Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T00:40:27.541Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Learning How to Solve Problems by Studying Examples

from Part II - Science and Math

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2019

John Dunlosky
Affiliation:
Kent State University, Ohio
Katherine A. Rawson
Affiliation:
Kent State University, Ohio
Get access

Summary

This chapter focuses on how students can learn to solve difficult problems by studying examples that have already been worked out. We discuss why example-based learning works and how it can best be implemented by educators.
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, D. M., McLaren, B. M., Durkin, K., Mayer, R. E., Rittle-Johnson, B., Isotani, S., & Van Velsen, M. (2014). Using erroneous examples to improve mathematics learning with a web-based tutoring system. Computers in Human Behavior, 36, 401411. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.053Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1993). Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R., Fincham, J. M., & Douglass, S. (1997). The role of examples and rules in the acquisition of a cognitive skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 932945. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.23.4.932Google Scholar
Atkinson, R. K. (2002). Optimizing learning from examples using animated pedagogical agents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 416427. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.94.2.416Google Scholar
Atkinson, R. K., Catrambone, R., & Merrill, M. M. (2003). Aiding transfer in statistical learning: Examining the use of conceptually-oriented equations and elaborations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 762773. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.762CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, R. K., Derry, S. J., Renkl, A., & Wortham, D. (2000). Learning from examples: Instructional principles from the worked examples research. Review of Educational Research, 70, 181214. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543070002181Google Scholar
Atkinson, R. K., Renkl, A., & Merrill, M. M. (2003). Transitioning from studying examples to solving problems: Effects of self-explanation prompts and fading worked-out steps. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 774783. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.774Google Scholar
Baars, M., Van Gog, T., De Bruin, A., & Paas, F. (2014). Effects of problem solving after worked example study on primary school children’s monitoring accuracy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 382391. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3008Google Scholar
Baars, M., Van Gog, T., De Bruin, A., & Paas, F. (2017). Effects of problem solving after worked example study on secondary school children’s monitoring accuracy. Educational Psychology, 37, 810834. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2016.1150419Google Scholar
Baars, M., Visser, S., Van Gog, T., De Bruin, A., & Paas, F. (2013). Completion of partially worked-out examples as a generation strategy for improving monitoring accuracy. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38, 395406. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2013.09.001Google Scholar
Baldwin, T. T. (1992). Effects of alternative modelling strategies on outcomes of interpersonal-skills training. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 147154. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.77.2.147Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1994). Self-efficacy. In Ramachaudran, V. S. (ed.), Encyclopedia of human behavior, Vol. 4 (pp. 7181). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Berthold, K. & Renkl, A. (2009). Instructional aids to support a conceptual understanding of multiple representations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 7087. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013247Google Scholar
Blandin, Y. & Proteau, L. (2000). On the cognitive basis of observational learning: Development of mechanisms for the detection and correction of errors. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A, 846867. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755917Google Scholar
Braaksma, M. A. H., Rijlaarsdam, G., & Van den Bergh, H. (2002). Observational learning and the effects of model-observer similarity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 405415. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.94.2.405CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, W. M. (1994). Using worked examples as an instructional support in the algebra classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 360367. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.86.3.360CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catrambone, R. (1996). Generalizing solution procedures learned from examples. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 10201031. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.4.1020Google Scholar
Catrambone, R. (1998). The subgoal learning model: Creating better examples so that students can solve novel problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 355376. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.127.4.355Google Scholar
Chandler, P. & Sweller, J. (1991). Cognitive load theory and the format of instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 8, 293332. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci0804_2Google Scholar
Chi, M. T. H., Bassok, M., Lewis, M. W., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 13, 145182. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1302_1Google Scholar
Chi, M. T. H., Roy, M., & Hausmann, R. G. M. (2008). Observing tutorial dialogues collaboratively: Insights about human tutoring effectiveness from vicarious learning. Cognitive Science, 32, 301341. https://doi.org/10.1080/03640210701863396Google Scholar
Cooper, G. & Sweller, J. (1987). Effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 347362. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.79.4.347CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, G., Tindall-Ford, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2001). Learning by imagining. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 6882. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.7.1.68Google Scholar
Driskell, J. E., Copper, C., & Moran, A. (1994). Does mental practice enhance performance? The Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 481492. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.79.4.481Google Scholar
Dunning, D., Johnson, K., Erlinger, J., & Kruger, J. (2003). Why people fail to recognize their own incompetence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 8387. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.01235Google Scholar
Durkin, K. & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2012). The effectiveness of using incorrect examples to support learning about decimal magnitude. Learning and Instruction, 22, 206214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.11.001Google Scholar
Ertelt, A., Renkl, A., & Spada, H. (2006). Making a difference: Exploiting the full potential of instructionally designed on-screen videos. In Barab, S. A., Hay, K. E., & Hickey, D. T. (eds.), Proceedings of the 7th International Conference of the Learning Sciences (pp. 154169). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Feltz, D. L. & Landers, D. M. (1983). The effects of mental practice on motor skill learning and performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport Psychology, 5, 2557. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsp.5.1.25Google Scholar
Fiorella, L. & Mayer, R. E. (2013). The relative benefits of learning by teaching and teaching expectancy. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38, 281288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2013.06.001Google Scholar
Fiorella, L. & Mayer, R. E. (2014). Role of expectations and explanations in learning by teaching. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 39, 7585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.01.001Google Scholar
Fiorella, L., Van Gog, T., Hoogerheide, V., & Mayer, R. E. (2017). It’s all a matter of perspective: Viewing first-person video modeling examples promotes learning of an assembly task. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109, 653665. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000161IssnGoogle Scholar
Foster, N. L., Rawson, K. A., & Dunslosky, J. (2018). Self-regulated learning of principle-based concepts: Do students prefer worked examples, faded examples, or problem solving? Learning and Instruction. 55, 124138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.10.002Google Scholar
Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K., & Catrambone, R. (2004). Designing instructional examples to reduce intrinsic cognitive load: Molar versus modular presentation of solution procedures. Instructional Science, 32, 3358. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:TRUC.0000021809.10236.71Google Scholar
Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K., & Catrambone, R. (2006). Can learning from molar and modular worked-out examples be enhanced by providing instructional explanations and prompting self-explanations? Learning and Instruction, 16, 104121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.02.007Google Scholar
Gick, M. L. & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 138. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(83)90002–6Google Scholar
Ginns, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). When imagining information is effective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 229251. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0361-476X(02)00016–4Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Alibali, M. W. (2012). Gesture’s role in learning and development. In Zelazo, P. (ed.), Oxford handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 953973). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gott, S. P., Parker Hall, E., Pokorny, R. A., Dibble, E., & Glaser, R. (1993). A naturalistic study of transfer: Adaptive expertise in technical domains. In Detterman, D. K. & Sternberg, R. J. (eds.), Transfer on trial: Intelligence, cognition, and instruction (pp. 258288). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Groenendijk, T., Janssen, T., Rijlaarsdam, G., & Van den Bergh, H. (2013). Learning to be creative: The effects of observational learning on students’ design products and processes. Learning and Instruction, 28, 3547. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.05.001Google Scholar
Große, C. S., & Renkl, A. (2007). Finding and fixing errors in worked examples: Can this foster learning outcomes? Learning and Instruction, 17, 612634. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.09.008Google Scholar
Hartmann, C., Rummel, N., & Van Gog, T. (2017). Productive or vicarious failure: Do students need to make every mistake by themselves? Paper presented at the 17th International Conference of the European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction (EARLI), Tampere, Finland.Google Scholar
Hilbert, T. S., Renkl, A., Schworm, S., Kessler, S., & Reiss, K. (2008). Learning to teach with worked-out examples: A computer-based learning environment for teachers. Journal of Computer-Assisted Learning, 24, 316332. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2729.2007.00266.xGoogle Scholar
Holyoak, K. J. (2005). Analogy. In Holyoak, K. J. & Morrison, R. G. (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 117142). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hoogerheide, V., Deijkers, L., Loyens, S. M. M., Heijltjes, A. E. G., & Van Gog, T. (2016). Gaining from explaining: Learning improves from explaining to fictitious others on video, not from writing to them. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 44–45, 95106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.02.005Google Scholar
Hoogerheide, V., Loyens, S. M. M., & Van Gog, T. (2014a). Effects of creating video-based modeling examples on learning and transfer. Learning and Instruction, 33, 108119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.04.005Google Scholar
Hoogerheide, V., Loyens, S. M. M., & Van Gog, T. (2014b). Comparing the effects of worked examples and modeling examples on learning. Computers in Human Behavior, 41, 8091. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.09.013Google Scholar
Hoogerheide, V., Loyens, S. M. M., & Van Gog, T. (2016). Learning from video modeling examples: Does gender matter? Instructional Science, 44, 6986. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-015-9360-yGoogle Scholar
Hoogerheide, V., Renkl, A., Fiorella, L., Paas, F., & Van Gog, T. (2018). Enhancing example-based learning: Teaching on video increases arousal and improves problem-solving performance. Journal of Educational Psychology. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000272Google Scholar
Hoogerheide, V., Van Wermeskerken, M., Loyens, S. M. M., & Van Gog, T. (2016). Learning from video modeling examples: Content kept equal, adults are more effective models than peers. Learning and Instruction, 44, 2230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2016.02.004Google Scholar
Hoogerheide, V., Van Wermeskerken, M. M., Van Nassau, H., & Van Gog, T. (2018). Model-observer similarity and task-appropriateness in learning from video-modeling examples: Do model and student gender affect test performance, self-efficacy, and perceived competence? Computers in Human Behavior, 89, 457464. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.11.012Google Scholar
Jarodzka, H., Balslev, T., Holmqvist, K. et al. (2012). Conveying clinical reasoning based on visual observation via eye-movement modelling examples. Instructional Science, 40, 813827. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-012-9218-5Google Scholar
Jarodzka, H., Van Gog, T., Dorr, M., Scheiter, K., & Gerjets, P. (2013). Learning to see: Guiding students’ attention via a model’s eye movements fosters learning. Learning and Instruction, 25, 6270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.11.004Google Scholar
Kalyuga, S. (2007). Expertise reversal effect and its implications for learner-tailored instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 509539. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-007-9054-3Google Scholar
Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., Tuovinen, J., & Sweller, J. (2001). When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 579588. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.93.3.579CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kant, J. M., Scheiter, K., & Oschatz, K. (2017). How to sequence video modeling examples and inquiry tasks to foster scientific reasoning. Learning and Instruction, 52, 4658. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.04.005Google Scholar
Kapur, M. (2008). Productive failure. Cognition and Instruction, 26, 379424. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370000802212669Google Scholar
Kapur, M. & Rummel, N. (2012). Productive failure in learning from generation and invention activities. Instructional Science, 40(4), 645650. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-012-9235-4Google Scholar
Koedinger, K. R. & Aleven, V. (2007). Exploring the assistance dilemma in experiments with cognitive tutors. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 239264. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-007-9049-0Google Scholar
Kopp, V., Stark, R., & Fischer, M. R. (2008). Fostering diagnostic knowledge through computer-supported, casebased worked examples: Effects of erroneous examples and feedback. Medical Education, 42, 823829. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2923.2008.03122.xGoogle Scholar
Kopp, V., Stark, R., Kühne‐Eversmann, L., & Fischer, M. R. (2009). Do worked examples foster medical students’ diagnostic knowledge of hyperthyroidism? Medical Education, 43, 12101217. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2923.2009.03531.xGoogle Scholar
Kostons, D., Van Gog, T., & Paas, F. (2012). Training self-assessment and task-selection skills: A cognitive approach to improving self-regulated learning. Learning and Instruction, 22, 121132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.08.004Google Scholar
Langton, S. R. H., Watt, R. J., & Bruce, V. (2000). Do the eyes have it? Cues to the direction of social attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 5059. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01436–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leahy, W., Hanham, J., & Sweller, J. (2015). High element interactivity information during problem solving may lead to failure to obtain the testing effect. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 291304. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9296-4Google Scholar
Leahy, W. & Sweller, J. (2005). Interactions among the imagination, expertise reversal and element interactivity effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 11, 266276. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.11.4.266Google Scholar
Leahy, W. & Sweller, J. (2008). The imagination effect increases with an increased intrinsic cognitive load. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 273283. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1373Google Scholar
Leppink, J., Paas, F., Van Gog, T., Van der Vleuten, C. P. M., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2014). Effects of pairs of problems and examples on task performance and different types of cognitive load. Learning and Instruction, 30, 3242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.12.001Google Scholar
Loibl, K., Roll, I., & Rummel, N. (2017). Towards a theory of when and how problem solving followed by instruction supports learning. Educational Psychology Review, 29, 693715. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-016-9379-xGoogle Scholar
Louwerse, M. M., Graesser, A. C., McNamara, D. S., & Lu, S. (2009). Embodied conversational agents as conversational partners. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 12441255. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1527Google Scholar
Lovett, M. C. (1992). Learning by problem solving versus by examples: The benefits of generating and receiving information. Proceedings of the 14th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 956961). Hillsdale: Erlaum.Google Scholar
Mason, L., Pluchino, P., & Tornatora, M. C. (2015). Eye-movement modeling of integrative reading of an illustrated text: Effects on processing and learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 41, 172187 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2015.01.004Google Scholar
Mason, L., Scheiter, K., & Tornatora, C. (2017). Using eye movements to model the sequence of text-picture processing for multimedia comprehension. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 33, 443460. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcal.12191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, R. E. & DaPra, C. S. (2012). An embodiment effect in computer-based learning with animated pedagogical agents. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 18, 239252. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028616Google Scholar
Mayer, R. E. & Wittrock, M. C. (1996). Problem-solving transfer. In Berliner, D. C. & Calfee, R. C. (eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 4762). New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
McLaren, B. M., Van Gog, T., Ganoe, C., Karabinos, M., & Yaron, D. (2016). The efficiency of worked examples compared to erroneous examples, tutored problem solving, and problem solving in computer-based learning environments. Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 8799. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.08.038Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. & Brooks, R. (2013). Gaze following and agency in human infancy. In Metcalfe, J. & Terrace, H. S. (eds.), Agency and joint attention (pp. 125138). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mousavi, S. Y., Low, R., & Sweller, J. (1995). Reducing cognitive load by mixing auditory and visual presentation modes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 319334.Google Scholar
Mwangi, W. & Sweller, J. (1998). Learning to solve compare word problems: The effect of example format and generating self-explanations. Cognition and Instruction, 16, 173199.Google Scholar
Newell, A. & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Ohlsson, S. & Rees, E. (1991). The function of conceptual understanding in the learning of arithmetic procedures. Cognition and Instruction, 8, 103179. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci0802_1Google Scholar
Ouwehand, K., Van Gog, T., & Paas, F. (2015). Designing effective video-based modeling examples using gaze and gesture cues. Educational Technology and Society, 18, 7888.Google Scholar
Paas, F. (1992). Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 429434. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.84.4.429Google Scholar
Paas, F. & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem solving skills: A cognitive load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 122133. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.86.1.122Google Scholar
Raaijmakers, S. F., Baars, M., Schaap, L., Paas, F., Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Van Gog, T. (2018). Training self-regulated learning skills with video modeling examples: Do task-selection skills transfer? Instructional Science, 64, 273290. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-017-9434-0Google Scholar
Reed, S. K., Willis, D., & Guarino, J. (1994). Selecting examples for solving word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 380388. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.86.3.380Google Scholar
Reeves, L. M. & Weisberg, R. W. (1993). On the concrete nature of human thinking: Content and context in analogical transfer. Educational Psychology, 13, 245258. https://doi.org/10.1080/0144341930130303Google Scholar
Reisslein, J., Atkinson, R., Seeling, P., & Reisslein, M. (2006). Encountering the expertise reversal effect with a computer-based environment on electrical circuit analysis. Learning and Instruction, 16, 92103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.02.008Google Scholar
Renkl, A. (1997). Learning from worked-out examples: A study on individual differences. Cognitive Science, 21, https://doi.org/1-29. 10.1207/s15516709cog2101_1Google Scholar
Renkl, A. (2014). Towards an instructionally-oriented theory of example-based learning. Cognitive Science, 38, 137. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12086Google Scholar
Renkl, A. (2017). Instruction based on examples. In Mayer, R. E. & Alexander, P. A. (eds.), Handbook of research on learning and instruction, 2nd edn (pp. 325348). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Renkl, A. & Atkinson, R. K. (2003). Structuring the transition from example study to problem solving in cognitive skills acquisition: A cognitive load perspective. Educational Psychologist, 38, 1522. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326985EP3801_3Google Scholar
Renkl, A., Atkinson, R. K., Maier, U. H., & Staley, R. (2002). From example study to problem solving: Smooth transitions help learning. Journal of Experimental Education, 70, 293315. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220970209599510Google Scholar
Richter, J., Scheiter, K., & Eitel, A. (2016). Signaling text-picture relations in multimedia learning: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 17, 1936. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2015.12.003Google Scholar
Rittle-Johnson, B. & Star, J. R. (2009). Compared with what? The effects of different comparisons on conceptual knowledge and procedural flexibility for equation solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 529544. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014224Google Scholar
Rittle-Johnson, B., Star, J. R., & Durkin, K. (2009). The importance of prior knowledge when comparing examples: Influences on conceptual and procedural knowledge of equation solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 836852. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016026Google Scholar
Roediger, H. L. & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). The power of testing memory: Basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 181210. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00012.xGoogle Scholar
Roscoe, R. & Chi, M. (2007). Understanding tutor learning: Knowledge-building and knowledge-telling in peer tutors’ explanations and questions. Review of Educational Research, 77, 534574. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654307309920Google Scholar
Ross, B. H. (1989). Distinguishing types of superficial similarities: Different effects on the access and use of earlier problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 456468.Google Scholar
Ross, B. H. & Kennedy, P. T. (1990). Generalizing from the use of earlier examples in problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 4245. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.16.1.42Google Scholar
Rourke, A. J. & Sweller, J. (2009). The worked-example effect using ill-defined problems: Learning to recognise designers’ styles. Learning and Instruction, 19, 185199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.03.006Google Scholar
Salden, R. J. C. M., Koedinger, K. R., Renkl, A., Aleven, V., & McLaren, B. M. (2010). Accounting for beneficial effects of worked examples in tutored problem solving. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 379392. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9143-6Google Scholar
Schunk, D. H. (1987). Peer models and children’s behavioral change. Review of Educational Research, 57, 149174. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543057002149Google Scholar
Schunk, D. H. & Hanson, A. R. (1985). Peer models: Influence on children’s self-efficacy and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 313322. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.77.3.313Google Scholar
Schunk, D. H. & Hanson, A. R. (1989). Influence of peer-model attributes on children’s beliefs and learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 431434. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.81.3.431Google Scholar
Schunk, D. H., Hanson, A. R., & Cox, P. D. (1987). Peer-model attributes and children’s achievement behaviors. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 5461. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.79.1.54Google Scholar
Schwaighofer, M., Bühner, M., & Fischer, F. (2016). Executive functions as moderators of the worked example effect: When shifting is more important than working memory capacity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108, 9821000. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000115Google Scholar
Schwonke, R., Renkl, A., Krieg, C. et al. (2009). The worked example effect: Not an artefact of lousy control conditions. Computers in Human Behavior, 25, 258266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.011Google Scholar
Spanjers, I. A. E., Van Gog, T., Wouters, P., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2012). Explaining the segmentation effect in learning from animations: The role of pausing and temporal cueing. Computers & Education, 59, 274280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2011.12.024Google Scholar
Spanjers, I. A. E., Wouters, P., Van Gog, T., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2011). An expertise reversal effect of segmentation in learning from animated worked-out examples. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 4652. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.05.011Google Scholar
Stark, R., Gruber, H., Renkl, A., & Mandl, H. (2000). Instruktionale effekte einer kombinierten lernmethode: Zahlt sich die kombination von lösungsbeispielen und problemlöseaufgaben aus? [Instructional effects of a combined learning method: Does the combination of worked-out examples and problem-solving tasks pay off?]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 14, 205217. https://doi.org/10.1024//1010-0652.14.4.206Google Scholar
Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem-solving: Effects on learning. Cognitive Science, 12, 257285. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1202_4Google Scholar
Sweller, J., Ayres, P. L., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory. New York: Springer. hhttps://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419–8126-4Google Scholar
Sweller, J. & Cooper, G. A. (1985). The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra. Cognition and Instruction, 2, 5989. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci0201_3Google Scholar
Sweller, J. & Levine, M. (1982). Effects of goal specificity on means-ends analysis and learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8, 463474. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.8.5.463Google Scholar
Sweller, J. & Sweller, S. (2006). Natural information processing systems. Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 434458. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470490600400135Google Scholar
Sweller, J., Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. G. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251296. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022193728205Google Scholar
Tarmizi, R. & Sweller, J. (1988). Guidance during mathematical problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 424436. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.80.4.424Google Scholar
Van Gerven, P. W. M., Paas, F., Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Schmidt, H. G. (2002). Cognitive load theory and aging: Effects of worked examples on training efficiency. Learning and Instruction, 12, 87105. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4752(01)00017-2Google Scholar
Van Gog, T. (2011). Effects of identical example-problem and problem-example pairs on learning. Computers & Education, 57, 17751779. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2011.03.019Google Scholar
Van Gog, T. (2015). Commentary: Learning from erroneous examples in medical education. Medical Education, 49, 142144. https://doi.org/10.1111/medu.12655Google Scholar
Van Gog, T., Jarodzka, H., Scheiter, K., Gerjets, P., & Paas, F. (2009). Attention guidance during example study via the model’s eye movements. Computers in Human Behavior, 25, 785791. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2009.02.007Google Scholar
Van Gog, T. & Kester, L. (2012). A test of the testing effect: Acquiring problem-solving skills from worked examples. Cognitive Science, 36, 15321541. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12002Google Scholar
Van Gog, T., Kester, L., Dirkx, K. et al. (2015). Testing after worked example study does not enhance delayed problem-solving performance compared to restudy. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 265289. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9297-3Google Scholar
Van Gog, T., Kester, L., & Paas, F. (2011). Effects of worked examples, example-problem, and problem-example pairs on novices’ learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 212218. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.10.004Google Scholar
Van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2004). Process-oriented worked examples: Improving transfer performance through enhanced understanding. Instructional Science, 32, 8398. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:TRUC.0000021810.70784.b0Google Scholar
Van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2006). Effects of process-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer performance. Learning and Instruction, 16, 154164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.02.003Google Scholar
Van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2008). Effects of studying sequences of process-oriented and product-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer efficiency. Learning and Instruction, 18, 211222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.03.003Google Scholar
Van Gog, T. & Rummel, N. (2010). Example-based learning: Integrating cognitive and social-cognitive research perspectives. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 155174. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9134-7Google Scholar
Van Gog, T., Verveer, I., & Verveer, L. (2014). Learning from video modeling examples: Effects of seeing the human model’s face. Computers & Education, 72, 323327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2013.12.004Google Scholar
Van Loon-Hillen, N. H., Van Gog, T., & Brand-Gruwel, S. (2012). Effects of worked examples in a primary school mathematics curriculum. Interactive Learning Environments, 20, 8999. https://doi.org/10.1080/10494821003755510Google Scholar
Van Marlen, T., Van Wermeskerken, M. M., Jarodzka, H., & Van Gog, T. (2016). Showing a model’s eye movements in examples does not improve learning of problem-solving tasks. Computers in Human Behavior, 65, 448459. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.041Google Scholar
Van Marlen, T., Van Wermeskerken, M. M., Jarodzka, H., & Van Gog, T. (2018). Effectiveness of Eye Movement Modeling Examples in problem solving: The role of verbal ambiguity and prior knowledge. Learning and Instruction, 58, 274–283.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.07.005Google Scholar
Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., Schuurman, J. G., De Croock, M. B. M., & Paas, F. (2002). Redirecting learners’ attention during training: Effects on cognitive load, transfer test performance and training. Learning and Instruction, 38, 1139. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4752(01)00020–2Google Scholar
Van Wermeskerken, M., Grimmius, B., & Van Gog, T. (2018). Attention to the model’s face when learning from video modeling examples in adolescents with and without autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 34, 3241. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcal.12211Google Scholar
Van Wermeskerken, M., Ravensbergen, S. J., & Van Gog, T. (2018). Effects of instructor presence in video modeling examples on attention and learning. Computers in Human Behavior, 89, 430438. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.11.038Google Scholar
Van Wermeskerken, M. & Van Gog, T. (2017). Seeing the instructor’s face and gaze in demonstration video examples affects attention allocation but not learning. Computers and Education, 113, 98107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2017.05.013Google Scholar
Wang, F., Li, W., Mayer, R. E., & Liu, H. (2017). Animated pedagogical agents as aids in multimedia learning: Effects on eye-fixations during learning and learning outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(2), 250268. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000221Google Scholar
Wittwer, J. & Renkl, A. (2010). How effective are instructional explanations in example-based learning? A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 393409. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9136-5Google Scholar
Wright, D. L., Li, Y., & Coady, W. (1997). Cognitive processes related to contextual interference and observational learning: A replication of Blandin, Proteau and Alain (1994). Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 68, 106109. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.1997.10608872Google Scholar
Zhu, X. & Simon, H. A. (1987). Learning mathematics from examples and by doing. Cognition and Instruction, 4, 137166. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci0403_1Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×