Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-nr4z6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-15T07:42:52.345Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

27 - Creativity in Non-Human Animals

from Part V - Newer Domains for Creativity Research

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 September 2017

James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Vlad P. Glăveanu
Affiliation:
Universitetet i Bergen, Norway
John Baer
Affiliation:
Rider University, New Jersey
Get access

Summary

Abstract

While they are not traditionally associated with creativity, non-human animals often use creative behaviors, innovative tools, and social learning to increase evolutionary fitness. This creativity can result in better foraging, higher mating success, increased social standing, better problem-solving abilities, or any number of other adaptive traits. This chapter discusses some general subcategories of creativity and innovation in non-human animals, along with key studies in the area.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, S. J., Bejder, L., & Krützen, M. (2010). Why do Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.) carry conch shells (Turbinella sp.) in Shark Bay, Western Australia? Marine Mammal Science, 27(2), 449454. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748–7692.2010.00409.xGoogle Scholar
Ashe, E. E., Heithaus, M. R., & Marshall, G. J. (2003). Studies of foraging in “southern resident” killer whales during July 2002: Dive depths, bursts in speed, and the use of a “Crittercam” system for examining sub-surface behavior. National Marine Fisheries Service, National Marine Mammal Laboratory.Google Scholar
Auersperg, A. M. I., Szabo, B., von Bayern, A. M. P., & Kacelnik, A. (2012). Spontaneous innovation in tool manufacture and use in a Goffin’s cockatoo. Current Biology, 22(21), R903904. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.002Google Scholar
Auersperg, A. M. I., van Horik, J. O., Bugnyar, T., Kacelnik, A., Emery, N. J., & von Bayern, A. M. P. (2015). Combinatory actions during object play in parrots (Psittacus erithacus) and corvids (Corvus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1, 6271.Google Scholar
Auersperg, A. M. I., von Bayern, A. M. I., Weber, S., Szabadvari, A., Bugnyar, T., & Kacelnik, A. (2014). Social transmission of tool use and tool manufacture in Goffin cockatoos (Cacatua goffini). Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1793), 2014097220140972. http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.0972Google Scholar
Bell, A. M. (2007). Evolutionary biology: Animal personalities. Nature, 447, 539540.Google Scholar
Belzung, C. (2010). Open-field test. In Stolerman, I. P. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychopharmacology (pp. 920926). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Beran, M. J., & Parrish, A. E. (2012). Sequential responding and planning in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Animal Cognition, 15(6), 10851094.Google Scholar
Boogert, N. J., Reader, S. M., Hoppitt, W., & Laland, K. N. (2008). The origin and spread of innovations in starlings. Animal Behaviour, 75(4), 15091518. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.033Google Scholar
Boogert, N. J., Reader, S. M., & Laland, K. N. (2006). The relation between social rank, neophobia and individual learning in starlings. Animal Behaviour, 72(6), 12291239. Retrieved on November 10, 2016, from www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6W9 W-4M1DB4 C-1/2/a54cdd8d1e495ece5335553bc0a6b1d8Google Scholar
Bourjade, M., Call, J., Pelé, M., Maumy, M., & Dufour, V. (2014). Bonobos and orangutans, but not chimpanzees, flexibly plan for the future in a token-exchange task. Animal Cognition. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014–0768-6Google Scholar
Brosnan, S., & Hopper, L. (2014). Psychological limits on animal innovation. Animal Behaviour. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.02.026Google Scholar
Brydson, R. (2014). Incipient tradition in wild chimpanzees. Physical Sciences, 12, 910.Google Scholar
Burkart, J. M., Strasser, A., & Foglia, M. (2009). Trade-offs between social learning and individual innovativeness in common marmosets, Callithrix jacchus. Animal Behaviour, 77(5), 12911301. Retrieved on November 15, 2016, from www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6W9 W-4VW4VC9-3/2/b5a6092d93262e59736d231c16d00926Google Scholar
Cantor, M., & Whitehead, H. (2013). The interplay between social networks and culture: theoretically and among whales and dolphins. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 368(1618), 20120340. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0340Google Scholar
Cnotka, J., Gunturkun, O., Rehkämpera, G., Gray, R. D., & Hunt, G. R. (2008). Extraordinary large brains in tool-using New Caledonian crows. Neuroscience Letters, 433, 241245.Google Scholar
Day, R. L., Coe, R. L., Kendal, J. R., & Laland, K. N. (2003). Neophilia, innovation and social learning: A study of intergeneric differences in callitrichid monkeys. Animal Behavior, 65(3), 559572.Google Scholar
Dellu, F., Mayo, W., Piazza, P. V. V, Le Moal, M., & Simon, H. (1993). Individual differences in behavioral responses to novelty in rats. Possible relationship with the sensation-seeking trait in man. Personality and Individual Differences, 15(4), 411418. http://doi.org/10.1016/0191–8869(93)90069-FGoogle Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Sources of openness/intellect: cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality. Journal of Personality, 73(4), 825858.Google Scholar
Dinets, V., & Brueggen, J. C. J. D. (2013). Crocodilians use tools for hunting. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 27(1), 15. http://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2013.858276Google Scholar
Fisher, J., & Hinde, R. A. (1949). The opening of milk bottles by birds. British Birds, 42, 347357.Google Scholar
Freeman, H. D., Brosnan, S. F., Hopper, L. M., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2013). Developing a comprehensive and comparative questionnaire for measuring personality in chimpanzees using a simultaneous top-down/bottom-up design. American Journal of Primatology. http://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22168Google Scholar
Glăveanu, V. (2015). Proto-c creativity? In Kaufman, A. B. & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.), Animal creativity and innovation (pp. 120128). Waltham, MA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Griffin, A. S., Lermite, F., Perea, M., & Guez, D. (2013). To innovate or not: contrasting effects of social groupings on safe and risky foraging in Indian mynahs. Animal Behaviour. Retrieved on October 29, 2016 from www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0003347213004399Google Scholar
Heck, P. S., & Ghosh, S. (2000). A study of synthetic creativity: Behavior modeling and simulation of an ant colony. IEEE Expert Intelligent Systems and Their Applications, 15(6), 5867. http://doi.org/10.1109/5254.895863Google Scholar
Heck, P. S., & Ghosh, S. (2002). The design and role of synthetic creative traits in artificial ant colonies. Journal of Intelligent & Robotic Systems, 33(4), 343370. Retrieved on November 15, 2015 from https://link.springer.com/article/10.1023/A:1015552602374 <Go to ISI>://000175751000001://000175751000001>Google Scholar
Herrmann, E., & Call, J. (2012). Are there geniuses among the apes? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 367(1603), 2753–61. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0191Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (2012). What’s social about social learning? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 193202.Google Scholar
Highfill, L. E., & Kuczaj, S. A. (2007). Do bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) have distinct and stable personalities? Aquatic Mammals, 33(3), 380389.Google Scholar
Huebner, F., & Fichtel, C. (2015). Innovation and behavioral flexibility in wild redfronted lemurs (Eulemur rufifrons). Animal Cognition, 18(3), 777787. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015–0844-6Google Scholar
Huffman, M., & Hirata, S. (2003). Biological and ecological foundations of primate behavioral traditions. The biology of traditions: Models and evidence. In Fragaszy, Dorothy M. and Perry, Susan (Eds.), The Biology of Traditions (pp. 267296). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., & Beghetto, R. A. (2009). Beyond big and little: The four C model of creativity. Review of General Psychology, 13, 112.Google Scholar
Kaufman, A. B., Butt, A. E., Kaufman, J. C., & Colbert-White, E. N. (2011). Towards a neurobiology of creativity in nonhuman animals. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125(3), 255272.Google Scholar
Kaufman, A. B., & Kaufman, J. C. (2014). Applying theoretical models on human creativity to animal studies. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1(1), 7890. http://doi.org/10.12966/abc.02.01.2014Google Scholar
Kaufman, A. B., & Rosenthal, R. (2009). Can you believe my eyes? The importance of interobserver reliability statistics in observations of animal behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 78(6), 14871491. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.014Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C. (2009). Creativity 101. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., & Kaufman, A. B. (2004). Applying a creativity framework to animal cognition. New Ideas in Psychology, 22(2), 143155. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2004.09.006Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B. (2013). Opening up openness to experience: A four-factor model and relations to creative achievement in the arts and sciences. Journal of Creative Behavior, 47, 233255.Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B., & Kaufman, J. C. (2007). Ten years to expertise, many more to greatness: An investigation of modern writers. Journal of Creative Behavior, 41(2), 114124.Google Scholar
Kawai, M. (1965). Newly-acquired pre-cultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima islet. Primates, 6(1), 130.Google Scholar
Kenward, B., Weir, A. A. S., Rutz, C., & Kacelnik, A. (2005). Behavioural ecology: Tool manufacture by naive juvenile crows. Nature, 433(7022), 121.Google Scholar
Kopps, A. M., Ackermann, C. Y., Sherwin, W. B., Allen, S. J., Bejder, L., Krutzen, M., & Krützen, M. (2014). Cultural transmission of tool use combined with habitat specializations leads to fine-scale genetic structure in bottlenose dolphins. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1782), 2013324520133245. http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.3245Google Scholar
Krueger, K., Farmer, K., & Heinze, J. (2014). The effects of age, rank and neophobia on social learning in horses. Animal Cognition, 17(3), 645–55. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-013–0696-xGoogle Scholar
Kruetzen, M., Mann, J., Heithaus, M. R., Connor, R. C., Bejder, L., Sherwin, W. B., & Sherman, W. B. (2005). Cultural Transmission of Tool Use in Bottlenose Dolphins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(25), 89398943. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0500232102Google Scholar
Krützen, M., Kreicker, S., Macleod, C. D., Learmonth, J., Kopps, A. M., Walsham, P., & Allen, S. J. (2014). Cultural transmission of tool use by Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.) provides access to a novel foraging niche. Proceedings. Biological Sciences / The Royal Society, 281(1784), 20140374. http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.0374Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A., & Eskelinen, H. C. (2014). The “Creative Dolphin” revisited: What do dolphins do when asked to vary their behavior? Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1(1), 6677. http://doi.org/10.12966/abc.02.05.2014Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A., Gory, J. D., & Xitco, M. J. Jr (2009). How intelligent are dolphins? A partial answer based on their ability to plan their behavior when confronted with novel problems. Japanese Journal of Animal Psychology, 59(1), 99115.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A., Highfill, L., & Byerly, H. (2012). The importance of considering context in the assessment of personality characteristics: Evidence from ratings of dolphin personality. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 25(4), 309329. Retrieved from http://escholarship.org/uc/item/4hg0p6cq.pdfGoogle Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A., Makecha, R., Trone, M., Paulos, R. D. R. D., & Ramos, J. A. A. J. A. (2006). Role of peers in cultural innovation and cultural transmission: Evidence from the play of dolphin calves. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 19(2), 223240. Retrieved on October 25, 2016 from http://escholarship.org/uc/item/4pn1t50s.pdfGoogle Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A., Xitco, M. J. Jr, & Gory, J. D. (2010). Can dolphins plan their behavior? International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23(4), 664670. Retrieved on November 15, 2016, from http://comparativepsychology.org/ijcp-2010–4/07.Kuczaj_etal_Final.pdfGoogle Scholar
Lefebvre, L. (1995). The opening of milk bottles by birds: Evidence for accelerating learning rates, but against the wave-of-advance model of cultural transmission. Behavioral Processes, 34, 4354.Google Scholar
Lefebvre, L., Reader, S. M., & Sol, D. (2004). Brains, innovations, and evolution in birds and primates. Brain, Behavior, and Evolution, 63(4), 233246. http://doi.org/10.1159/000076784Google Scholar
Lefebvre, L., Reader, S. M., & Sol, D. (2013). Innovating innovation rate and its relationship with brains, ecology and general intelligence. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 81(3), 143–5. http://doi.org/10.1159/000348485Google Scholar
Lefebvre, L., Timmermans, S., & Rosza, L. (1998). Feeding Innovations and forebrain size in Australasian birds. Behaviour, 135, 10071097.Google Scholar
Lefebvre, L., Whittle, P., Lascaris, E., & Finklestein, A. (1997). Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds. Animal Behavior, 53, 549560.Google Scholar
Mann, J., Sargeant, B. L., Watson-Capps, J. J., Gibson, Q. A., Heithaus, M. R., Connor, R. C., & Patterson, E. (2008). Why do dolphins carry sponges? PloS One, 3(12), e3868. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003868Google Scholar
Mazur, R., & Seher, V. (2008). Socially learned foraging behaviour in wild black bears, Ursus americanus. Animal Behaviour, 75(4), 15031508. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.10.027Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 12581265.Google Scholar
Menzel, E. W., Davenport, R. K., & Rogers, C. M. (1961). Some aspects of behavior toward novelty in young chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 54, 1619.Google Scholar
Mery, F., & Burns, J. (2010). Behavioral plasticity: An interaction between evolution and experience. Evolutionary Ecology, 24, 571583.Google Scholar
Mitchell, R. W., Thompson, N. S., & Miles, H. L. (1997). Taking anthropomorphism and anecdotes seriously. In Mitchell, R. W., Thompson, N. S., & Miles, H. L. (Eds.), Anthropomorphism, anecdotes, and animals. Albany, New York: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Monteiro de Almeida Rocha, J., Pedreira dos Reis, P., & de Carvalho Oliveira, L. (2014). Play behavior of the golden-headed lion tamarin in Brazilian cocoa agroforests. Folia Primatologica, 85(3), 192199. http://doi.org/10.1159/10.1159/000362813Google Scholar
Montgomery, S. H. (2014). The relationship between play, brain growth and behavioural flexibility in primates. Animal Behaviour, 90, 281286. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.02.004Google Scholar
Morand-Ferron, J., Lefebvre, L., Reader, S. M., Sol, D., & Elvin, S. (2004). Dunking behaviour in Carib grackles. Animal Behaviour, 68(6), 12671274.Google Scholar
Ottoni, E. B., de Resende, B. D., Izar, P., & Resende, B. D. (2005). Watching the best nutcrackers: What capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) know about others’ tool-using skills. Animal Cognition, 24(4), 215219. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-004–0245-8Google Scholar
Overington, S. E., Morand-Ferron, J., Boogert, N. J., & Lefebvre, L. (2009). Technical innovations drive the relationship between innovativeness and residual brain size in birds. Animal Behaviour, 78(4), 10011010. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.033Google Scholar
Patricelli, G. L., Coleman, S. W., & Borgia, G. (2006). Male satin bowerbirds, Ptilonorhunchus violaceus, adjust their display intensity in response to female startling: An experiment with robotic females. Animal Behavior, 71(1), 4959. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.03.029Google Scholar
Pryor, K. W. (2014). Historical perspectives: A dolphin journey. Aquatic Mammals, 40(1), 104114.Google Scholar
Pryor, K. W., Haag, R., & O’Reilly, J. (1969). The creative porpoise: Training for novel behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12(4), 653. Retrieved on November 30, 2016, from www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1338662/Google Scholar
Ramsey, G., Bastian, M. L., & van Schaik, C. (2007). Animal innovation defined and operationalized. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(4), 393407; discussion 407–32. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X07002373Google Scholar
Reader, S. M., & Laland, K. N. (2002). Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99(7), 44364441. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.062041299Google Scholar
Reader, S. M., & Laland, K. N. (Eds.). (2003). Animal innovation: An introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Reader, S. M., & Laland, K. N. N. (2001). Primate innovation: Sex, age and social rank differences. International Journal of Primatology, 22(5), 787805. Retrieved on October 15, 2015, from www.springerlink.com/index/ln47117526184421.pdfGoogle Scholar
Reader, S. M., & MacDonald, K. (2003). Environmental variability and primate flexibility. In Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N. (Eds.), Animal innovation (pp. 83116). Oxford: Oxford Univ Press.Google Scholar
Reale, D., Reader, S. M., Sol, D., McDougall, P. T., Dingemanse, N. J., & Réale, D. (2007). Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biological Reviews, 82(2), 291318. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00010.xGoogle Scholar
Rendell, L. E., Mesnick, S. L., Dalebout, M. L., Burtenshaw, J., & Whitehead, H. (2011). Can genetic differences explain vocal dialect variation in sperm whales, Physetermacrocephalus? Behavior Genetics, 42(2), 332–43. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-011–9513-yGoogle Scholar
Rendell, L. E., & Whitehead, H. (2001). Culture in whales and dolphins. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(2), 309382. Retrieved on November 25, 2016, from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11530544 <Go to ISI>://000170177900061://000170177900061>Google Scholar
Rymer, T., Schradin, C., & Pillay, N. (2008). Social transmission of information about novel food in two populations of the African striped mouse, Rhabdomys pumilio. Animal Behaviour, 76(4), 12971304. Retrieved on November 15, 2016, from www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6W9W-4T1SKGD-1/2/05b77926cd3356c31853a5e8e5f24c39Google Scholar
Sargeant, B. L., & Mann, J. (2009). Developmental evidence for foraging traditions in wild bottlenose dolphins. Animal Behaviour, 78(3), 715721. Retrieved on November 24, 2016, from www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6W9W-4WWPFWR-2/2/c599b183fc722418d061b4c8005b81a2Google Scholar
Sedley, D. (2003). The Cambridge companion to Greek and Roman philosophy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sirianni, G., Mundry, R., & Boesch, C. (2015). When to choose which tool: Multidimensional and conditional selection of nut-cracking hammers in wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 100, 152165. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.11.022Google Scholar
Sol, D., & Lefebvre, L. (2000). Behavioral flexibility predicts invasion success in birds introduced to New Zealand. Oikos, 90, 599615.Google Scholar
Sol, D., Timmermans, S., & Lefebvre, L. (2002). Behavioral flexibility and invasion success in birds. Animal Behavior, 60, 495502.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1999). Handbook of creativity. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stöwe, M., Bugnyar, T., Heinrich, B., Kotrschal, K., & Stowe, M. (2006). Effects of group size on approach to novel objects in ravens (Corvus corax). Ethology, 112(11), 10791088. http://doi.org/doi:10.1111/j.1439–0310.2006.01273.xGoogle Scholar
Stöwe, M., & Kotrschal, K. (2007). Behavioural phenotypes may determine whether social context facilitates or delays novel object exploration in ravens (Corvus corax). Journal of Ornithology, 148(S2), 179184. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10336-007–0145-1Google Scholar
Thornton, A., & Malapert, A. (2009). Experimental evidence for social transmission of food acquisition techniques in wild meerkats. Animal Behaviour, 78(2), 255264. Retrieved on October 30, 2016, from www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6W9W-4WKJ5GC-1/2/12eb7df91fb86724be45a89cce069b1eGoogle Scholar
Weir, A. A. S., Chappell, J., & Kacelnik, A. (2002). Shaping of hooks in New Caledonian crows. Science (New York, N.Y.), 297(5583), 981. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1073433Google Scholar
Wimpenny, J. H., Weir, A. A. S., Clayton, L., Rutz, C., & Kacelnik, A. (2009). Cognitive processes associated with sequential tool use in New Caledonian crows. PloS One, 4(8), e6471. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006471Google Scholar
Wynne, C. D. L. (2004). The perils of anthropomorphism. Nature, 428(6983), 606.Google Scholar
Yi, X., Plucker, J. A., & Guo, J. (2015). Modeling influences on divergent thinking and artistic creativity. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 16, 6268. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2015.02.002Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×