Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-gtxcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T07:30:42.433Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Harnessing Our Hands to Teach Mathematics

How Gesture Can Be Used as a Teaching Tool in the Classroom

from Part II - Science and Math

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2019

John Dunlosky
Affiliation:
Kent State University, Ohio
Katherine A. Rawson
Affiliation:
Kent State University, Ohio
Get access

Summary

Gestures are movements of the hands that can express ideas and often accompany speech. In this chapter, we review evidence that gesture is an excellent educational tool, particularly when teaching mathematics. We focus on naturalistic studies that establish gesture's relevance to classroom teaching, and laboratory studies that show that gestures promote learning when produced by students or teachers. We conclude this chapter with recommendations for best educational practices involving gesture and future directions for research.
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adelman, C. (2006). The toolbox revisited: Paths to degree completion from high school through college. Washington, DC: US Department of Education.Google Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Flevares, L. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1997). Assessing knowledge conveyed in gesture: Do teachers have the upper hand? Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 183193. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022–0663.89.1.183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Heath, D. C., & Myers, H. J. (2011). Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: Some gestures are meant to be seen. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 169188. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2752Google Scholar
Alibali, M. W., & Nathan, M. J. (2012). Embodiment in mathematics teaching and learning: Evidence from learners’ and teachers’ gestures. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21, 247286. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508406.2011.611446Google Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Nathan, M. J., Church, R. B., Wolfgram, M. S., Kim, S., & Knuth, E. J. (2013). Teachers’ gestures and speech in mathematics lessons: Forging common ground by resolving trouble spots. Mathematics Education, 45, 425440. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-012–0476-0Google Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Nathan, M. J., & Fujimori, Y. (2011). Gesture in the mathematics classroom: What’s the point? In Stein, N. & Raudenbush, S. (eds.), Developmental cognitive science goes to school (pp. 219234). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Nathan, M. J., Wolfgram, M. S., Church, R. B., Jacobs, S. A., Maritinex, C. J., & Knuth, E. J. (2014). How teachers link ideas in mathematics instruction using speech and gesture: A corpus analysis. Cognition and Instruction, 32, 65100. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370008.2013.858161CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Young, A. G., Crooks, N. M., Yeo, A., Wolfgram, M. S., Ledesma, I. M., … Knuth, E. J. (2013). Students learn more when their teacher has learned to gesture effectively. Gesture, 13, 210233. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.13.2.05aliGoogle Scholar
Allensworth, E. M., & Easton, J. Q. (2005). The on-track indicator as a predictor of high school graduation. Chicago: Consortium on Chicago school research. https://consortium.uchicago.edu/sites/default/files/publications/p78.pdfGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577660. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002149Google Scholar
Broaders, S. C., Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2007). Making children gesture brings out implicit knowledge and leads to learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 136, 539550. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096–3445.136.4.539CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, A. J., & James, K. H. (2013). Active learning of novel sound-producing objects: Motor reactivation and enhancement of visuo-motor connectivity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 203218. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00284CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, A. J., James, T. W., & James, K. H. (2011). Enhanced multisensory integration and motor reactivation after active motor learning of audiovisual associations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 35153528. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00015Google Scholar
Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grezes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 12431249. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhi007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casile, A., & Giese, M. A. (2006). Nonvisual motor training influences biological motion perception. Current Biology, 16, 6974. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2005.10.071Google Scholar
Chao, L. L., & Martin, A. (2000). Representation of manipulable man-made objects in the dorsal stream. Neuroimage, 12, 478484. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0635Google Scholar
Church, R. B., Ayman-Nolley, S., & Mahootian, S. (2004). The role of gesture in bilingual education: Does gesture enhance learning? International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 7, 303319. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050408667815Google Scholar
Church, R. B., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1986). The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index on transitional knowledge. Cognition, 23, 4371. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010–0277(86)90053–3Google Scholar
Congdon, E. L., Novack, M. A., Brooks, N., Hemani-Lopez, N., O’Keefe, L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2017). Better together: Simultaneous presentation of speech and gesture in math instruction supports generalization and retention. Learning and Instruction, 50, 6574. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.03.005Google Scholar
Cook, S. W., Duffy, R. G., & Fenn, K. M. (2013). Consolidation and transfer of learning after observing hand gesture. Child Development, 84, 1863–1871. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12097CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, S. W., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006). The role of gesture in learning: Do children use their hands to change their minds? Journal of Cognition and Development, 7, 211232. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327647jcd0702_4Google Scholar
Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Gesturing makes learning last. Cognition, 106, 10471058. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.010Google Scholar
Cooperrider, K., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2017). When gesture becomes analogy. Topics in Cognitive Science, 9, 719737. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12276CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crais, E. R., Watson, L. R., & Baranek, G. T. (2009). Use of gesture development in profiling children’s prelinguistic communication skills. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 18, 95108. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058–0360(2008/07–0041(2008/07–0041Google Scholar
DeCaro, M. S., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2012). Exploring mathematics problems prepares children to learn from instruction. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113, 552568. https://doi.org/10.1016/jecp.2012.06.009Google Scholar
Department of Education. (1997). Mathematics equals opportunity. White Paper prepared for US Secretary of Education R. W. Riley.Google Scholar
Department of Education (1999). Do gatekeeper courses expand education options?. National Center for Education Statistics.Google Scholar
Flevares, L. M., & Perry, M. (2001). How many do you see? The use of nonspoken representations in first-grade mathematics lessons. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 330345. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022–0663.93.2.330Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Cook, S. W., & Mitchell, Z. (2009). Gestures gives children new ideas about math. Psychological Science, 20, 267271. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–9280.2009.02297.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., Levine, S. C., Zinchenko, E., Yip, T. K., Hemani, N., & Factor, L. (2012). Doing gesture promotes learning a mental transformation task better than seeing gesture. Developmental Science, 15, 876884. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–7687.2012.01185.xGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Singer, M. A. (2003). From children’s hands to adults’ ears: Gesture’s role in the learning process. Developmental Psychology, 39, 509520. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012–1649.39.3.509Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Wein, D., & Chang, C. (1992). Assessing knowledge through gesture: Using children’s hands to read their minds. Cognition and Instruction, 9, 201219. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci0903_2Google Scholar
Goldstone, R. L., & Son, J. Y. (2005). The transfer of scientific principles using concrete and idealized simulations. The Journal of Learning Sciences, 14, 69110. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327809jls1401_4Google Scholar
Hansen, M. (2014). Characteristics of schools successful in STEM: Evidence from two states’ longitudinal data. The Journal of Educational Research, 107, 374391. https://doi.org/10.1090/00220671.2013.823364Google Scholar
Harman, K. L., Humphrey, G. K., & Goodale, M. A. (1999). Active manual control of object views facilitates visual recognition. Current Biology, 9, 13151318. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0960-9822(00)80053–6Google Scholar
Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2008). Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 495514. https://doi.org/10.3758/pbr.15.3.495Google Scholar
Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2010). Language, gesture, action! A test of the Gesture as Simulated Action framework. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 245257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.04.003Google Scholar
James, K. H. (2010). Sensori-motor experience leads to changes in visual processing in the developing brain. Developmental Science, 13, 279288. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–7687.2009.00883.xGoogle Scholar
James, K. H., & Atwood, T. P. (2009). The role of sensorimotor learning in the perception of letter-like forms: Tracking the causes of neural specialization for letters. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26, 91110. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290802425914Google Scholar
James, K. H., & Gauthier, I. (2006). Letter processing automatically recruits a sensory-motor brain network. Neuropsychologia, 44, 29372949. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.026Google Scholar
James, K. H., & Maouene, J. (2009). Auditory verb perception recruits motor developing brain: An fMRI investigation. Developmental Psychology, 12, F26–34. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–7687.2009.00919.xGoogle Scholar
James, K. H., & Swain, S. N. (2011). Only self-generated actions create sensori-motor systems in the developing brain. Developmental Psychology, 14, 16. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–7687.2010.01011.xGoogle Scholar
Kelly, S. D., Healy, M., Ozyurek, A., & Holler, J. (2014). The processing of speech, gesture, and action during language comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014–0681-7Google Scholar
Kelly, S. D., Singer, M., Hicks, J., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2002). A helping hand in assessing children’s knowledge: Instructing adults to attend to gesture. Cognition and Instruction, 20, 126. https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532690XCI2001_1Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (1980). Gesticulation and speech: Two aspects of the process of utterance. In Key, M. R. (ed.), The Relationship of Verbal and Nonverbal Communication (pp. 207227). The Hague: Mouton and Co.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible Action as Utterance. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, L. M., & Tipps, S. (1994). Guiding children’s learning of mathematics, 7th edition. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Kontra, C., Lyons, D. J., Fischer, C., & Beilock, S. L. (2015). Physical experience enhances science learning. Psychological Science, 26, 737749. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615569355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koumoutsakis, T., Church, R. B., Alibali, M. W., Singer, M., & Ayman-Nolley, S. (2016). Gesture in instruction: Evidence from live and video lessons. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 40, 301315. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-016–0234-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lillard, A. (2005). Montessori: The science behind the genius. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lillard, A., & Else-Quest, N. (2006). Evaluating Montessori method. Science, 313, 1893–1894. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1132362Google Scholar
Longcamp, M., Anton, J.-L., Roth, M., & Velay, J.-L. (2003). Visual presentation of single letters activates a premotor area involved in writing. Neuroimage, 19, 14921500. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00088–0Google Scholar
Longcamp, M., Tanskanen, T., & Hari, R. (2006). The imprint of action: Motor cortex involvement in visual perception of handwritten letters. Neuroimage, 33, 681688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.042Google Scholar
Macedonia, M., Muller, K., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). The impact of iconic gestures on foreign language word learning and its neural substrate. Human Brain Mapping, 32, 982998. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21084CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEldoon, K. L., Durkin, K. L., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2013). Is self-explanation worth the time? A comparison to additional practice. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 615632. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044–8279.2012.02083.xGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal about Thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mix, K. S. (2010). Spatial tools for mathematical thought. In Mix, K. S., Smith, L. B., & Gasser, M. (eds.), Space and Language (pp. 4166). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Montessori, M. (1995). The Absorbent Mind. New York: Henry Holt and Company.Google Scholar
Novack, M., Congdon, E., Hemani-Lopez, N., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). From action to abstraction: Using the hands to learn math. Psychological Science, 25, 903910. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613518351Google Scholar
Perry, M., Church, R. B., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1988). Transitional knowledge in the acquisition of concepts. Cognitive Development, 3, 359400. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885–2014(88)90021–4(88)90021–4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The Origins of Intelligence in Children. New York: International University Press.Google Scholar
Pulvermüller, F. (2001). Brain reflections of words and their meaning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 517524. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01803–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richland, L. E. (2015). Linking gestures: Cross-cultural variation during instructional analogies. Cognition and Instruction, 33, 295321. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370008.2015.1091459Google Scholar
Richland, L. E., Zur, O., & Holyoak, K. J. (2007). Cognitive supports for analogies in the mathematics classroom. Science, 316, 11281129. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1142103Google Scholar
Rohlfing, K. J., Longo, M. R., & Bertenthal, B. I. (2012). Dynamic pointing triggers shifts of visual attention in young infants. Developmental Science, 15, 426435. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–7687.2012.01139.xGoogle Scholar
Singer, M. A., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Children learn when their teacher’s gestures and speech differ. Psychological Science, 16, 8589. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956–7976.2005.00786.xGoogle Scholar
Tooke, D. J., Hyatt, B., Leigh, M., Snyder, B., & Borda, T. (1992). Why aren’t manipulatives used in every middle school mathematics classroom? Middle School Journal, 24, 6162.Google Scholar
Valenzeno, L., Alibali, M. W., & Klatzky, R. (2003). Teachers’ gestures facilitate students’ learning: A lesson in symmetry. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 187204. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0361-476x(02)00007–3Google Scholar
Wakefield, E. M., Hall, C., James, K. H., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2018b). Gesture for generalization: Gesture facilitates flexible learning of words for actions on objects. Developmental Science. Doi: 10.1111/desc.12656Google Scholar
Wakefield, E. M., & James, K. H. (2015). Effects of learning with gesture on children’s understanding of a new language concept. Developmental Psychology, 5, 11051114. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039471Google Scholar
Wakefield, E. M., Novack, M., Congdon, E., Franconeri, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2018a). Gesture helps learners learn, but not merely by guiding their visual attention. Developmental Science, https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12664Google Scholar
Wakefield, E. M., Novack, M., Congdon, E., Goldin-Meadow, S., & James, K. H. (2014). Understanding the neural effects of learning with gesture: Does gesture help learners because it is grounded in action? Talk presented at the International Society of Gesture Studies meeting, July, San Diego, California.Google Scholar
Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2012). Embodied attention and word learning by toddlers. Cognition, 125, 244262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.016Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×