Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T00:08:10.315Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - The Problem of Altruism and Future Directions

from Part IV - Group Living

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 March 2020

Lance Workman
Affiliation:
University of South Wales
Will Reader
Affiliation:
Sheffield Hallam University
Jerome H. Barkow
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University, Nova Scotia
Get access

Summary

Augustes Comte first coined the term “altruism” as a cornerstone for his ethic doctrine Positivism, describing it as selfless concern for another’s welfare (Sutton, 1982). The existence of altruism and basic human kindness has been heavily debated throughout history, permeating through psychological, philosophical, and theological fields. Early philosophical and religious figures discussed concepts such Saint Augustine’s theory of conscience – that basic human kindness was an innate human feature (Fortin, 1996) – or Thomas Aquinas’ synderesis rule, which states that humans desire to be good and can innately distinguish between right and wrong (Davies, 2014). While these speculations have little or no empirical basis, they do have something in common with more recent research: they view altruism as having an innate component. Augustine and Aquinas attributed God as the innate origin of human altruism. Evolutionary psychologists view altruism as genetically influenced, having arisen, in part, from the Darwinian selective forces of natural and sexual selection.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbot, P., Abe, J., Alcock, J., et al. (2011). Inclusive fitness theory and eusociality. Nature, 471(7339), E1E4.Google Scholar
Ackerman, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., & Schaller, M. (2007). Is friendship akin to kinship? Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 365374.Google Scholar
Allen, A. P., Dinan, T. G., Clarke, G., & Cryan, J. F. (2017). A psychology of the human brain–gut–microbiome axis. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 11(4). e12309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersson, M. (1984). The evolution of eusociality. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 15, 165189.Google Scholar
Andreoni, J., & Petrie, R. (2004). Public goods experiments without confidentiality: A glimpse into fund-raising. Journal of Public Economics, 88(7), 16051623.Google Scholar
Ariely, D., Bracha, A., & Meier, S. (2009). Doing good or doing well? Image motivation and monetary incentives in behaving prosocially. American Economic Review, 99(1), 544555.Google Scholar
Arnocky, S., Piché, T., Albert, G., Ouellette, D., & Barclay, P. (2016). Altruism predicts mating success in humans. British Journal of Psychology, 108, 416435.Google Scholar
Arnold, W. (1990a). The evolution of marmot sociality: I. Why disperse late? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 27(4), 229237.Google Scholar
Arnold, W. (1990b). The evolution of marmot sociality: II. Costs and benefits of joint hibernation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 27(4), 239246.Google Scholar
Ashworth, T. (2000). Trench Warfare, 1914–1918: The Live and Let Live System, 2nd ed. Basingstoke: Pan Macmillan.Google Scholar
Axelrod, R. (1984). The Evolution of Co-operation. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, 211(4489), 13901396.Google Scholar
Barclay, P. (2010). Altruism as a courtship display: Some effects of third-party generosity on audience perceptions. British Journal of Psychology, 101(1), 123135.Google Scholar
Barclay, P. (2011). Competitive helping increases with the size of biological markets and invades defection. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 281, 4755.Google Scholar
Barclay, P. (2013). Strategies for cooperation in biological markets, especially for humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(3), 164175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barclay, P. (2016). Biological markets and the effects of partner choice on cooperation and friendship. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 3338.Google Scholar
Baum, W. M., Richerson, P. J., Efferson, C. M., & Paciotti, B. M. (2004). Cultural evolution in laboratory micro-societies including traditions of rule-giving and rule-following. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 305326.Google Scholar
Bell, D. C., & Richard, A. J. (2000). Caregiving: The forgotten element in attachment. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 6983.Google Scholar
Bell, G. (2008). Selection: The Mechanism of Evolution, 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bercik, P., Verdu, E. F., Foster, J. A., et al. (2010). Chronic gastrointestinal inflammation induces anxiety-like behavior and alters central nervous system biochemistry in mice. Gastroenterology, 139(6), 21022112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bercik, P., Park, A. J., Sinclair, D., et al. (2011). The anxiolytic effect of Bifidobacterium longum NCC3001 involves vagal pathways for gut–brain communication. Neurogastroenterology & Motility, 23(12), 11321139.Google Scholar
Berte, N. A. (1988). K’ekchi’ horticultural labor exchange: Productive and reproductive implications. In Betzig, L., Borgerhofl Mulder, M., & Turke, P., eds., Human Reproductive Behavior: A Darwinian Perspective. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 8395.Google Scholar
Bhogal, M. S. (2017). Physical Attractiveness, Altruism and Fairness in a Game-Theoretic Framework. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Wolverhampton.Google Scholar
Blumstein, D. T., Steinmetz, J., Armitage, K. B., & Daniel, J. C. (1997). Alarm calling in yellow-bellied marmots: II. The importance of direct fitness. Animal Behaviour, 53(1), 173184.Google Scholar
Bowles, S. (2009). Did warfare among ancestral hunter–gatherers affect the evolution of human social behaviors? Science, 324(5932), 12931298.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1982). Cultural transmission and the evolution of cooperative behavior. Human Ecology, 10(3), 325351.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2005). The Origin and Evolution of Cultures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bravo, J. A., Forsythe, P., Chew, M. V., et al. (2011). Ingestion of Lactobacillus strain regulates emotional behavior and central GABA receptor expression in a mouse via the vagus nerve. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(38), 1605016055.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bressan, P., Colarelli, S. M., & Cavalieri, M. B. (2009). Biologically costly altruism depends on emotional closeness among step but not half or full sibling. Evolutionary Psychology, 7(1), 118132.Google Scholar
Brown, S. L., & Brown, R. M. (2006). Selective investment theory: Recasting the functional significance of close relationships. Psychological Inquiry, 17, 129.Google Scholar
Browning, L. E., Patrick, S. C., Rollins, L. A., Griffith, S. C., & Russell, A. F. (2012). Kin selection, not group augmentation, predicts helping in an obligate cooperatively breeding bird. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 279(1743), 38613869.Google Scholar
Burda, H., Honeycutt, R. L., Begall, S., Locker-Grütjen, O., & Scharff, A. (2000). Are naked and common mole-rats eusocial and if so, why? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 47(5), 293303.Google Scholar
Burnstein, E., Crandall, C., & Kitayama, S. (1994). Some neo-Darwinian decision rules for altruism: Weighing cues for inclusive fitness as a function of the biological importance of the decision. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(5), 773789.Google Scholar
Carter, G., & Wilkinson, G. (2013). Does food sharing in vampire bats demonstrate reciprocity? Communicative & integrative biology, 6(6), e25783.Google Scholar
Carpenter, J., & Myers, C. K. (2010). Why volunteer? Evidence on the role of altruism, image, and incentives. Journal of Public Economics, 94(11), 911920.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1985). Vervet monkey alarm calls: Manipulation through shared information? Behaviour, 94(1), 150166.Google Scholar
Chiang, Y. S. (2010). Self-interested partner selection can lead to the emergence of fairness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(4), 265270.Google Scholar
Cialdini, R. B., Brown, S. L., Lewis, B. P., Luce, C., & Neuberg, S. L. (1997). Reinterpreting the empathy–altruism relationship: When one into one equals oneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(3), 481494.Google Scholar
Clark, M. S., & Mills, J. (1993). The difference between communal and exchange relationships: What it is and is not. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19(6), 684691.Google Scholar
Clutton-Brock, T. (2009). Cooperation between non-kin in animal societies. Nature, 462(7269), 5157.Google Scholar
Colquhoun, L., Davies, J., & Workman, L. (2017). Kith or kin – Who do we favour? The relationship between emotional closeness and genetic relatedness in altruistic willingness to help. Presented at British Psychological Society Annual Conference.Google Scholar
Crittenden, A. N., & Marlowe, F. W. (2008). Allomaternal care among the Hadza of Tanzania. Human Nature, 19(3), 249262.Google Scholar
Curry, O., Roberts, S. G., & Dunbar, R. I. (2013). Altruism in social networks: Evidence for a “kinship premium.” British Journal of Psychology, 104(2), 283295.Google Scholar
Cutts, C., & Speakman, J. (1994). Energy savings in formation flight of pink-footed geese. Journal of Experimental Biology, 189(1), 251261.Google Scholar
Danielsbacka, M., & Tanskanen, A. O. (2012). Adolescent grandchildren’s perceptions of grandparents’ involvement in UK: An interpretation from life course and evolutionary theory perspective. European Journal of Ageing, 9, 329341.Google Scholar
Danielsbacka, M., Tanskanen, A. O., Jokela, M., & Rotkirch, A. (2011). Grandparental child care in Europe: Evidence for preferential investment in more certain kin. Evolutionary Psychology, 9, 324.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1859). On The Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Davies, B. (2014). Thomas Aquinas’s Summa Theologiae: A Guide and Commentary. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (2016). The Selfish Gene. 4th ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
DeNault, L. K., & McFarlane, D. A. (1995). Reciprocal altruism between male vampire bats, Desmodus rotundus. Animal Behaviour, 49(3), 855856.Google Scholar
Deulofeu, J. (2014). Prisioneros con dilemas y estrategias dominantes: Teoría de juegos. [Prisoners with dilemmas and dominant strategies: Game theory], trans. London: Windmill Books and Vespa Design, RBA Collectibles.Google Scholar
Diamond, J. (2012). The World Until Yesterday: What Can We Learn from Traditional Societies? London: Penguin UK.Google Scholar
Dierkes, P., Heg, D., Taborsky, M., Skubic, E., & Achmann, R. (2005). Genetic relatedness in groups is sex‐specific and declines with age of helpers in a cooperatively breeding cichlid. Ecology Letters, 8(9), 968975.Google Scholar
Dobson, A. P. (1988). The population biology of parasite-induced changes in host behavior. Quarterly Review of Biology, 63(2), 139165.Google Scholar
Duffy, J. E., Morrison, C. L., & Ríos, R. (2000). Multiple origins of eusociality among sponge-dwelling shrimps (Synalpheus). Evolution, 54(2), 503516.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I., Clark, A., & Hurst, N. L. (1995). Conflict and cooperation among the Vikings: Contingent behavioral decisions. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16(3), 233246.Google Scholar
Efferson, C., Lalive, R., Richerson, P. J., McElreath, R., & Lubell, M. (2008). Conformists and mavericks: The empirics of frequency-dependent cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(1), 5664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, A. R. (1988). Grandchildren’s perspectives on relationships with grandparents: The influence of gender across generations. Sex Roles, 19, 205217.Google Scholar
Emlen, S. T., & Wrege, P. H. (1988). The role of kinship in helping decisions among white-fronted bee-eaters. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 23(5), 305315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Essock-Vitale, S. M., & McGuire, M. T. (1980). Predictions derived from the theories of kin selection and reciprocation assessed by anthropological data. Ethology and Sociobiology, 1(3), 233243.Google Scholar
Essock-Vitale, S. M., & McGuire, M. T. (1985). Women’s lives viewed from an evolutionary perspective. II. Patterns of helping. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6(3), 155173.Google Scholar
Euler, H. A., & Weitzel, B. (1996). Discriminative grandparental solicitude as reproductive strategy. Human Nature, 7, 3959.Google Scholar
Feder, H. M. (1966). Cleaning symbioses in the marine environment. In Henry, S. M., ed., Associations of Microorganisms, Plants and Marine Organisms, Vol. 1. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press, pp. 327380.Google Scholar
Feldman, M., & Cavalli-Sforna, L. (1976). Cultural and biological evolutionary processes, selection for a trait under complex transmission. Theoretical Population Biology, 9, 238259.Google Scholar
Filiz-Ozbay, E., & Ozbay, E. Y. (2014). Effect of an audience in public goods provision. Experimental Economics, 17(2), 200214.Google Scholar
Fisher, R. A. (1930). The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection. Wotton-under-Edge: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Fortin, E. L. (1996). Classical Christianity and the Political Order: Reflections on the Theologico-political Problem. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Frank, R. H. (1993). The strategic role of the emotions reconciling over-and undersocialized accounts of behavior. Rationality and Society, 5(2), 160184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaulin, S. J., & Schlegel, A. (1980). Paternal confidence and paternal investment: A cross cultural test of a sociobiological hypothesis. Ethology and Sociobiology, 1(4), 301309.Google Scholar
Goehler, L. E., Park, S. M., Opitz, N., Lyte, M., & Gaykema, R. P. (2008). Campylobacter jejuni infection increases anxiety-like behavior in the holeboard: Possible anatomical substrates for viscerosensory modulation of exploratory behavior. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 22(3), 354366.Google Scholar
Gorrell, J. C., McAdam, A. G., Coltman, D. W., Humphries, M. M., & Boutin, S. (2010). Adopting kin enhances inclusive fitness in asocial red squirrels. Nature Communications, 1, 22.Google Scholar
Grafen, A. (1990). Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 144, 517546.Google Scholar
Grayson, D. K. (1993). Differential mortality and the Donner Party disaster. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 2(5), 151159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grey, P. B., & Brogdon, E. (2017). Do step- and biological grandparents show differences in investment and emotional closeness with their grandchildren? Evolutionary Psychology, 15(1), 19.Google Scholar
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., & Van den Bergh, B. (2010). Going green to be seen: Status, reputation, and conspicuous conservation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 392404.Google Scholar
Gross, M. R., & Shine, R. (1981). Parental care and mode of fertilization in ectothermic vertebrates. Evolution, 35(4), 775793.Google Scholar
Gurven, M., Allen-Arave, W., Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (2001). Reservation food sharing among the Ache of Paraguay. Human Nature, 12(4), 273297.Google Scholar
Haldane, J. B. S. (1932). The Causes of Evolution, 3rd ed. Harlow: Longmans, Green & Co.Google Scholar
Haldane, J. B. S. (1955). Population genetics. New Biology, 18, 3451.Google Scholar
Hames, R. (1987). Garden labor exchange among the Ye’kwana. Ethology and Sociobiology, 8(4), 259284.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964a). The genetical evolution of social behaviour I. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 116.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964b). The genetical evolution of social behaviour II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 1752.Google Scholar
Hawkes, K. (1991). Showing off: Tests of an hypothesis about men’s foraging goals. Ethology and Sociobiology, 12(1), 2954.Google Scholar
He, W. (2005). Introduction: Kinship and family in international context. International Journal of Sociology and Social Policy, 25(3), 18.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2007). Dual-inheritance theory: The evolution of human cultural capacities and cultural evolution. In Dunbar, R. I. M. & Barrett, L., eds., Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 555570.Google Scholar
Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (1996). Ache Life History: The Ecology and Demography of a Foraging People. London: Aldine.Google Scholar
Honeycutt, R. L. (1992). Naked mole-rats. American Scientist, 80(1), 4353.Google Scholar
Hughes, W. O., Oldroyd, B. P., Beekman, M., & Ratnieks, F. L. (2008). Ancestral monogamy shows kin selection is key to the evolution of eusociality. Science, 320(5880), 12131216.Google Scholar
Johnson, L. L., & Boyce, M. S. (1991). Female choice of males with low parasite loads in sage grouse. In Loye, J. E. & Zuk, M., eds., Bird Parasite Interactions: Ecology, Evolution, and Behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 377388.Google Scholar
Karlan, D., & McConnell, M. A. (2014). Hey look at me: The effect of giving circles on giving. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 106, 402412.Google Scholar
Kobayashi, Y., & Yamamura, N. (2007). Evolution of signal emission by uninfested plants to help nearby infested relatives. Evolutionary Ecology, 21(3), 281294.Google Scholar
Korchmaros, J. D., & Kenny, D. A. (2001). Emotional closeness as a mediator of the effect of genetic relatedness on altruism. Psychological Science, 12(3), 262265.Google Scholar
Korchmaros, J. D., & Kenny, D. A. (2006). An evolutionary and close-relationship model of helping. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 23(1), 2143.Google Scholar
Kruger, D. J. (2003). Evolution and altruism: Combining psychological mediators with naturally selected tendencies. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(2), 118125.Google Scholar
Lacetera, N., & Macis, M. (2010a). Do all material incentives for pro-social activities backfire? The response to cash and non-cash incentives for blood donations. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31(4), 738748.Google Scholar
Lacetera, N., & Macis, M. (2010b). Social image concerns and prosocial behavior: Field evidence from a nonlinear incentive scheme. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 76(2), 225237.Google Scholar
Laham, S. M., Gonsalkorale, K., & von Hippel, W. (2005). Darwinian grandparenting: Preferential investment in more certain kin. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 6372.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., & Brown, G. R. (2011). Sense and Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behaviour. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lee, R. B. (1979). The !Kung San: Men, Women, and Work in a Foraging Society. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lee, T. R., Mancini, J. A., & Maxwell, J. W. (1990). Sibling relationships in adulthood: Contact patterns and motivations. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52(2), 431440.Google Scholar
Lehmann, L., Rousset, F., Roze, D., & Keller, L. (2007). Strong reciprocity or strong ferocity? A population genetic view of the evolution of altruistic punishment. American Naturalist, 170(1), 2136.Google Scholar
Lehmann, L., Feldman, M. W., & Foster, K. R. (2008). Cultural transmission can inhibit the evolution of altruistic helping. American Naturalist, 172(1), 1224.Google Scholar
Lentz, B. F., & Laband, D. N. (1989). Why so many children of doctors become doctors: Nepotism vs. human capital transfers. Journal of Human Resources, 24(3), 396413.Google Scholar
Lewin-Epstein, O., Aharonov, R., & Hadany, L. (2017). Microbes can help explain the evolution of host altruism. Nature Communications, 8, 14040.Google Scholar
Linardi, S., & McConnell, M. (2011). No excuses for good behavior. Journal of Public Economics, 95(5–6), 445454.Google Scholar
Littlefield, C. H., & Rushton, J. P. (1986). When a child dies: The sociobiology of bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(4), 797802.Google Scholar
Low, B. S., & Heinen, J. T. (1993). Population, resources, and environment: Implications of human behavioral ecology for conservation. Population and Environment, 15(1), 741.Google Scholar
Madsen, E. A., Tunney, R. J., Fieldman, G., et al. (2007). Kinship and altruism: A cross‐cultural experimental study. British Journal of Psychology, 98(2), 339359.Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1955). Characteristics of some animal calls. Nature, 176, 68.Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1957). Specific distinctiveness in the communication signals of birds. Behaviour, 11(1), 1338.Google Scholar
Maynard Smith, J. (1964). Group selection and kin selection. Nature, 201, 11451147.Google Scholar
McCullough, J. M., & Barton, E. Y. (1991). Relatedness and mortality risk during a crisis year: Plymouth colony, 1620–1621. Ethology and Sociobiology, 12(3), 195209.Google Scholar
McCullough, J. M., Heath, K. M., & Fields, J. D. (2006). Culling the cousins: Kingship, kinship, and competition in mid-millennial England. History of the Family, 11(1), 5966.Google Scholar
McElreath, R., Lubell, M., Richerson, P. J., et al. (2005). Applying evolutionary models to the laboratory study of social learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(6), 483508.Google Scholar
Milius, S. (1998). The science of EEEEEK!: What a squeak can tell researchers about life, society, and all that. Science News, 154(11), 174175.Google Scholar
Mills, J., & Clark, M. S. (1994). Communal and exchange relationships: Controversies and research. In Erber, R. & Gilmour, R., eds., Theoretical Frameworks for Personal Relationships. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 2942.Google Scholar
Mongkolsamrit, S., Kobmoo, N., Tasanathai, K., et al. (2012). Life cycle, host range and temporal variation of Ophiocordyceps unilateralis/Hirsutella formicarum on Formicine ants. Journal of Invertebrate Pathology, 111(3), 217224.Google Scholar
Moore, J. (1984). The evolution of reciprocal sharing. Ethology and Sociobiology, 5(1), 514.Google Scholar
Morris, K., & Goodall, J (1977). Competition for meat between chimpanzees and baboons of the Gombe National Park. Folia Primatologica. 28, 109121.Google Scholar
Murphy, G. P., & Dudley, S. A. (2009). Kin recognition: Competition and cooperation in Impatiens (Balsaminaceae). American Journal of Botany, 96(11), 19901996.Google Scholar
Neyer, F. J., & Lang, F. R. (2003). Blood is thicker than water: Kinship orientation across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 310321.Google Scholar
Nishida, T., Uehara, S., & Nyundo, R (1979). Predatory behavior among wild chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. Primates, 20, 120.Google Scholar
Nowak, M., & Sigmund, K. (2005) Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature, 437, 12911298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowak, M. A., Tarnita, C. E., & Wilson, E. O. (2010). The evolution of eusociality. Nature, 466(7310), 10571062.Google Scholar
Owens, D., & Owens, M. (1984). Helping behaviour in brown hyenas. Nature, 308(5962), 843845.Google Scholar
Packer, C. (1977). Reciprocal altruism in olive baboons. Nature, 265, 441443.Google Scholar
Panchanathan, K., & Boyd, R. (2004). Indirect reciprocity can stabilize cooperation without the second-order free rider problem. Nature, 432(7016), 499502.Google Scholar
Pashos, A., & McBurney, D. H. (2008). Kin relationships and the caregiving biases of grandparents, aunts and uncles: A two generational questionnaire study. Human Nature, 19, 311330.Google Scholar
Pashos, A., Schwarz, S., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2016). Kin investment by step-grandparents – More than expected. Evolutionary Psychology, 14(1), 111.Google Scholar
Pollet, T. V., Roberts, S. G., & Dunbar, R. I. (2013). Going that extra mile: Individuals travel further to maintain face-to-face contact with highly related kin than with less related kin. PLoS ONE, 8(1), e53929.Google Scholar
Portin, P. (2015). A comparison of biological and cultural evolution. Journal of Genetics, 94(1), 155168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Portugal, S. J., Hubel, T. Y., Fritz, J., et al. (2014). Upwash exploitation and downwash avoidance by flap phasing in ibis formation flight. Nature, 505(7483), 399402.Google Scholar
Refardt, D., Bergmiller, T., & Kümmerli, R. (2013). Altruism can evolve when relatedness is low: Evidence from bacteria committing suicide upon phage infection. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 280(1759), 20123035.Google Scholar
Rege, M., & Telle, K. (2004). The impact of social approval and framing on cooperation in public good situations. Journal of Public Economics, 88(7), 16251644.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Biology. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., Baldini, R., Bell, A. V., et al. (2016). Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: A sketch of the evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, 168.Google Scholar
Riss, D. C., & Busse, C. D (1977). Fifty-day observation of a free-ranging adult male chimpanzee. Folia Primatologica. 28, 283297.Google Scholar
Rudebeck, G. (1950). The choice of prey and modes of hunting of predatory birds with special reference to their selective effect. Oikos, 2(1), 6588.Google Scholar
Rudebeck, G. (1951). The choice of prey and modes of hunting of predatory birds with special reference to their selective effect. Oikos, 3(2), 200231.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (1984). Grooming, alliances, and reciprocal altruism in vervet monkeys. Nature, 308, 541543.Google Scholar
Shavit, Y., Fischer, C. S., & Koresh, Y. (1994). Kin and nonkin under collective threat: Israeli networks during the gulf war. Social Forces, 72(4), 11971215.Google Scholar
Singer, P. (1981). The Expanding Circle: Ethics and Sociobiology. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Smith, M. S., Kish, B. J., & Crawford, C. B. (1987). Inheritance of wealth as human kin investment. Ethology and Sociobiology, 8(3), 171182.Google Scholar
Sober, E., & Wilson, D. S. (1998). Unto Others: The Evolution and Psychology of Unselfish Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Soler, M., Soler, J. J., Martinez, J. G., & Moller, A. P. (1995). Magpie host manipulation by great spotted cuckoos: Evidence for an avian mafia? Evolution, 49, 770775.Google Scholar
Soltis, J., Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1995). Can group-functional behaviors evolve by cultural group selection?: An empirical test. Current Anthropology, 36(3), 473494.Google Scholar
Sprankel, H., Richarz, K., Ludwig, H., & Rott, R. (1978). Behavior alterations in tree shrews (Tupaia glis, Diard 1820) induced by Borna disease virus. Medical Microbiology and Immunology, 165(1), 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart-Williams, S. (2007). Altruism among kin vs. nonkin: Effects of cost of help and reciprocal exchange. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(3), 193198.Google Scholar
Stewart-Williams, S. (2018). The Ape That Understood the Universe: How the Mind and Culture Evolve. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Storey, S., & Workman, L. (2013). The effects of temperature priming on cooperation in the iterated prisoner’s dilemma. Evolutionary Psychology, 11(1), 5267.Google Scholar
Sutton, M. (1982). Nationalism, Positivism, and Catholicism. The Politics of Charles Maurras and French Catholics 1890–1914. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, L. (1960). The Dynamics of Insect and Bird Populations in Pine Woods. Leiden: Brill Archive.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 3557.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1985). Social Evolution. San Francisco, CA: Benjamin/Cummings.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (2011). The Folly of Fools: The Logic of Deceit and Self-Deception in Human Life. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Voelkl, B., Portugal, S. J., Unsöld, M., et al. (2015). Matching times of leading and following suggest cooperation through direct reciprocity during V-formation flight in ibis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(7), 21152120.Google Scholar
Waibel, M., Keller, L., & Floreano, D. (2009). Genetic team composition and level of selection in the evolution of cooperation. IEEE Transactions on Evolutionary Computation, 13(3), 648660.Google Scholar
Waibel, M., Floreano, D., & Keller, L. (2011). A quantitative test of Hamilton’s rule for the evolution of altruism. PLoS Biology, 9(5), e1000615.Google Scholar
Waynforth, D. (2011). Mate choice and sexual selection. In Swami, V., ed., Evolutionary Psychology: A Critical Introduction. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 107130.Google Scholar
Weimerskirch, H., Martin, J., Clerquin, Y., Alexandre, P., & Jiraskova, S. (2001). Energy saving in flight formation. Nature, 413(6857), 697698.Google Scholar
West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., & Gardner, A. (2007a). Evolutionary explanations for cooperation. Current Biology, 17(16), R661R672.Google Scholar
West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., & Gardner, A. (2007b). Social semantics: Altruism, cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 20(2), 415432.Google Scholar
West, S. A., El Mouden, C., & Gardner, A. (2011). Sixteen common misconceptions about the evolution of cooperation in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(4), 231262.Google Scholar
Wheeler, B. C. (2008). Selfish or altruistic? An analysis of alarm call function in wild capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella nigritus. Animal Behaviour, 76(5), 14651475.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, G. S. (1984). Reciprocal food sharing in the vampire bat. Nature, 308(5955), 181184.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S. (2007). Evolution for Everyone: How Darwin’s Theory Can Change the Way We Think About Our Lives. New York: Delacorte Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S., & Brown, J. M. (1994). Poecilochirus carabi: Behavioral and life-history adaptations to different hosts and the consequences of geographical shifts in host communities. In Houck, M. A., ed., Mites: Ecological and Evolutionary Analyses of Life-History Patterns. Berlin: Springer, pp. 122.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S., & Sober, E. (1994). Reintroducing group selection to the human behavioral sciences. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 17, 585654.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S., & Wilson, E. O. (2007). Rethinking the theoretical foundation of sociobiology. Quarterly Review of Biology, 82(4), 327348.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (2005). Kin selection as the key to altruism: Its rise and fall. Social Research, 72(1), 159166.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (2012). The Social Conquest of Earth. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Woolfenden, G. E. (1975). Florida scrub jay helpers at the nest. The Auk, 92(1), 115.Google Scholar
Workman, L., & Reader, W. (2014). Evolutionary Psychology, 3rd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Workman, L., & Reader, W. (2015). Evolution and Behavior. Abingdon: Routledge.Google Scholar
Wynne-Edwards, V. C. (1962). Animal Dispersion: In Relation to Social Behaviour. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd.Google Scholar
Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection: A selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205214.Google Scholar
Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The Handicap Principle: A Missing Piece of Darwin’s Puzzle. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ziker, J. (2005). Food sharing at meals: Kinship, reciprocity, and clustering in the Taimyr autonomous Okrug, northern Russia. Human Nature, 16, 178211.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×