Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-8kt4b Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-17T10:25:14.782Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

19 - Gestures in Cognition: Actions that Bridge the Mind and the World

from Part IV - Gestures in Relation to Cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 May 2024

Alan Cienki
Affiliation:
Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam
Get access

Summary

First, how does the human cognitive system give rise to gestures? A growing body of literature suggests that gestures are based in people’s perceptual and physical experience of the world. Second, do gestures influence how people take in information from the world? Research suggests that producing gestures modifies producers’ experience of the world in specific ways. Third, does externalizing information in gestures affect cognitive processing? There is evidence that expressing spatial and motoric information in gestures has consequences for thinking, including for memory and problem solving. Fourth, how do gestures influence other people’s cognitive processing? Research indicates that gestures can highlight certain forms of information for others’ thinking, thus engaging social mechanisms that influence cognitive processing. Gestures are closely tied to action, and they reveal how producers schematize information in the objects, tasks, events, and situations that they gesture about. In brief, gestures play an integral role in cognition, both for gesture producers and for gesture recipients, because they are actions of the body that bridge the mind and the world.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2024

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alibali, M. W., Church, R. B., Kita, S., & Hostetter, A. B. (2014). Embodied knowledge in the development of conservation of quantity: Evidence from gesture. In Edwards, L., Ferrara, F., & Moore-Russo, D. (Eds.), Emerging perspectives on gesture and embodiment in mathematics (pp. 2749). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Alibali, M. W., & DiRusso, A. A. (1999). The function of gesture in learning to count: More than keeping track. Cognitive Development, 14 (1), 3756. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0885-2014(99)80017-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., & Hostetter, A. B. (2010). Mimicry and simulation in gesture comprehension. (Commentary on P. Niedenthal, M. Maringer, M. Mermillod, & U. Hess, The Simulation of Smiles (SIMS) model: Embodied simulation and the meaning of facial expression). Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 433434. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x10000865CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., & Kita, S. (2010). Gesture highlights perceptually present information for speakers. Gesture, 10 (1), 328. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.10.1.02aliCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Kita, S., & Young, A. (2000). Gesture and the process of speech production: We think, therefore we gesture. Language & Cognitive Processes, 15, 593613. https://doi.org/10.1080/016909600750040571CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., McNeill, N. M., & Perrott, M. A. (1998). What makes children change their minds? Changes in encoding lead to changes in strategy selection. In Gernsbacher, M. A. & Derry, S. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 3641). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Spencer, R. C., Knox, L., & Kita, S. (2011). Spontaneous gestures influence strategy choices in problem solving. Psychological Science, 2 (9), 11381144. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611417722CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Yeo, A., Hostetter, A. B., & Kita, S. (2017). Representational gestures help speakers package information for speaking. In Church, R. B., Alibali, M. W., & Kelly, S. D. (Eds.), Why gesture? How the hands function in speaking, thinking, and communicating (pp. 1537). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L. W., Simmons, W. K., Barbey, A. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2003). Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7 (2), 8491. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(02)00029-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bavelas, J. B., Chovil, N., Lawrie, D. A., & Wade, A. (1992). Interactive gestures. Discourse Processes, 15, 469489. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539209544823CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J., & Healing, S. (2013). Reconciling the effects of mutual visibility on gesturing: A review. Gesture, 13 (1), 6392. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.13.1.03bavCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beattie, G., & Coughlan, J. (1999). An experimental investigation of the role of iconic gestures in lexical access using the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 3556. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712699161251CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beilock, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010). Gesturing changes thought by grounding it in action. Psychological Science, 21 (11), 16051610. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610385353CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bone, M. B., St-Laurent, M., Dant, C., McQuiggan, D. A., Ryan, J. D., & Buchsbaum, B. R. (2019). Eye movement reinstatement and neural reactivation during mental imagery. Cerebral Cortex, 29 (3), 10751089. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brookes, H. (2014). Gesture in the communicative ecology of a South African township. In Seyfeddinipur, M. & Gullberg, M. (Eds.), From gesture in conversation to visible action as utterance: Essays in honor of Adam Kendon (pp. 5974). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Brooks, N., Barner, D., Frank, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2017). The role of gesture in supporting mental representations: The case of mental abacus arithmetic. Cognitive Science, 42 (2), 554575. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12527CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casasanto, D. (2013). Different bodies, different minds: The body-specificity of language and thought. In Caballero, R. & Díaz Vera, J. (Eds.), Sensuous cognition – Explorations into human sentience (pp. 917). Berlin, Germany: de Gruyter Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cho, P. S., & So, W. C. (2018). A feel for numbers: The changing role of gesture in manipulating the mental representation of an abacus among children at different skill levels. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 1267. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01267CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chu, M., & Kita, S. (2008). Spontaneous gestures during mental rotation tasks: Insights into the microdevelopment of the motor strategy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137 (4), 706723. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013157CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chu, M., & Kita, S. (2011). The nature of gestures’ beneficial role in spatial problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140 (1), 102116. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021790CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chu, M., & Kita, S. (2016). Co-thought and co-speech gestures are generated by the same action generation process. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 42 (2), 257270. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000168Google ScholarPubMed
Cienki, A. (2013). Image schemas and mimetic schemas in cognitive linguistics and gesture studies. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 11(2), 417432. https://doi.org/10.1075/rcl.11.2.13cieCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, S. W., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006). The role of gesture in learning: Do children use their hands to change their minds? Journal of Cognition and Development, 7, 211232. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327647jcd0702_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, S. W., & Fenn, K. M. (2017). The function of gesture in learning and memory. In Church, R. B., Alibali, M. W., & Kelly, S. D. (Eds.), Why gesture? How the hands function in speaking, thinking and communicating (pp. 129153). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, S. W., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2009). Embodied communication: Speakers’ gestures affect listeners’ actions. Cognition, 113 (1), 98104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, S. W., Yip, T. K., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010). Gesture makes memories that last. Journal of Memory and Language, 63 (4), 465475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.07.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, S. W., Yip, T. K., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). Gestures, but not meaningless movements, lighten working memory load when explaining math. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 594610. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.567074CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N. (2010). The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19 (1), 5157. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721409359277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dargue, N., Sweller, N., & Jones, M. P. (2019). When our hands help us understand: A meta-analysis into the effects of gesture on comprehension. Psychological Bulletin, 145 (8), 765784. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000202CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Droll, J. A., & Hayhoe, M. M. (2007). Trade-offs between gaze and working memory use. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33 (6), 13521365. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.6.1352Google ScholarPubMed
Eielts, C., Pouw, W., Ouwehand, K., van Gog, T., Zwaan, R. A., & Paas, F. (2020). Co-thought gesturing supports more complex problem solving in subjects with lower visual working-memory capacity. Psychological Research, 84, 502513. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1065-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enfield, N. J. (2005). The body as a cognitive artifact in kinship representations. Hand gesture diagrams by speakers of Lao. Current Anthropology, 46 (1), 5181. https://doi.org/10.1086/425661CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engelkamp, J., & Zimmer, H. D. (1994). Motor similarity in subject-performed tasks. Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung, 57, 4753. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00452995CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eskritt, M., & Ma, S. (2014). Intentional forgetting: Note-taking as a naturalistic example. Memory & Cognition, 42, 237246. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0362-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flood, V. J. (2018). Multimodal revoicing as an interactional mechanism for connecting scientific and everyday concepts. Human Development, 61 (3), 145173. https://doi.org/10.1159/000488693CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frick-Horbury, D., & Guttentag, R. E. (1998). The effects of restricting hand gesture production on lexical retrieval and free recall. The American Journal of Psychology, 111 (1), 4362. https://doi.org/10.2307/1423536CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 155. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X97000010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glenberg, A. M., Witt, J. K., & Metcalfe, J. (2013). From the revolution to embodiment: 25 years of Cognitive Psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8 (5), 573585. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613498098CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Göksun, T., Goldin-Meadow, S., Newcombe, N., & Shipley, T. F. (2013). Individual differences in mental rotation: What does gesture tell us? Cognitive Processing, 14, 153162. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-013-0549-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Beilock, S. L. (2010). Action’s influence on thought: The case of gesture. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5 (6), 664674. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610388764CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., Nusbaum, H., Kelly, S. D., & Wagner, S. (2001). Explaining math: Gesturing lightens the load. Psychological Science, 12, 516522. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00395CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, C. (2000). Action and embodiment in human interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 32 (10), 14891522. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0378-2166(99)00096-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, M. H., & Goodwin, C. (1986). Gesture and coparticipation in the activity of searching for a word. Semiotica, 62 (1–2), 5175. https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.1986.62.1-2.51Google Scholar
Graham, T. A. (1999). The role of gesture in children’s learning to count. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74 (4), 333355. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1999.2520CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guillot, A., Collet, C., Nguyen, V. A., Malouin, F., Richards, C., & Doyon, J. (2009). Brain activity during visual versus kinesthetic imagery: An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 30 (7), 21572172. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20658CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haviland, J. B. (1993). Anchoring, iconicity and orientation in Guugu Yimithirr pointing gestures. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 3 (1), 345. https://doi.org/10.1525/jlin.1993.3.1.3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holler, J., & Wilkin, K. (2011). Co-speech gesture mimicry in the process of collaborative referring during face-to-face dialogue. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 35, 133153. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-011-0105-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hostetter, A. B. (2011). When do gestures communicate? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137 (2), 297315. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2008). Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 495514. https://doi.org/10.3758/pbr.15.3.495CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2010). Language, gesture, action! A test of the Gesture as Simulated Action framework. Journal of Memory and Language, 63 (2), 245257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.04.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2019). Gesture as simulated action? Revisiting the framework. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 721752. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1548-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hostetter, A. B., Alibali, M. W., & Kita, S. (2007). I see it in my hands’ eye: Representational gestures reflect conceptual demands. Language & Cognitive Processes, 22, 313336. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960600632812CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iani, F., Burin, D., Salatino, A., Pia, L., Ricci, R., & Bucciarelli, M. (2018). The beneficial effect of a speaker’s gestures on the listener’s memory for action phrases: The pivotal role of the listener’s premotor cortex. Brain & Language, 180–182, 813. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2018.03.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordan, N., Kaplan, D., Ramineni, C., & Locuniak, M. (2008). Development of number combination skill in the early school years: When do fingers help? Developmental Science, 11 (5), 662668. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00715.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamermans, K. L., Pouw, W., Fassi, L., Aslanidou, A., Paas, F., & Hostetter, A. B. (2019). The role of gesture as simulated action in reinterpretation of mental imagery. Acta Psychologica, 197, 131142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.05.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, S. D., Byrne, K., & Holler, J. (2011). Raising the ante of communication: Evidence for enhanced gesture in high stakes situations. Information, 2 (4), 579593. https://doi.org/10.3390/info2040579CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimbara, I. (2006). On gestural mimicry. Gesture, 6 (1), 3961. http://doi.org/10.1075/gest.6.1.03kimCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimbara, I. (2008). Gesture form convergence in joint description. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 32, 123131. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-007-0044-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirk, E., & Lewis, C. (2017). Gesture facilitates children’s creative thinking. Psychological Science, 28 (2), 225232. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616679183CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kita, S. (2000). How representational gestures help speaking. In McNeill, D. (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 162185). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kita, S., Alibali, M. W., & Chu, M. (2017). How do gestures influence thinking and speaking? The gesture for conceptualization hypothesis. Psychological Review, 124 (3), 245266. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000059CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kita, S., & Özyürek, A. (2003). What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48 (1), 1632. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0749-596x(02)00505-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R. M. (1998). Why do we gesture when we speak? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7 (2), 5460. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep13175642CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R. M., Chen, Y., & Gottesman, R. (2000). Lexical gestures and lexical access: A process model. In McNeill, D. (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 261283). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladewig, S. H. (2014). Recurrent gestures. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., & Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body - language - communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (Vol. 2, pp. 15581575). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Lavergne, J., & Kimura, D. (1987). Hand movement asymmetry during speech: No effect of speaking topic. Neuropsychologia, 25 (4), 689693. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(87)90060-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levinson, S. C. (1997). Language and cognition: The cognitive consequences of spatial description in Guugu Yimithirr. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 7 (1), 98131. https://doi.org/10.1525/jlin.1997.7.1.98CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liddell, S. K., & Metzger, M. (1998). Gesture in sign language discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 30 (6), 657697. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0378-2166(98)00061-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClave, E. (1994). Gestural beats: The rhythm hypothesis. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 23, 4566. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02143175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (2005). Gesture and thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marghetis, T., & Bergen, B. K. (2014). Embodied meaning, inside and out: The coupling of gesture and mental simulation. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., & Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body - language - communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (Vol. 2, pp. 20002007). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Masson-Carro, I., Goudbeek, M., & Krahmer, E. (2017). How what we see and what we know influence iconic gesture production. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 41, 367394. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-017-0261-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsumoto, D., & Hwang, H. C. (2013). Cultural similarities and differences in emblematic gestures. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 37, 127. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-012-0143-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, D., Collett, P., Marsh, P., & O’Shaughnessy, M. (1979). Gestures: Their origin and distribution. New York, NY: Stein & Day.Google Scholar
Moulton, S. T., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2009). Imagining predictions: Mental imagery as mental emulation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364 (1521), 12731280. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0314CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müller, C. (1998). Iconicity and gesture. In Cavé, C., Guaïtella, I., & Santi, S. (Eds.), Oralité et gestualité: Interactions et comportements multimodaux dans la communication [Orality and gestuality: Multimodal interaction and behavior in communication]. Actes du colloque [Proceedings of the meeting of] ORAGE 2001 (pp. 321328). Paris, France: L’Harmattan.Google Scholar
Müller, C. (2014). Gestural modes of representation as techniques of depiction. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., & Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body - language - communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (Vol. 2, pp. 16871701). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Müller, C. (2017). How recurrent gestures mean: Conventionalized contexts-of-use and embodied motivation. Gesture, 16 (2), 276303. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.16.2.05mulCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nathan, M. J., & Walkington, C. (2017). Grounded and embodied mathematical cognition: Promoting mathematical insight and proof using action and language. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 2, 9. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-016-0040-5Google ScholarPubMed
Payrató, L., & Teßendorf, S. (2014). Pragmatic gestures. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., & Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body - language - communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (Vol. 2, pp. 15311539). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Pine, K. J., Bird, H., & Kirk, E. (2007). The effects of prohibiting gestures on children’s lexical retrieval ability. Developmental Science, 10 (6), 747754. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00610.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ping, R., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010). Gesturing saves cognitive resources when talking about non-present objects. Cognitive Science, 34 (4), 602619. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01102.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ping, R., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Beilock, S. (2014). Understanding gesture: Is the listener’s motor system involved? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 195204. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032246CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pouw, W. T. J. L., de Nooijer, J. A., van Gog, T., Zwaan, R. A., & Paas, F. (2014). Toward a more embedded/extended perspective on the cognitive function of gestures. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 359. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00359CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pouw, W. T. J. L., & Hostetter, A. (2016). Gesture as predictive action. Reti, Saperi, Linguaggi: Italian Journal of Cognitive Sciences, 3, 5780. https://doi.org/10.12832/83918Google Scholar
Pouw, W., Wassenburg, S. I., Hostetter, A. B., de Koning, B. B., & Paas, F. (2020). Does gesture strengthen sensorimotor knowledge of objects? The case of the size-weight illusion. Psychological Research, 84, 966980. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1128-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pyers, J. E., Magid, R., Gollan, T. H., & Emmorey, K. (2021). Gesture helps, only if you need it: Inhibiting gesture reduces tip‐of‐the‐tongue resolution for those with weak short-term memory. Cognitive Science, 45 (1), e12914. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12914CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radford, L., Demers, S., Guzmán, J., & Cerulli, M. (2003). Calculators, graphs, gestures, and the production of meaning. In Pateman, N., Dougherty, B., & Zilliox, J. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27the Annual Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 4, pp. 5562). Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii.Google Scholar
Ravizza, S. (2003). Movement and lexical access: Do noniconic gestures aid in retrieval? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 610615. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196522CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richarz, A. (2017). Why Japan’s rail workers can’t stop pointing at things. Retrieved from https://www.atlasobscura.com/articles/pointing-and-calling-japan-trains, October 27, 2020.Google Scholar
Richland, L. E. (2015). Linking gestures: Cross-cultural variation during instructional analogies. Cognition and Instruction, 33, 295321. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370008.2015.1091459CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rimé, B., Shiaratura, L., Hupet, M., & Ghysselinckx, A. (1984). Effects of relative immobilization on the speaker’s nonverbal behavior and on the dialogue imagery level. Motivation & Emotion, 8, 311325. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00991870CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Risko, E. F., & Gilbert, S. J. (2016). Cognitive offloading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20 (9), 676688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.07.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shattuck-Hufnagel, S., & Ren, A. (2018). The prosodic characteristics of non-referential co-speech gestures in a sample of academic-lecture-style speech. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1514. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01514CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171 (3972), 701703. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.171.3972.701CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solyu, F., Lester, F. K., & Newman, S. D. (2018). You can count on your fingers: The role of fingers in early mathematical development. Journal of Numerical Cognition, 4 (1), 107135. https://doi.org/10.5964/jnc.v4i1.85CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Streeck, J. (2009). Gesturecraft: The manu-facture of meaning. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vest, N. A., Fyfe, E. R., Nathan, M. J., & Alibali, M. W. (2020). Learning from an avatar video instructor: Gesture mimicry supports middle school students’ algebra learning. Gesture, 19 (1), 128155. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.18019.vesCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, S. M., Nusbaum, H., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2004). Probing the mental representation of gesture: Is handwaving spatial? Journal of Memory and Language, 50 (4), 395407. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.01.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walkington, C., Woods, D., Nathan, M. J., Chelule, G., & Wang, M. (2019). Does restricting hand gestures impair mathematical reasoning? Learning and Instruction, 64, Article 101225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2019.101225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 625636. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196322CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolfgram, M. (2014). Gesture and the communication of mathematical ontologies in classrooms. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 24, 216237. https://doi.org/10.1111/jola.12049CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zlatev, J. (2014). Image schemas, mimetic schemas and children’s gestures. Cognitive Semiotics, 7 (1), 329. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogsem-2014-0002CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×