analysis of mathematics classrooms. Learning and Instruction, 17, 478–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L. M., Durksen, T. L., Becker-Kurz, B., & Klassen, R. (2016). Measuring teachers' enjoyment, anger, and anxiety: The Teacher Emotions Scales (TES). Contemporary Educational Psychology, 46, 148–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geiwitz, P. J. (1966). Structure of boredom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 592600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gjesme, T. (1977). General satisfaction and boredom at school as a function of the pupils' personality characteristics. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 21(3), 113–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goetz, T., Becker, E. S., Bieg, M., Keller, M. M., Frenzel, A. C., & Hall, N. C. (2015). The glass half empty: How emotional exhaustion affects the state-trait discrepancy in self-reports of teaching emotions. PLoS ONE, 10(9): e0137441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goetz, T. & Bieg, M. (2016). Academic emotions and their regulation via emotional intelligence. In Lipnevich, A. A., Preckel, F., & Roberts, R. D. (Eds.), Psychosocial skills and school systems in the 21st century: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 279–98). New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Goetz, T., Cronjaeger, H., Frenzel, A. C., Lüdtke, O., & Hall, N. C. (2010). Academic self-concept and emotion relations: Domain specificity and age effects. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35, 4458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goetz, T. & Frenzel, A. C. (2006). Phänomenologie schulischer Langeweile [Phenomenology of boredom at school]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 38(4), 149–53.Google Scholar
Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Hall, N. C., Nett, U., Pekrun, R., & Lipnevich, A. (2014a). Types of boredom: An experience sampling approach. Motivation and Emotion, 38, 401–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., & Pekrun, R. (2007). Regulation von Langeweile im Unterricht. Was Schuelerinnen und Schueler bei der “Windstille der Seele” (nicht) tun [Regulation of boredom in class. What students (do not) do when experiencing the “Windless Calm of the Soul”]. Unterrichtswissenschaft, 35(4), 312–33.Google Scholar
Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., Hall, N. C., & Lüdtke, O. (2007). Between- and within-domain relations of students’ academic emotions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(4), 715–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goetz, T., Haag, L., Lipnevich, A. A., Keller, M. M., Frenzel, A. C., & Collier, A. P. M. (2014b). Between-domain relations of students' academic emotions and their judgments of school domain similarity. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goetz, T. & Hall, N. C. (2013). Emotion and achievement in the classroom. In Hattie, J. and Anderman, E. M. (Eds.), International guide to student achievement (pp. 192–5). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Goetz, T., Luedtke, O., Nett, U. E., Keller, M., & Lipnevich, A. A. (2013). Characteristics of teaching and students’ emotions in the classroom: Investigating differences across domains. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38, 383–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goetz, T. & Nett, U. E. (2012). Boredom in university students. Codebook, University of Konstanz, Konstanz.Google Scholar
Goetz, T., Nett, U. E., Martiny, S. E., Hall, N. C., Pekrun, R., Dettmers, S., & Trautwein, U. (2012). Students’ emotions during homework: Structures, self-concept antecedents, and achievement outcomes. Learning and Individual Differences, 22(2), 225–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goetz, T., Pekrun, R., Hall, N. C., & Haag, L. (2006). Academic emotions from a social-cognitive perspective: Antecedents and domain specificity of students’ affect in the context of Latin instruction. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(2), 289308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goetz, T., Sticca, F., Pekrun, R., Murayama, K., & Elliot, A. J. (2016). Intraindividual relations between achievement goals and discrete achievement emotions: An experience sampling approach. Learning and Instruction, 41, 115–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grubb, E. A. (1975). Assembly line boredom and individual differences in recreation participation. Journal of Leisure Research, 7, 256–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haag, L. & Goetz, T. (2012). Mathe ist schwierig und Deutsch aktuell. Vergleichende Studie zur Charakterisierung von Schulfächern aus Schülersicht [Math is difficult and German up to date: A study on the characterization of subject domains from students' perspective]. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 59, 3246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, N. C., Perry, R. P., Goetz, T., Ruthig, J. C., Stupnisky, R. H., & Newall, N. E. (2007). Attributional retraining and elaborative learning: Improving academic development through writing-based interventions. Learning and Individual Differences, 17, 280–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, J. A., Haier, R. J., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (1984). Intrinsic enjoyment and boredom coping scales: Validation with personality, evoked potential, and attention measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 5, 183–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harasymchuk, C. & Fehr, B. (2012). Development of a prototype-based measure of relational boredom. Personal Relationships, 19, 162–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harley, J. M., Bouchet, F., & Azevedo, R. (2012). Measuring learners' unfolding, discrete emotional responses to different pedagogical agent scaffolding strategies. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Vancouver.Google Scholar
Harris, M. B. (2000). Correlates and characteristics of boredom proneness and boredom. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(3), 576–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hidi, S. & Renninger, A. (2006). The four-phase model of interest development. Educational Psychologist, 41, 111–27.Google Scholar
Hill, A. B. & Perkins, R. E. (1985). Towards a model of boredom. British Journal of Psychology, 76, 235–40.Google Scholar
Holahan, C. J., Moos, R. H., & Schaefer, J. A. (1996). Coping, stress resistance, and growth: Conceptualizing adaptive functioning. In Zeidner, M. & Endler, N. S. (Eds.), Handbook of coping: Theory, research, applications (pp. 2443). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Hulleman, C. S. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2009). Promoting interest and performance in high school science classes. Science, 326(5958), 1410–12.Google Scholar
Iso-Ahola, S. E. & Weissinger, E. (1990). Perceptions of boredom in leisure: Conceptualization reliability, and validity of the Leisure Boredom Scale. Journal of Leisure Research, 22, 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jagacinski, C. M. & Duda, J. L. (2001). A comparative analysis of contemporary achievement goal orientation measures. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 61, 1013–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarvis, S. & Seifert, T. (2002). Work avoidance as a manifestation of hostility, helplessness, and boredom. Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 48, 174–87.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Kanevsky, L. & Keighley, T. (2003). On gifted students in school: To produce or not to produce? Understanding boredom and the honor in underachievement. Roeper Review, 26, 20–8.Google Scholar
Karabenick, S. A., Woolley, M. E., Friedel, J. M., Ammon, B. V., Blazevski, J., Bonney, C. R., ... Kelly, K. L. (2007). Cognitive processing of self-report items in educational research: Do they think what we mean? Educational Psychologist, 42(3), 139–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kass, S. J., Vodanovich, S. J., Stanny, C. J., & Taylor, T. M. (2001). Watching the clock: Boredom and vigilance performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 92, 969–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, M. M., Woolfolk Hoy, A. E., Goetz, T., & Frenzel, A. C. (2016). Teacher enthusiasm: Reviewing and redefining a complex construct. Educational Psychology Review, 28, 743–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleinginna, P. R. & Kleinginna, A. M. (1981). A categorized list of emotion definitions, with suggestions for a consensual definition. Motivation and Emotion, 5, 345–79.Google Scholar
Krannich, M., Goetz, T., & Lipnevich, A.A. (2016). The effects of boredom due to being over- or underchallenged on students' occupational choice intentions. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Larson, R.W. & Richards, M. H. (1991). Boredom in the middle school years: Blaming schools versus blaming students. American Journal of Education, 99(4), 418–43.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. & Folkman, S. (1987). Transactional theory and research on emotions and coping. European Journal of Personality, 1(3), 141–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, T. W. (1986). Toward the development and validation of a measure of job boredom. Manhattan College Journal of Business, 15, 22–8.Google Scholar
London, H., Schubert, D. S., & Washburn, D. (1972). Increase of autonomic arousal by boredom. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 80(1), 2936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikulas, W. L. & Vodanovich, S. J. (1993). The essence of boredom. The Psychological Record, 43(1), 312.Google Scholar
Miller, G. E. & Wrosch, C. (2007). You've gotta know when to fold ‘em: Goal disengagement and systemic inflammation in adolescence. Psychological Science, 18(9), 773–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nett, U. E., Daschmann, E. C., Goetz, T., & Stupnisky, R. (2016). How accurately can parents judge their children's boredom in school? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nett, U. E., Goetz, T., & Daniels, L. (2010). What to do when feeling bored? Students’ strategies for coping with boredom. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 626–38.Google Scholar
Nett, U. E., Goetz, T., & Hall, N. C. (2011). Coping with boredom in school: An experience sampling perspective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36(1), 4959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Hanlon, J. F. (1981). Boredom: Practical consequences and a theory. Acta Psychologica, 49, 5382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pekrun, R. (2006). The control-value theory of achievement emotions: Assumptions, corollaries, and implications for educational research and practice. Educational Psychology Review, 18, 315–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2006). Achievement goals and discrete achievement emotions: A theoretical model and prospective test. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 583–97.Google Scholar
Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2009). Achievement goals and achievement emotions: Testing a model of their joint relations with academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 115–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L. M., Stupnisky, R. H., & Perry, R. P. (2010). Boredom in achievement settings: Exploring control-value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(3), 531–49.Google Scholar
Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., & Frenzel, A. C. (2005). Achievement Emotions Questionnaire – Mathematics (AEQ-M) – User's Manual. University of Munich: Department of Psychology.Google Scholar
Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Barchfeld, P., & Perry, R. P. (2011). Measuring emotions in students’ learning and performance: The achievement emotions questionnaire (AEQ). Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36(1), 3648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W., & Perry, R. P. (2002). Academic emotions in students’ self-regulated learning and achievement: A program of qualitative and quantitative research. Educational Psychologist, 37(2), 91105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pekrun, R., Hall, N. C., Goetz, T., & Perry, R. P. (2014). Boredom and academic achievement: Testing a model of reciprocal causation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(3), 696710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perkins, R. E. & Hill, A. B. (1985). Cognitive and affective aspects of boredom. British Journal of Psychology, 76(2), 221–34.Google Scholar
Phillips, A. (1993). On kissing, tickling, and being bored: Psychoanalytic essays on the unexamined life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Preckel, F., Goetz, T., & Frenzel, A. (2010). Ability grouping of gifted students: Effects on academic self-concept and boredom. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 451–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pressick-Kilborn, K. J. (2015). Canalization and connectedness in the development of science interest. In Renninger, K. A., Hidi, S., & Nieswandt, M. (Eds.), Interest in mathematics and science learning (pp. 353–67). American Educational Research Association, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Ragheb, M. G. & Merydith, S. P. (2001). Development and validation of a unidimensional scale measuring free time boredom. Leisure Studies, 20, 4159.Google Scholar
Reijseger, G., Schaufeli, W. B., Peeters, M. C. W., Taris, T. W., van Beek, I., & Ouweneel, E. (2013). Watching the paint dry at work: Psychometric examination of the Dutch Boredom Scale. Anxiety, Stress & Coping: An International Journal, 26, 508–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Renninger, K. A. & Hidi, S. (2016). The power of interest for motivation and learning. New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Robinson, M. D. & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: Evidence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychological Bulletin, 128(6), 934–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, W. P. (1975). Boredom at school. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 45, 141–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In Rosch, E. & Lloyd, B. B. (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 2748). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161–78.Google Scholar
Sansone, C. (2009). What's interest got to do with it?: Potential trade-offs in the self-regulation of motivation. In Forgas, J., Baumeister, R., and Tice, D. (Eds.), Psychology of self-regulation: Cognitive, affective, and motivational processes (pp. 3551). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Sansone, C., Thoman, D., & Fraughton, T. (2015). The relation between interest and self-regulation in mathematics and science. In Renninger, K. A., Nieswandt, M., & Hidi, S. (Eds.), Interest in mathematics and science learning (pp. 111–31). Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association.Google Scholar
Sansone, C., Weir, C., Harpster, L., & Morgan, C. (1992). Once a boring task always a boring task? Interest as a self-regulatory mechanism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 379–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scerbo, M. W. (1998). What's so boring about vigilance? In Hoffman, R. R., Sherrick, M. F., & Warm, J. S. (Eds.), Viewing psychology as a whole: The integrative science of William N. Dember (pp. 145–66). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (2000). Emotions as episodes of subsystems synchronization driven by nonlinear appraisal processes. In Lewis, M. D. & Granic, I. (Eds.), Emotion, development, and self-organization (pp. 7099). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schimmack, U. & Reisenzein, R. (2002). Experiencing activation: Energetic arousal and tense arousal are not mixtures of valence and activation. Emotion, 2, 412–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O'Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1061–86.Google Scholar
Shaw, S. M., Caldwell, L. L., & Kleiber, D. A. (1996). Boredom, stress and social control in the daily activities of adolescents. Journal of Leisure Research, 28(4), 274–92.Google Scholar
Skinner, E. A. (1996). A guide to constructs of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 549–70.Google Scholar
Skinner, E. A., Edge, K., Altman, J., & Sherwood, H. (2003). Searching for the structure of coping: A review and critique of category systems for classifying ways of coping. Psychological Bulletin, 129(2), 216–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, R. P. (1981). Boredom: A review. Human Factors, 23, 329–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strain, A. C. & Graesser, A. C. (2012). Cognitive reappraisal to alleviate boredom during learning. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Vancouver.Google Scholar
Todman, M. (2013). The dimensions of state boredom: Frequency, duration, unpleasantness, consequences and causal attributions. Educational Research International, 1(1), 3240.Google Scholar
Tze, V. M. C., Daniels, L. M., & Klassen, R. M. (2016). Evaluating the relationship between boredom and academic outcomes: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychological Review, 28, 119–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tze, V. M. C., Daniels, L. M., Klassen, R. M., & Li, J. C. (2013). Canadian and Chinese university students’ approaches to coping with academic boredom. Learning and Individual Differences, 23, 3243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Tilburg, W. A. P. & Igou, E. R. (2012). On boredom: Lack of challenge and meaning as distinct boredom experiences. Motivation and Emotion, 36, 181–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vandewiele, M. (1980). On boredom of secondary school students in Senegal. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 137, 267–74.Google Scholar
Vodanovich, S. J. (2003a). On the possible benefits of boredom: A neglected area in personality research. Psychology and Education – An Interdisciplinary Journal, 40, 2833.Google Scholar
Vodanovich, S. J. (2003b). Psychometric measures of boredom: A review of the literature. Journal of Psychology, 137(6), 569–95.Google Scholar
Vodanovich, S. J. & Watt, J. D. (2016). Self-report measures of boredom: An updated review of the literature. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 150(2), 196228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallace, J. C., Vodanovich, S. J., & Restino, B. M. (2003). Predicting cognitive failures from boredom proneness and daytime sleepiness scores: An investigation within military and undergraduate samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 635–44.Google Scholar
Watson, D. & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98(2), 219–35.Google Scholar
Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The two general activation systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 820–38.Google Scholar
Watt, J. D. & Ewing, J. E. (2010). Toward the development and validation of a measure of sexual boredom. Journal of Sex Research, 33, 5766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Won, H. J. (1989). The daily leisure of Korean school adolescents and its relationship to subjective well-being and leisure functioning. Doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon, Department of Leisure Studies and Services.Google Scholar
Xu, J., Du, J., & Fan, X. (2014). Emotion management in online group work reported by Chinese students. Educational Technology Research and Development 62(6), 795819.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level of arousal. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×