Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Print publication year: 2009
  • Online publication date: June 2012

CHAPTER 9 - Language and Literacy from a Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective1

from Part II - LITERACY AND LANGUAGE

References

Abadzi, H. (2003). Improving adult literacy outcomes: Lessons from cognitive research for developing countries. Washington, DC: The World Bank, Operation Evaluation Department.
Arbib, M. A. (Ed.) (2003). The handbook of brain theory and neural networks (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., & Rosas, P. (1989). Neuropsychological assessment in illiterates: Visuospatial and memory abilities. Brain and Cognition, 11, 147–166.
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Baddeley, A. (1996). Working memory: The interface between memory and cognition. In S. E. Gathercole (Ed.), Models of short-term memory. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4 (11), 417–423.
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 4, 829–839.
Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–173.
Baggio, G., Van Lambalgen, M., & Hagoort, P. (2007, in press). Language, linguistics, and cognition. In M. Stokhof & J. Groenendijk (Eds.), Handbook of philosophy of linguistics. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Barton, D. (1985). Awareness of language units in adults and children. In A. W. Ellis (Ed.), Progress in the psychology of language I. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bastiaansen, M., & Hagoort, P. (2006). Oscillatory neuronal dynamics during language comprehension. In C. Neuper & W. Klimesch (Eds.), Event-related dynamics of brain oscillations (pp. 179–196). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Becker, J. T., MacAndrew, D. K., & Fiez, J. A. (1999). A comment on the functional localization of the phonological storage subsystem of working memory. Brain and Cognition, 41, 27–38.
Bookheimer, S. (2002). Functional MRI of language: New approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 25, 151–188.
Brady, S. A. (1991). The role of working memory in reading disability. In S. A. Brady & D. P. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy (pp. 129–151). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bramão, I., Mendonça, A., Faísca, L., Ingvar, M., Petersson, K. M., & Reis, A. (2007). The impact of reading and writing skills on a visuomotor integration task: A comparison between illiterate and literate subjects. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 359–364.
Broca, P. (1861). Remarques sur le siege de la faculte de langage articulation suivies d’une observation d’aphemie (perte de la parole). Bulletine Societe d’Anatomie, 36, 330–357.
Brown, C. M., & Hagoort, P. (Eds.). (1999). The neurocognition of language. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Byrd, D. A., Jacobs, D. M., Hilton, H. J., Stern, Y., & Manly, J. J. (2005). Source of errors on visuoperceptual tasks: Role of education, literacy, and search strategy. Brain and Cognition, 58, 251–257.
Cary, L., & Verhaeghe, A. (1991). Efeito da prática da linguagem ou da alfabetização no conhecimento das fronteiras formais das unidades lexicais: Comparação de dois tipos de tarefas. Actas das Jornadas de Estudos dos Processos Cognitivos (pp. 33–49): Sociedade Portuguesa de Psicologia: Secção de Psicologia Cognitiva.
Castro-Caldas, A., Miranda Cavaleiro, P., Carmo, I., Reis, A., Leote, F., Ribeiro, C., et al. (1999). Influence of learning to read and write on the morphology of the corpus callosum. European Journal of Neurology, 6, 23–28.
Castro-Caldas, A., Reis, A., & Guerreiro, M. (1997). Neuropsychological aspects of illiteracy. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 7, 327–338.
Chokron, S., & Imbert, M. (1993). Influence of reading habits on line bisection. Cognitive Brain Research, 1, 219–222.
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chomsky, N. (1980). On the biological basis of language capacities. In N. Chomsky, Rules and representations (pp. 185–216). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of language. New York: Praeger.
Chomsky, N. (2000). New horizons in the study of language and mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Christiansen, M. H., & Kirby, S. (2003). Language evolution: Consensus and controversies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 300–307.
Coppens, P., Parente, M. A. M. P., & Lecours, A. R. (1998). Aphasia in illiterate individuals. In P. Coppens, Y. Lebrun, & A. Basso (Eds.), Aphasia in atypical populations (pp. 175–202). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cutland, N. J. (1980). Computability: An introduction to recursive function theory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Davis, M. D., Sigal, R., & Weyuker, E. J. (1994). Computability, complexity, and languages: Fundamentals of theoretical computer science (2nd ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Dehaene, S. (2005). Evolution of human cortical circuits for reading and arithmetic: The neuronal recycling hypothesis. In S. Dehaene, J.-R. Duhamel, M. D. Hauser, & G. Rizzolatti (eds.), From monkey brain to human brain. A Fyssen Foundation Symposium (pp. 133–157). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dehaene, S., & Hertz-Pannier, L. (2002) Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in infants. Science, 298, 2013–2015.
Eck, D. J. (2000). The most complex machine: A survey of computers and computing. Natick, MA: A K Peters.
Felleman, D. J., & Van Essen, D. C. (1991). Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 1, 1–47.
Forkstam, C., Hagoort, P., Fernandez, G., Ingvar, M., & Petersson, K. M. (2006). Neural correlates of artificial syntactic structure classification. NeuroImage, 32, 956–967.
Forkstam, C., & Petersson, K. M. (2005a). Syntactic classification of acquired structural regularities. Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society, 696–701.
Forkstam, C., & Petersson, K. M. (2005b). Towards an explicit account of implicit learning. Current Opinion in Neurology, 18, 435–441.
Frackowiak, R. S. J., Friston, K. J., Frith, C., Dolan, R., Price, C., Zeki, S., et al. (Eds.). (2004). Human brain function (2nd ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Fries, P. (2005). A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: Neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 474–480.
Friston, K. (1994). Functional and effective connectivity. Human Brain Mapping, 2, 56–78.
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1995). Working memory and language. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Hall, M., & Peacker, S. M. (2001). Dissociable lexical and phonological influences on serial recognition and serial recall. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A (1), 1–30.
Gernsbacher, M. A. (Ed.) (1994). Handbook of psycholinguistics. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Gerstner, W., & Kistler, W. (2002). Spiking neuron models: Single neurons, populations, plasticity. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Giedd, J. N., Rumsey, J. M., Castellanos, F. X., Rajapakse, J. C., Kaysen, D., Vaituzis, A. C., et al. (1996). A quantitative MRI study of the corpus callosum in children and adolescents. Developmental Brain Research, 91, 274–280.
Goody, J. (2000). The power of the written tradition. Washington: Smithsonian Institution Press.
Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. E. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.
Hagoort, P. (2005). On Broca, brain, and binding: A new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 416–423.
Hagoort, P., Hald, L., Baastiansen, M., & Petersson, K. M. (2004). Integration of word meaning and world knowledge in language comprehension. Science, 304, 438–441.
Hamilton, M. E., & Barton, D. (1983). Adult's definition of “word”: The effects of literacy and development. Journal of Pragmatics, 7, 581–594.
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298 (5598), 1569–1579.
Haykin, S. (1998). Neural networks: A comprehensive foundation (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Hebb, D. O. (1949). Organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.
Hofstadter, D. R. (1999). Godel, Escher, Bach: An eternal golden braid (20th anniversary edition). New York: Basic Books.
Indefrey, P., & Cutler, A. (2005). Prelexical and lexical processing in listening. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences III. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ingvar, M., & Petersson, K. M. (2000). Functional maps – cortical networks. In A. W. Toga & J. C. Mazziotta (Eds.), Brain mapping: The systems (pp. 111–140). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Izhikevich, E. M. (2007). Dynamical systems in neuroscience: The geometry of excitability and bursting. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Jackendoff, R. (2002). Foundations of language: Brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Jusczyk, P. W. (1997). The discovery of spoken language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., Sims, K., Jones, M. C., & Cuckle, P. (1996). Rethinking metalinguistic awareness: Representing and accessing knowledge about what counts as a word. Cognition, 58, 197–219.
Koch, C. (1999). Biophysics of computation: Information processing in single neurons. New York: Oxford University Press.
Koch, C., & Laurent, G. (1999). Complexity and the nervous system. Science, 284, 96–98.
Koch, C., & Segev, I. (1998). Methods in neuronal modeling: From ions to networks (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Kolinsky, R., Cary, L., & Morais, J. (1987). Awareness of words as phonological entities: The role of literacy. Applied Psycholinguistics, 8, 223–237.
Kosmidis, M. K., Tsapkini, K., & Folia, V. (2007). Lexical processing in illiteracy: Effect of literacy or education? Cortex, 42, 1021–1027.
Kosmidis, M. K., Tsapkini, K., Folia, V., Vlahou, C., & Kiosseoglou, G. (2004). Semantic and phonological processing in illiteracy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 818–827.
Lasota, A., & Mackey, M. C. (1994). Chaos, fractals, and noise: Stochastic aspects of dynamics. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Le Carret, N., Rainville, C., Lechevallier, N., Lafont, S., Letenneur, L., & Fabrigoule, C. (2003). Influence of education on the Benton Visual Retention test performance as mediated by a strategic search component. Brain and Cognition, 53, 408–411.
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Lewis, H. R., & Papadimitriou, C. H. (1981). Elements of the theory of computation. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Maass, W., Natschläger, T., & Markram, H. (2004). Computational models for generic cortical microcircuits. In J. Feng (Ed.), Computational neuroscience: A comprehensive approach (pp. 575–605). Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC.
Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.
Matute, E., Leal, F., Zarabozo, D., Robles, A., & Cedillo, C. (2000). Does literacy have an effect on stick construction tasks? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 668–672.
McCulloch, W. S., & Pitts, E. (1943). A logical calculus of the ideas immanent in nervous activity. Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics, 5, 115–133.
Mendonça, A., Mendonça, S., Reis, A., Faisca, L., Ingvar, M., & Petersson, K. M. (2003, November 10–12). Reconhecimento de unidades lexicais em contexto frásico: O efeito da literacia. Paper presented at the Congresso em Neurociências Cognitivas, Évora, Portugal.
Mendonça, S., Faisca, L., Silva, C., Ingvar, M., Reis, A., & Petersson, K. M. (2002). The role of literacy in the awareness of words as independent lexical units. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 483.
Mesulam, M. M. (1998). From sensation to cognition. Brain, 121, 1013–1052.
Miller, E. K., Li, L., & Desimone, R. (1991). A neural mechanism for working and recognition memory in inferior temporal cortex. Science, 254, 1377–1379.
Minsky, M. L. (1967). Computation: Finite and infinite machines. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Mishking, M., & Forgays, D. G. (1952). Word recognition as a function of retinal locus. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43, 43–48.
Morais, J., Cary, L., Alegria, J., & Bertelson, P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously? Cognition, 7, 323–331.
Morais, J. (1993). Phonemic awareness, language and literacy. In R. M. Joshi & C. K. Leong (Eds.), Reading disabilities: Diagnosis and component processes (pp. 175–184). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Newell, A., & Simon, H. (1976). Computer science as empirical inquiry: Symbols and search. Communications of the Association for Computing Machinery, 19, 111–126.
Nieuwenhuys, R., Voogd, J., & van Huijzen, C. (1988). The human central nervous system: A synopsis and atlas (3rd revised ed.). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Nowak, M. A., Komarova, N. L., & Niyogi, P. (2002). Computational and evolutionary aspects of language. Nature, 417, 611–617.
Øksendal, B. (2000). Stochastic differential equations: An introduction with applications (5th ed.). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Olson, D. R. (1996). Towards a psychology of literacy: On the relations between speech and writing. Cognition, 60, 83–104.
Ostrosky-Solis, A., Efron, R., & Yund, E. W. (1991). Visual detectability gradients: Effect of illiteracy. Brain and Cognition, 17, 42–51.
Ostrosky-Solís, F., Arellano García, M., & Pérez, P. (2004). Can learning to read and write change the brain organization? An electrophysiological study. International Journal of Psychology, 39, 27–35.
Ott, E. (1993). Chaos in dynamical systems. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Partee, B. H., ter Meulens, A., & Wall, R. E. (1990). Mathematical methods in linguistics. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Petersson, K. M. (2005a). Learning and memory in the human brain. Stockholm: Karolinska University Press.
Petersson, K. M. (2005b). On the relevance of the neurobiological analogue of the finite-state architecture. Neurocomputing, 65–66, 825–832.
Petersson, K. M. (2008, in press). On cognition, structured sequence processing, and adaptive dynamical systems. Proceedings of the American Institute of Physics, 1060.
Petersson, K. M., Elfgren, C., & Ingvar, M. (1997). A dynamic role of the medial temporal lobe during retrieval of declarative memory in man. NeuroImage, 6, 1–11.
Petersson, K. M., Elfgren, C., & Ingvar, M. (1999). Dynamic changes in the functional anatomy of the human brain during recall of abstract designs related to practice. Neuropsychologia, 37, 567–587.
Petersson, K. M., Forkstam, C., & Ingvar, M. (2004). Artificial syntactic violations activate Broca's region. Cognitive Science, 28, 383–407.
Petersson, K. M., Gisselgård, J., Gretzer, M., & Ingvar, M. (2006). Interaction between a verbal working memory network and the medial temporal lobe. NeuroImage, 33, 1207–1217.
Petersson, K. M., Nichols, T. E., Poline, J.-B., & Holmes, A. P. (1999a). Statistical limitations in functional neuroimaging I: Non-inferential methods and statistical models. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 354, 1239–1260.
Petersson, K. M., Nichols, T. E., Poline, J.-B., & Holmes, A. P. (1999b). Statistical limitations in functional neuroimaging II: Signal detection and statistical inference. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 354, 1261–1282.
Petersson, K. M., & Reis, A. (2006). Characteristics of illiterate and literate cognitive processing: Implications for brain-behavior co-constructivism. In P. B. Baltes, F. Rösler, & P. A. Reuter-Lorenz (Eds.), Lifespan development and the brain: The perspective of biocultural co-constructivism (pp. 279–305). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Petersson, K. M., Reis, A., Askelöf, S., Castro-Caldas, A., & Ingvar, M. (1998). Differences in inter-hemispheric interactions between literate and illiterate subjects during verbal repetition. NeuroImage, 7, S217.
Petersson, K. M., Reis, A., Askelöf, S., Castro-Caldas, A., & Ingvar, M. (2000). Language processing modulated by literacy: A network-analysis of verbal repetition in literate and illiterate subjects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 364–382.
Petersson, K. M., Reis, A., & Ingvar, M. (2001). Cognitive processing in literate and illiterate subjects: A review of some recent behavioral and functional data. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42, 251–167.
Petersson, K. M., Silva, C., Castro-Caldas, A., Ingvar, M., & Reis, A. (2007). Literacy: A cultural influence on the hemispheric balance in the inferior parietal cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 791–799.
Quartz, S. R., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1997). The neural basis of cognitive development: A constructivist manifesto. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 537–596.
Reis, A., & Castro-Caldas, A. (1997). Illiteracy: A bias for cognitive development. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 444–450.
Reis, A., Faísca, L., Ingvar, M., & Petersson, K. M. (2006). Color makes a difference: Two-dimensional object-naming skills in literate and illiterate subjects. Brain and Cognition, 60, 49–54.
Reis, A., Faísca, L., Mendonça, A., Ingvar, M., & Petersson, K. M. (2007). Semantic interference on a phonological task in illiterate subjects. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 48, 69–74.
Reis, A., Guerreiro, M., & Castro-Caldas, A. (1994). Influence of educational level of non-brain-damaged subjects on visual naming capacities. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16 (6), 939–942.
Reis, A., Guerreiro, M., Garcia, C., & Castro-Caldas, A. (1995). How does an illiterate subject process the lexical component of arithmetics? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1, 206.
Reis, A., Guerreiro, M., & Petersson, K. M. (2001). Educational level on a neuropsychological battery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7 (4), 422–423.
Reis, A., Guerreiro, M., & Petersson, K. M. (2003). A socio-demographic and neuropsychological characterization of an illiterate population. Applied Neuropsychology, 10, 191–204.
Reis, A., & Petersson, K. M. (2003). Educational level, socioeconomic status and aphasia research: A comment on Connor et al. (2001): Effect of socioeconomic status on aphasia severity and recovery. Brain and Language, 87, 1795–1810.
Reis, A., Petersson, K. M., Castro-Caldas, A., & Ingvar, M. (2001). Formal schooling influences two- but not three-dimensional naming skills. Brain and Cognition, 47, 394–411.
Reis, A., Silva, C., Faísca, L., Bramao, I., Mendonça, A., Miranda, P. C., et al. (2007). The impact of reading and writing skills on brain structure: A voxel-based morphometry study. The International Neuropsychological Society, Federation of Spanish Societies of Neuropsychology, Spanish Neuropsychological Society, Spanish Psychiatry Society Joint Mid-Year Meeting. Bilbao, Spain.
Sag, I. A., Wasow, T., & Bender, E. M. (2003). Syntactic theory: A formal introduction (2nd ed.). Stanford, CA: Center for the Study of Language and Information.
Savage, J. E. (1998). Models of computation: Exploring the power of computing. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Shepherd, G. M. (1997). The synaptic organization of the brain (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Shieber, S. M. (1986). An introduction to unification-based approaches to grammar (Vol. 4). Stanford, CA: CSLI, Stanford University.
Silva, C., Faisca, L., Mendonça, S., Ingvar, M., Petersson, K. M., & Reis, A. (2002). Awareness of words as phonological entities in an illiterate population. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 483.
Silva, C. G., Petersson, K. M., Faísca, L., Ingvar, M., & Reis, A. (2004). The effects of formal education on the quantitative and qualitative aspects of verbal semantic fluency. Journal of Clinical and Experimental. Neuropsychology, 26, 266–277.
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1998). Neuroimaging analyses of human working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 95 (20), 12061–12068.
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283 (5408), 1657–1661.
Tanenbaum, A. S. (1990). Structured computer organization (3rd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Thompson, P. M., Giedd, J. N., Woods, R. P., MacDonald, D., Evans, A. C., & Toga, A. W. (2000). Growth patterns in the developing brain detected by using continuum mechanical tensor maps. Nature, 404, 190–193.
UNESCO (2003). Literacy, a global perspective. Paris: UNESCO. (http://unesdoc.unesco.org/images/0013/001318/131817eo.pdf)
Vosse, T., & Kempen, G. (2000). Syntactic structure assembly in human parsing: A computational model based on competitive inhibition and a lexicalist grammar. Cognition, 75, 105–143.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-III) Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Wells, A. (2005). Rethinking cognitive computation: Turing and the science of the mind. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.
Wernicke, C. (1874). Der Aphasiche Symptomen Komplex. Breslau: Cohn and Weigart.
Zaidel, E., & Iacoboni, M. (Eds.) (2003). The parallel brain: The cognitive neuroscience of the corpus callosum. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.