Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
×
Home
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 19
  • Cited by
    This chapter has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by CrossRef.

    Arrabito, G. Robert Ho, Geoffrey Aghaei, Behzad Burns, Catherine and Hou, Ming 2015. Sustained Attention in Auditory and Visual Monitoring Tasks. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Vol. 57, Issue. 8, p. 1403.

    Greenlee, Eric T. Funke, Gregory J. Warm, Joel S. Finomore, Victor S. Patterson, Robert E. Barnes, Laura E. Funke, Matthew E. and Vidulich, Michael A. 2015. Effects of Stereoscopic Depth on Vigilance Performance and Cerebral Hemodynamics. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Vol. 57, Issue. 6, p. 1063.

    Greenlee, Eric T. Warm, Joel S. Funke, Gregory J. Patterson, Robert E. Strang, Adam J. Finomore, Victor S. Barnes, Laura E. and Funke, Matthew E. 2016. Event-related cerebral hemodynamics in 2-D and 3-D Visual Vigilance Tasks. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, Vol. 60, Issue. 1, p. 1558.

    Greenlee, Eric T. Funke, Gregory J. Warm, Joel S. Sawyer, Ben D. Finomore, Victor S. Mancuso, Vince F. Funke, Matthew E. and Matthews, Gerald 2016. Advances in Human Factors in Cybersecurity. Vol. 501, Issue. , p. 153.

    Jipp, Meike 2016. Reaction Times to Consecutive Automation Failures. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Vol. 58, Issue. 8, p. 1248.

    Cummings, Mary L. Gao, Fei and Thornburg, Kris M. 2016. Boredom in the Workplace. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Vol. 58, Issue. 2, p. 279.

    Finomore, Victor S. McClernon, Christopher K. Amick, Jason R. Pee, Derrick Funke, Gregory J. and Warm, Joel S. 2016. Short Vigilance Tasks are Hard Work Even If Time Flies. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, Vol. 60, Issue. 1, p. 1367.

    Funke, Gregory J. Warm, Joel S. Baldwin, Carryl L. Garcia, Andre Funke, Matthew E. Dillard, Michael B. Finomore, Victor S. Matthews, Gerald and Greenlee, Eric T. 2016. The Independence and Interdependence of Coacting Observers in Regard to Performance Efficiency, Workload, and Stress in a Vigilance Task. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Vol. 58, Issue. 6, p. 915.

    Meuter, Renata F. I. and Lacherez, Philippe F. 2016. When and Why Threats Go Undetected. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Vol. 58, Issue. 2, p. 218.

    Hancock, Peter A. Baldwin, Carryl L. Warm, Joel S. and Szalma, James L. 2017. Between Two Worlds. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Vol. 59, Issue. 1, p. 28.

    Rupp, Michael A. Sweetman, Richard Sosa, Alejandra E. Smither, Janan A. and McConnell, Daniel S. 2017. Searching for Affective and Cognitive Restoration: Examining the Restorative Effects of Casual Video Game Play. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Vol. 59, Issue. 7, p. 1096.

    Helton, William S. and Russell, Paul N. 2017. Rest Is Still Best. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Vol. 59, Issue. 1, p. 91.

    Laurie-Rose, Cynthia Curtindale, Lori M. and Frey, Meredith 2017. Measuring Sustained Attention and Perceived Workload. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Vol. 59, Issue. 1, p. 76.

    Wixted, Fiona and O’ Sullivan, Leonard 2017. The moderating role of end-tidal CO 2 on upper trapezius muscle activity in response to sustained attention. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, Vol. 61, Issue. , p. 1.

    Funke, Matthew E. Warm, Joel S. Matthews, Gerald Funke, Gregory J. Chiu, Peter Y. Shaw, Tyler H. and Greenlee, Eric T. 2017. The Neuroergonomics of Vigilance. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Vol. 59, Issue. 1, p. 62.

    Wohleber, Ryan W. Matthews, Gerald Lin, Jinchao Szalma, James L. Calhoun, Gloria L. Funke, Gregory J. Chiu, C.-Y. Peter and Ruff, Heath A. 2018. Vigilance and Automation Dependence in Operation of Multiple Unmanned Aerial Systems (UAS): A Simulation Study. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, p. 001872081879946.

    Greenlee, Eric T. DeLucia, Patricia R. and Newton, David C. 2018. Driver Vigilance in Automated Vehicles: Effects of Demands on Hazard Detection Performance. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, p. 001872081880209.

    Greenlee, Eric T. DeLucia, Patricia R. and Newton, David C. 2018. Driver Vigilance in Automated Vehicles: Hazard Detection Failures Are a Matter of Time. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Vol. 60, Issue. 4, p. 465.

    Dillard, Michael B. Warm, Joel S. Funke, Gregory J. Nelson, W. Todd Finomore, Victor S. McClernon, Christopher K. Eggemeier, F. Thomas Tripp, Lloyd D. and Funke, Matthew E. 2018. Vigilance Tasks: Unpleasant, Mentally Demanding, and Stressful Even When Time Flies. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, p. 001872081879601.

    ×
  • Online publication date: July 2015

13 - Vigilance: A Perceptual Challenge

from Part II - Attention and Perceptual Processes
Recommend this book

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this book to your organisation's collection.

The Cambridge Handbook of Applied Perception Research
  • Online ISBN: 9780511973017
  • Book DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511973017
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to *
×

References

Aaslid, R. (1986). Transcranial Doppler examination techniques. In R. Aaslid, (Ed.), Transcranial Doppler sonography (pp. 39–59). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Adams, J. A. and Boulter, L. R. (1964). Spatial and temporal uncertainty as determinants of vigilance performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 127–131.
Adams, J. A. and Humes, J. M. (1963). Monitoring of complex visual displays. IV. Training for vigilance. Human Factors, 5, 147–153.
Aiken, E. G. and Lau, A. W. (1967). Response prompting and response confirmation: A review of recent literature. Psychological Bulletin, 68, 330–341.
Alikonis, C. R., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., Dember, W. N., Hitchcock, E. M. and Kellaris, J. J. (2002). Vigilance, workload, and boredom: Two competing models. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 46, 1531–1535.
Alluisi, E. A. (1966). Attention and vigilance as mechanisms of response. In E. A. Bilodeau (Ed.), Acquisition of skill (pp. 201–213). New York: Academic Press.
Alluisi, E. A., Coates, G. D. and Morgan, B. B., Jr. (1977). Effects of temporal stressors on vigilance and information processing. In R. R. Mackie (Ed.), Vigilance: Theory, operational performance and physiological correlates (pp. 361–422). New York: Plenum.
Alves, E. E. and Kelsey, C. M. (2010). Combating vigilance decrement in a single-operator radar platform. Ergonomics in Design: The Quarterly of Human Factors Applications, 18, 6–9.
Ariga, A. and Lleras, A. (2011). Brief and rare mental “breaks” keep you focused: Deactivation and reactivation of task goals preempt vigilance decrements. Cognition, 118, 439–443.
Arrabito, G. R., Abel, S. M. and Lam, K. (2007). Methods for mitigating the vigilance decrement in an auditory sonar monitoring task: A research synthesis. Canadian Acoustics, 35, 15–24.
Asterita, C. R. (1985). The physiology of stress. New York: Human Science Press.
Bakan, P. (1959). Extraversion-introversion and improvement in an auditory vigilance task. British Journal of Psychology, 50, 325–332.
Baker, C. H. (1962). Man and radar displays. New York: Macmillan.
Baker, C. H. (1963). Further toward a theory of vigilance. In D. N. Buckner and J. J. McGrath (Eds.), Vigilance: A symposium (pp. 127–170). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Ballard, J. C. (1996). Computerized assessment of sustained attention: A review of factors affecting vigilance performance. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18, 843–863.
Becker, A. B., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N. and Hancock, P. A. (1995). Effects of jet engine noise and performance feedback on perceived workload in a monitoring task. International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 5, 49–62.
Bell, C. R., Symington, L. E. and Bevan, W. (1974). Watchkeeping performance as a function of certain properties of the viewing situation. Human Factors, 16, 65–69.
Bennett, K. B., and Flach, J. M. (2011). Display and interface design: Subtle science, exact art. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press
Berch, D. B. and Kanter, D. R. (1984). Individual differences. In J. S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 143–178). Chichester: Wiley.
Bridger, R. S. (2009). Introduction to Ergonomics (3rd ed.): Boca Raton, FL.: CRC Press
Broadbent, D. E. (1971). Decision and stress. New York: Academic Press.
Broadbent, D. E., Cooper, P. F., Fitzgerald, P. and Parkes, K. R. (1982). The Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21, 1–16.
Buck, L. (1966). Reaction time as a measure of perceptual vigilance. Psychological Bulletin, 65, 291–304.
Buckner, D. N. and McGrath, J. J. (1963a). A comparison of performance on single and dual sensory mode vigilance tasks. In D. N. Buckner and J. J. McGrath (Eds.), Vigilance: A symposium (pp. 53–71). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Buckner, D. N. and McGrath, J. J. (Eds.), (1963b). Vigilance: A symposium. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Cabeza, R. and Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 1–47.
Caggiano, D. M. and Parasuraman, R. (2004). The role of memory representation in the vigilance decrement. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 932–937.
Carriero, N. (1977). Physiological correlates of performance in a long duration repetitive task. In R. R. Mackie (Ed.), Vigilance, theory, operational performance, and physiological correlates (pp. 307–330). New York: Plenum.
Carter, L., Russell, P.N., and Helton, W.S. (2013). Target predicatability, sustained attention, and response inhibition. Brain and Cognition, 82, 35–42.
Colquhoun, W. P. (1987). A shipboard study of a four crew rotating watchkeeping system. Ergonomics, 30, 1341–1352.
Colquhoun, W. P. and Baddeley, A. D. (1967). Influence of signal probability during pretraining on vigilance decrement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73, 153–155.
Corcoran, D. W. J., Mullin, J., Rainey, M. T. and Frith, G. (1977). The effects of raised signal and noise amplitude during the course of vigilance tasks. In R. R. Mackie (Ed.), Vigilance: Theory, operational performance and physiological correlates (pp. 645–664). New York: Plenum.
Coren, S., Ward, L. M. and Enns, J. T. (2004). Sensation and perception (6th ed.). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt-Brace.
Craig, A. (1984). Human engineering: The control of vigilance. In J. S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 247–291). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Craig, A. (1985). Vigilance: Theories and laboratory studies. In: S. Folkard and T. H. Monk (Eds.), Hours of work (pp. 107–121). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Craig, A. (1987). Signal detection theory and probability matching apply to vigilance. Human Factors, 29, 645–652.
Craig, A. (1991). Vigilance and monitoring for multiple signals. In D. L. Damos (Ed.), Multiple task performance (pp. 153–171). London: Taylor and Francis.
Craig, A., Colquhoun, W. P. and Corcoran, D. W. J. (1976). Combining evidence presented simultaneously to the eye and the ear: A comparison of some predictive models. Perception & Psychophysics, 19, 473–484.
Davies, D. R. and Parasuraman, R. (1982). The psychology of vigilance. London: Academic Press.
Davies, D. R. and Tune, G. S. (1969). Human vigilance performance. New York: American Elsevier
Dember, W. N. and Warm, J. S. (1979). Psychology of perception (2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Desmond, P. A., Matthews, G. and Bush, J. (2001). Sustained visual attention during simultaneous and successive vigilance tasks. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 45, 1386–1389.
Detre, J. A. and Wang, J. (2002). Technical aspects and utility of fMRI using BOLD and ASL. Clinincal Neurophysiology, 113, 621–634.
Dittmar, M. L., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N. and Ricks, D. F. (1993). Sex differences in vigilance performance and perceived workload. Journal of General Psychology, 120, 309–322.
Doll, T. J. and Hanna, T. E. (1989). Enhanced detection with bimodal sonar displays. Human Factors, 31, 539–550.
Donald, F. M. (2008). The classification of vigilance tasks in the real world. Ergonomics, 51, 1643–1655.
Duschek, S. and Schandry, R. (2003). Functional transcranial Doppler sonography as a tool in psychophysiological research. Psychophysiology, 40, 436–454.
Finomore, V.S, Matthews, G., Shaw, T.H., and Warm, J.S. (2009). Predicting vigilance: A fresh look at an old problem. Ergonomics, 52, 791–808.
Finomore, V. S, Shaw, T. H., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G. and Boles, D. B. (2013). Viewing the workload of vigilance through the lenses of the NASA-TLX and the MRQ. Human Factors, 55, 1044–1063.
Fisk, A. D. and Schneider, W. (1981) Control and automatic processing during tasks requiring sustained attention: A new approach to vigilance. Human Factors, 23, 737–750.
Frankmann, J. P. and Adams, J. A. (1962). Theories of vigilance. Psychological Bulletin, 59, 257–272.
Funke, M. E., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., Finomore, V., Vidulich, M. A., Knott, B., Helton, W. S., Shaw, T. H. and Parasuraman, R. (2011). Static and dynamic discriminations in vigilance: Effects on cerebral hemodynamics and workload. In T. Marek, W. Karwowski and V. Rice (Eds.), Advances in understanding human performance: Neuroergonomics, human factors design, and special populations (pp. 80–90). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Funke, M. E., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., Funke, G. J., Chiu, P. and Riley, M. (August, 2012). Neuroergonomic dynamics associated with spatial uncertainty during vigilance performance. Paper Presented at the Meeting of the American Psychological Association, Orlando, FL.
Galinsky, T. L., Rosa, R. R., Warm, J. S. and Dember, W. N. (1993). Psychophysical determinants of stress in sustained attention. Human Factors, 35, 603–614.
Galinsky, T. L., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N., Weiler, E. M. and Scerbo, M. (1990). Sensory alternation and vigilance performance: The role of pathway inhibition. Human Factors, 32, 717–728.
Giambra, L. M. (1995). A laboratory method for investigating influences on switching attention to task-unrelated imagery and thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 4, 1–21.
Gilbert, D. G., Dibb, W. D., Platii, L. C. and Hiyane, S. G. (2000). Effects of nicotine and caffeine separately and in combination on EEG topography, mood, heart rate, cortisol, and vigilance. Psychophysiology, 37, 583–595.
Grier, R. A., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N., Matthews, G., Galinsky, T. L., Szalma, J. L. and Parasuraman, R. (2003). The vigilance decrement reflects limitations in effortful attention, not mindlessness. Human Factors, 45, 349–359.
Griffin, J. A., Dember, W. N. and Warm, J. S. (1986). Effects of depression on expectancy in sustained attention. Motivation and Emotion, 10, 195–205.
Grubb, P. L, Miller, L. C., Nelson, W. T., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N. and Davies, D. R. (1994). Cognitive failure and perceived workload in vigilance performance. In M. Mouloua and R. Parasuraman (Eds.), Human performance in automated systems: Current research and trends (pp. 115–121). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Grubb, P. L., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N. and Berch, D. B. (1995). Effects of multiple-signal discrimination on vigilance performance and perceived workload. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 39, 1360–1364.
Gunn, D. V., Warm, J. S., Nelson, W. T., Bolia, R. S., Schumsky, D. A. and Corcoran, K. J. (2005). Target acquisition with UAVs: Vigilance displays and advanced cueing interfaces. Human Factors, 47, 488–497.
Gunn, W. J. and Loeb, M. (1967). Correlation of performance in detecting visual and auditory signals. American Journal of Psychology, 80, 236–242.
Gunzelman, G., Gross, J. B., Gluck, K. A. and Dinges, D. F. (2009). Sleep deprivation and sustained attention performance: Integrating mathematical and cognitive modeling. Cognitive Science, 33, 880–910.
Guralnick, M. J. (1973). The effects of event rate and signal difficulty on observing responses and detection measures in vigilance. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 99, 261–265.
Hancock, P. A. (1984). Environmental stressors. In J. S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 103–142). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Hancock, P. A. (2013). In search of vigilance: The problem of iatrogenically created psychological phenomenon. American Psychologist, 68, 97–109.
Hancock, P. A. and Hart, G. (2002). Defeating terrorism: What can human factors/ergonomics offer? Ergonomics and Design, 10, 6–16.
Hancock, P. A. and Warm, J. S. (1989). A dynamic model of stress and sustained attention. Human Factors, 31, 519–537.
Hart, S. G. and Staveland, L. E. (1988). Development of NASA-TLX (task load index): Results of empirical and theoretical research. In P. A. Hancock and N. Meshkati (Eds.), Human mental workload. (pp. 139–183). Oxford: North-Holland.
Hatfield, J. L. and Loeb, M. (1968). Sense mode and coupling in a vigilance task. Perception & Psychophysics, 4, 29–36.
Hawley, J. K. (2006). Patriot fratricides: The human dimension lessons of operation Iraqi Freedom. Field Artillery, 18–19.
Head, H. (1923). The conception of nervous and mental energy, II: Vigilance: A physiological state of the nervous system. British Journal of Psychology, 14, 126–147.
Helton, W. S., Dember, W. N., Warm, J. S. and Matthews, G. (2000). Optimism, pessimism, and false failure feedback: Effects on vigilance performance. Current Psychology, 18, 311–325.
Helton, W. S., Hayrynen, L. and Schaeffer, D. (2009). Sustained attention to local and global target features is different: Performance and tympanic membrane temperature. Brain and Cognition, 71, 9–13.
Helton, W. S., Head, J. and Russell, P. N. (2011). Reliable and unreliable warning cues in the Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART). Experimental Brain Research, 209, 401–407.
Helton, W. S., Hollander, T. D., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., Dember, W. N., Wallart, M. et al. (2005). Signal regularity and the mindlessness model of vigilance. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 249–261.
Helton, W. S., Hollander, T. D., Warm, J. S., Tripp, L. D., Parsons, K., Matthews, G. et al. (2007). The abbreviated vigilance task and cerebral hemodynamics. Journal of Clinincal and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29, 545–552.
Helton, W. S., Matthews, G. and Warm, J. S. (2009). Stress state mediation between environmental variables and performance: The case of noise and vigilance. Acta Psychologica, 130, 204–213.
Helton, W. S. and Russell, P. N. (2011). The effects of arousing negative and neutral picture stimuli on target detection in a vigilance task. Human Factors, 53, 132–141.
Helton, W. S. and Russell, P. N. (2012). Brief mental breaks and content-free cues may not keep you focused. Experimental Brain Research, 219(1), 37–46.
Helton, W. S. and Warm, J. S. (2008). Signal salience and the mindlessness theory of vigilance. Acta Psychologica, 129, 18–25.
Helton, W. S., Weil, L., Middlemiss, A. and Sawers, A. (2010). Global interference and spatial uncertainty in the sustained attention to response task (SART). Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 77–85.
Hitchcock, E. M., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., Dember, W. N., Shear, P. K., Tripp, L. D. et al. (2003). Automation cueing modulates cerebral blood flow and vigilance in a simulated air traffic control task. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 4, 89–112.
Hoddes, E., Zarcone, V., Smythe, H., Phillips, R. and Dement, W. C. (1973). Quantification of sleepiness: A new approach. Psychophysiology, 10, 431–436.
Hollander, T. D., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., Shockley, K., Dember, W. N., Weiler, E. M., et al. (2004). Feature presence/absence modifies the event rate effect and cerebral hemovelocity in vigilance performance. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 48, 1943–1947.
Hovanitz, C. A., Chin, K. and Warm, J. S. (1989). Complexities in life-stress dysfunction relationships: A case in point – tension headache. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 12, 55–75.
Howell, W. C. (1993). Engineering psychology in a changing world. Annual Review of Psychology, 5, 179–190.
Howell, W. C. and Goldstein, I. L. (1971). Engineering psychology: Current perspectives in research. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Howell, W. C., Johnston, W. A. and Goldstein, I. L. (1966). Complex monitoring and its relation to the classical problem of vigilance. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 1, 129–150.
James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt.
Jerison, H. J. (1959). Experiments on vigilance: The empirical model for human vigilance (WADC Report No. 58–526), Aero-Medical Laboratory, Wright Air Development Center, Air Research and Development Command, United States Air Force, WPAFB, OH.
Jerison, H. J. (1967). Signal detection theory in the analysis of human vigilance. Human Factors, 9, 285–288.
Jerison, H. J. (1977). Vigilance: Biology, psychology, theory, and practice (keynote address). In R. R. Mackie (Ed.), Vigilance: Theory, operational performance and physiological correlates (pp. 27–40). New York: Plenum.
Jerison, H. J. and Pickett, R. M. (1963). Vigilance: A review and re-evaluation. Human Factors, 5, 211–238.
Jerison, H. J. and Pickett, R. M. (1964). Vigilance: The importance of the elicited observing rate. Science, 143, 970–971.
Jerison, H. J. Pickett, R. M. and Stenson, H. H. (1965). The elicited observing rate and decision processes in vigilance. Human Factors, 7, 107–128.
Johnson, A. and Proctor, R. W. (2004). Attention: Theory and practice. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Kantowitz, B. H. (1985). Stages and channels in human information processing: A limited analysis of theory and methodology. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 29, 135–174.
Kibler, A. W. (1965). The relevance of vigilance research to aerospace monitoring tasks. Human Factors, 7, 93–99.
Kimchi, R. (1992). Primacy of wholistic processing and global/local paradigm: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 24–38.
Kluger, A. N. and DeNisi, A. (1996). The effects of feedback interventions on performance: A historical review, meta-analysis, and a preliminary feedback intervention theory. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 2, 254–284.
Koelega, H. S. (1993). Stimulant drugs and vigilance performance: A review. Psychopharmacology, 111, 1–16.
Koelega, H. S. (1996). Sustained attention. In O. Neumann and A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action, Vol. 3: Attention (pp. 277–231). London: Academic Press.
Krulewitz, J. E. and Warm, J. S. (1977). The event rate context in vigilance: Relation to signal probability and expectancy. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 10, 429–432.
Kulp, R. A. and Alluisi, E. A. (1967). Effects of stimulus-response uncertainty on watchkeeping performance and choice reactions. Perception & Psychophysics, 2, 511–515.
Langner, R., and Eickhoff, S.B. (2012). Sustaining attention to simple tasks: A meta-analytic review of the neural mechanism of vigilant attention. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 870–900.
Langner, R., Eickhoff, S. B. and Steinborn, M. B. (2011). Mental fatigue modulates dynamic adaptation to perceptual demand in speeded detection. PLos ONE, 6, 1–10.
Langner, R., Willmes, K., Chatterjee, A., Eickhoff, S. B. and Sturm, W. (2010). Energetic effects of stimulus intensity on prolonged simple reaction-time performance. Psychological Research, 74(5), 499–512.
Lanzetta, T. M., Dember, W. N., Warm, J. S. and Berch, D. B. (1987). Effects of task type and stimulus homogeneity on the event rate function in sustained attention. Human Factors, 29, 625–633.
Leonard, J. P. (Ed.) (1988). Vigilance: Methods, models, and regulation. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.
Lewandowski, L. J. and Kobus, D. A. (1989). Bimodal information processing in sonar performance. Human Performance, 2, 73–84.
Lieberman, H.R., Coffey, B., and Kobrick, J. (1998). A vigilance task sensitive to the effects of stimulants, hypnotics, and environmental stress: The Scanning Visual Vigilance Test. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 30, 416–422.
Lim, J., Wu, W, Wang, J., Detre, J. A., Dinges, D. F. and Rao, H. (2010). Imaging brain fatigue from sustained mental workload: An ASL perfusion study of the time-on-task effect. NeuroImage, 49, 3426–3435.
Linton, S. (1996). General internal medicine. In U. B. S. Prakash (Ed.), Mayo internal medicine board review (pp. 333–352). Rochester, MN: Mayo Clinic.
Lisper, H. O. and Törnros, J. (1974). Effects of inter-signal interval regularity on increase in reaction time in a one hour auditory monitoring task. Acta Psychologica, 38, 455–460.
Loeb, M. and Alluisi, E. A. (1984). Theories of vigilance. In J. S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 179–205). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Loeb, M. and Binford, J. R. (1963). Some factors influencing the effective auditory intensive difference limen. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 35, 884–891.
Loeb, M. and Binford, J. R. (1968). Variation in performance on auditory and visual monitoring tasks as a function of signal and stimulus frequencies. Perception & Psychophysics, 4, 361–367.
Loeb, M. and Binford, J. (1970). Examination of some factors influencing performance on an auditory monitoring task with one signal per session. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83, 40–44.
Loeb, M., Noonan, T. K., Ash, D. W. and Holding, D. H. (1987). Limitations of the cognitive vigilance increment. Human Factors, 29, 661–674.
Lundberg, U. and Frankenhaeuser, M. (1979). Pituitary-adrenal and sympathetic-adrenal correlates of distress and effort. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 3, 125–130.
Mackie, R. R. (Ed.). (1977). Vigilance: Theory, operational performance, and physiological correlates. New York: Plenum.
Mackie, R. R. (1987). Vigilance research: Are we ready for counter measures? Human Factors, 29, 707–723.
Mackworth, J. F. (1968). Vigilance, arousal, and habituation. Psychological Review, 75, 308–322.
Mackworth, J. F. (1969). Vigilance and habituation. Baltimore: Penguin.
Mackworth, J. F. (1970). Vigilance and attention. Baltimore: Penguin
Mackworth, N. H. (1948). The breakdown of vigilance during prolonged visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1, 6–21.
Mackworth, N. H. (1950/1961). Researches on the measurement of human performance. In H. W. Sinaiko (Ed.), Selected papers in the design and use of control systems (pp. 174–331). New York: Dover. (Reprinted from Medical Research Council Special Report Series 268, H. M. Stationery Office, 1950).
MacLean, K. A., Aichele, S. R., Bridwell, D. A., Mangun, G. R., Wojciulik, E. and Saron, C. D. (2009). Interactions between endogenous and exogenous attention during vigilance. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 1042–1058.
Macmillan, N. A. and Creelman, C. D. (2005). Detection theory: A user’s guide. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Matthews, G., Campbell, S. E., Falconer, S., Joyner, L. A., Huggins, J., Gilliland, K. et al. (2002). Fundamental dimensions of subjective state in performance settings: Task engagement, distress, and worry. Emotion, 2, 315–340
Matthews, G. and Davies, D. R. (1998). Arousal and vigilance: The role of task factors. In R. R. Hoffman, M. F. Sherrick and J. S. Warm (Eds.), Viewing psychology as a whole: The integrative science of William N. Dember (pp. 113–144). Washington, DC: APA.
Matthews, G., Davies, D. R. and Holley, P. J. (1993). Cognitive predictors of vigilance. Human Factors, 35, 3–24.
Matthews, G., Davies, D. R., Westerman, S. J. and Stammers, R. B. (2000). Human performance: Cognition, stress, and individual differences. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.
Matthews, G., Szalma, J., Panganiban, A.R., Neubauer, C. and Warm, J.S. (2013). Profiling task stress with the Dundee Stress State Questionnaire. In L. Cavalcanti and S. Azevedo (Eds.), Psychology of Stress (pp. 50–91): Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science.
Matthews, G., Warm, J. S., Reinerman-Jones, L. E., Langheim, L. K., Washburn, D. A. and Tripp, L. (2010). Task engagement, cerebral blood flow velocity, and diagnostic monitoring for sustained attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 16, 187–203.
McCarley, J. S. and Kramer, A. F. (2007). Eye movements as a window on perception and cognition. In R. Parasuraman and M. Rizzo (Eds.), Neuroergonomics: The brain at work (pp. 95–112). New York: Oxford University Press.
McGrath, J. J. (1963). Some problems of definitions and criteria in the study of vigilance performance. In D. N. Buckner and J. J. McGrath (Eds.), Vigilance: A symposium (pp. 227–237). New York: McGraw-Hill.
McVay, J. C. and Kane, M. J. (2010). Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). Psychological Bulletin, 136, 188–197.
Metzger, K. R., Warm, J. S. and Senter, R. J. (1974). Effects of background event rate and critical signal amplitude on vigilance performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38, 1175–1181.
Milosevic, S. (1974). Effects of time and space uncertainty on a vigilance task. Perception & Psychophysics, 15, 331–334.
Molloy, R. and Parasuraman, R. (1996). Monitoring an automated system for a single failure: Vigilance and task complexity effects. Human Factors, 38, 311–322.
Montague, W. E., Weber, C. E. and Adams, J. A. (1965). The effects of signal and response complexity on eighteen hours of visual monitoring. Human Factors, 7, 163–172.
Moore, S. F. and Gross, S. J. (1973). Influence of critical signal regularity, stimulus event matrix and cognitive style on vigilance performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 99, 137–139.
Morris, T. L. and Miller, J. C. (1996). Electrooculographic and performance indices of fatigue during simulated flight. Biological Psychology, 42, 343–360.
Nachreiner, F. (1977). Experiments on the validity of vigilance experiments. In R. R. Mackie (Ed.), Vigilance: Theory, operational performance, and physiological correlates (pp. 665–678). New York: Plenum.
Nachreiner, F. and Hanecke, K. (1992). Vigilance. In A. P. Smith and D. M. Jones (Eds.), Handbook of human performance (pp 262–288). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Natsoulas, T. (1967). What are perceptual reports about? Psychological Bulletin, 67, 249–272.
Navon, D. (1984). Resources: A theoretical soupstone. Psychological Review, 91, 216–234.
Neuchterlein, K. H., Parasuraman, R. and Jiang, Q. (1983). Visual sustained attention: Image degradation produces rapid sensitivity decrement over time. Science, 220, 327–329.
Nicely, P. E. and Miller, G. A. (1957). Some effects of unequal spatial distributions on the detectability of radar targets. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53, 195–198.
Nickerson, R. S. (1992). Looking ahead: Human factors challenges in a changing world. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Noonan, T. K., Ash, D., Loeb, M. and Warm, J. S. (1984). Task complexity, noise, and cognitive vigilance performance. In A. Mital (Ed.), Trends in ergonomics human factors I (pp. 33–38). New York: North-Holland.
Nygren, T. E. (1991). Psychometric properties of subjective workload measurement techniques: Implications for their use in the assessment of perceived mental workload. Human Factors, 33, 17–33.
Parasuraman, R. (1979). Memory load and event rate control sensitivity decrements in sustained attention. Science, 205, 924–927.
Parasuraman, R. (1984). Sustained attention in detection and discrimination. In R. Parasuraman and D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 243–271). Orlando: Academic Press.
Parasuraman, R. (1986). Vigilance, monitoring, and search. In K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman and J. P. Thomas (Eds.), Handbook of human perception and performance, Vol. II: Cognitive processes and performance (pp. 41-1–41-49). New York: Wiley.
Parasuraman, R. (1987). Human computer monitoring. Human Factors, 29, 695–706.
Parasuraman, R. (2011). Neuroergonomics: Brain, cognition, and performance at work. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 181–186.
Parasuraman, R. and Davies, D. R. (1977). A taxonomic analysis of vigilance. In R. R. Mackie (Ed.), Vigilance: Theory, operational performance, and physiological correlates (pp. 559–574). New York: Plenum.
Parasuraman, R., De Visser, E., Clarke, E., McGarry, W. R., Hussey, E., Shaw, T. and Thompson, J. (2009). Detecting threat-related intentional actions of others: Effects of image quality, response mode, and target cueing on vigilance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15, 275–290.
Parasuraman, R., Hancock, P. A. and Olofinoba, O. (1997). Alarm effectiveness in driver-centered collision warning systems. Ergonomics, 19, 390–399.
Parasuraman, R., Warm, J. S. and Dember, W. N. (1987). Vigilance: Taxonomy and utility. In L. S. Mark, J. S. Warm and R. L. Huston (Eds.), Ergonomics and human factors: Recent research (pp. 11–32). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Parasuraman, R., Warm, J. S. and See, J. E. (1998). Brain systems of vigilance. In R. Parasuraman (Ed.), The attentive brain (pp. 221–256). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Pigeau, R. A., Angus, R. G., O’Neil, P. and Mack, I. (1995). Vigilance latencies to aircraft detection among NORAD surveillance operators. Human Factors, 37, 622–634.
Pop, V. L., Stearman, E. J., Kazi, S., and Durso, F. T. (2012). Using engagement to negate vigilance decrements in the Nextgen environment. International Journal of Human Computer Interaction, 28, 99–106.
Posner, M. I. and Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma and D. G. Bowhui (Eds.), Attention and performance (pp. 531–556). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Proctor, R. W. and Van Zandt, T. (1994). Human factors in simple and complex systems. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Proctor, R. W. and Vu, K-PL. (2010). Cumulative knowledge and progress in human factors. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 623–651.
Quinlan, P. T. (2003). Visual feature integration theory: Past, present, and future. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 643–673.
Reinerman-Jones, L. E., Matthews, G., Warm, J. S. and Langheim, L. K. (2011). Selection for vigilance assignments: A review and proposed new directions. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 12, 273–296.
Risko, E. F. Anderson, N., Sarwal, A., Engelhardt, M. and Kingstone, A. (2012). Everyday attention: Variation in mind wandering and memory in a lecture. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 234–242.
Robertson, I. H., Manly, T. Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T. and Yiend, J. (1997). “Oops!”: Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologica, 35, 747–758.
Santee, J. L. and Egeth, H. E. (1982). Do reaction time and accuracy measure the same aspects of letter recognition? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 489–501.
Sarter, M., Givens, B., and Bruno, J.P. (2001). The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: where top-down meets bottom-up. Brain Research Reviews, 35, 146–160.
Scerbo, M. W. (1998). What’s so boring about vigilance? In R. R. Hoffman, M. F. Sherrick and J. S. Warm (Eds.), Viewing psychology as a whole: The integrative science of William N. Dember (pp. 145–166). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Scerbo, M. W., Warm, J. S., Doettling, V. S., Parasuraman, R. and Fisk, A. D. (1987). Event asynchrony and task demands in sustained attention. In L. S. Mark, J. S. Warm and R. L. Huston (Eds.), Ergonomics and human factors: Recent research (pp. 33–39). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Scerbo, M. W., Warm, J. S. and Fisk, A. D. (1987). Event asynchrony and signal regularity in sustained attention. Current Psychological Research and Reviews, 5, 335–343.
Schmidke, H. (1976). Vigilance. In E. Simpson and P. C. Weiser (Eds.), Psychological and physiological correlates of work and fatigue. Springfield, IL: Thomas.
Schneider, W. and Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Control and automatic human information processing, 1: Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1–66S.
Schoenfeld, V. S. and Scerbo, M. W. (1999). The effects of search differences for the presence and absence of features on vigilance performance and mental workload. In M. W. Scerbo and M. Mouloua (Eds.), Automation technology and human performance: Current research and trends (pp. 177–182). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schroeder, S. R. and Holland, J. G. (1968). Operant control of eye movements. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 161–166.
See, J. E., Howe, S. R., Warm, J. S. and Dember, W. N. (1995). Meta-analysis of the sensitivity decrement in vigilance. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 230–249.
Seli, P., Cheyne, J.A., and Smilek, D. (2012). Attention failures verses misplaced diligence: Separating attention lapses from speed-accuracy trade-off. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 277–291.
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Shaw, T. H., Finomore, V. S., Warm, J. S., and Matthews, G. (2012). Effects of regular or irregular event schedules on cerebral hemovelocity during a sustained attention task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34, 57–66.
Shaw, T. H., Funke, M. E., Dillard, M., Funke, G. J., Warm, J.S. and Parasuraman, R. (2013). Event related cerebral hemodynamics reveal target-specific resource allocation for both “go” and “no-go” response based vigilance tasks. Brain and Cognition, 82, 265–273.
Shaw, T. H., Warm, J. S., Finomore, V., Tripp, L., Matthews, G., Weiler, E. and Parasuraman, R. (2009). Effects of sensory modality on cerebral blood flow velocity during vigilance. Neuroscience Letters, 461, 207–211.
Sheridan, T. (1980). Supervisory control. In G. Salvendy (Ed.), Handbook of human factors (pp. 1243–1268). New York: Wiley.
Smallwood, J., Davies, J. B., Heim, D., Finnigan, F., Sudberry, M., O’Conner, R. and Obonsawin, M. (2004). Subjective experience and the attentional lapse: Task engagement and disengagement during sustained attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 657–690.
Smit, A., Eling, P. A. T. M. and Coenen, A. M. L. (2004). Mental effort causes vigilance decrease due to resource depletion. Acta Psychologica, 115, 35–42.
Smith, R. P., Warm, J. S. and Alluisi, E. A. (1966). Effects of temporal uncertainty on watchkeeping performance. Perception & Psychophysics, 1, 293–299.
Stern, J. A., Boyer, D., Schroeder, D., Touchstone, R. M. and Stoliarov, N. (1996). Blinks, saccades, and fixation pauses during vigilance task performance, II: Gender and time of day. (Tech. Rep. DOT-FAA-AM-96–9), Washington DC: Office of Aviation Medicine, Federal Aviation Administration.
Stollery, B. T. (2006). Vigilance. In W. Karwowski (Ed.), International encyclopedia of ergonomics and human factors, Vol. 1, (2nd ed., pp. 965–968). Boca Raton, FL: Taylor & Francis.
Strauch, B. (2002). Investigating human error: Incidents, accidents, and complex systems. Burlington, VT: Ashgate.
Stroh, C. M. (1971). Vigilance: The problem of sustained attention. Oxford: Pergamon Press.
Swets, J. A. (1977). Signal detection theory applied to vigilance. In R. R. Mackie (Ed.), Vigilance: Operational performance and physiological correlates (pp. 705–718). New York: Plenum Press.
Swets, J. A. and Kristofferson, A. B. (1970). Attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 21, 339–366.
Szalma, J. L. (2011). Workload and stress in vigilance: the impact of display format and task type. American Journal of Psychology, 124, 441–454.
Szalma, J. L. and Hancock, P. A. (2011). Noise effects on human performance: A meta-analytic synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 682–707.
Szalma, J. L., Hancock, P. A., Dember, W. N. and Warm, J. S. (2006). Training for vigilance: The effect of knowledge of results format and dispositional optimism and pessimism on performance and stress. British Journal of Psychology, 97, 115–135.
Szalma, J. L., Hancock, P. A., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N. and Parsons, K. S. (2006). Training for vigilance: Using predictive power to evaluate feedback effectiveness. Human Factors, 48, 682–692.
Szalma, J. L., Miller, L. C., Hitchcock, E. M., Warm, J. S. and Dember, W. N. (1999). Intraclass and interclass transfer of training for vigilance. In M. W. Scerbo and M. Mouloua (Eds.), Automation Technology and Human Performance: Current Research and Trends (pp. 183–187). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Szalma, J. L., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., Dember, W. N., Weiler, E. M., Meier, A. and Eggemeier, F. T. (2004), Effects of sensory modality and task duration on performance, workload, and stress in sustained attention. Human Factors, 46, 219–233.
Teichner, W. H. (1974). The detection of a simple visual signal as a function of time on watch. Human Factors, 16, 339–353.
Temple, J. G., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N., Jones, K. S., LaGrange, C. M. and Matthews, G. (2000). The effects of signal salience and caffeine on performance, workload, and stress in an abbreviated vigilance task. Human Factors, 42, 183–194.
Thackray, R. I., Bailey, J. P. and Touchstone, R. M. (1977). Physiological, subjective, and performance correlates of reported boredom and monotony while performing a simulated radar control task. In R. R. Mackie (Ed.), Vigilance, theory, operational performance, and physiological correlates (pp. 203–215). New York: Plenum.
Thackray, R. I., Bailey, J. P. and Touchstone, R. M. (1979). The effect of increased monitoring load on vigilance performance using a simulated radar display. Ergonomics, 22, 529–539.
Tickner, A. H. and Poulton, E. C. (1973). Monitoring up to 16 synthetic television pictures showing a great deal of movement. Ergonomics, 16, 381–401.
Toole, J. F. (1984). Cerebrovascular disorders (3rd ed.). New York: Raven Press.
Treisman, A. M. and Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological Review, 95, 15–48.
Tripp, L. D. and Warm, J. S. (2007). Transcranial Doppler sonography. In R. Parasuraman and M. Rizzo (Eds.), Neuroergonomics: The brain at work (pp. 82–94). New York: Oxford University Press.
Vernon, H. M. (1921). Industrial fatigue and efficiency. London: G. Routledge and Sons.
Vidulich, M. A., Wickens, C. D., Tsang, P. S. and Flach, J. M. (2010). Information processing in aviation. In Salas, E. and Maurino, D. (Eds.), Human Factors in Aviation (pp. 175–215). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Warm, J. S. (1977). Psychological processes in sustained attention. In R. R. Mackie (Ed.), Vigilance: Theory, Operational Performance, and Physiological Correlates (pp. 623–644). New York: Plenum.
Warm, J. S. (1984a). An introduction to vigilance. In J. S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 1–14). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Warm, J. S. (Ed.) (1984b). Sustained attention in human performance. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Warm, J. S. (1993). Vigilance and target detection. In C. D. Wickens and B. Huey (Eds.), Teams in transition: Workload, stress, and human factors (pp. 139–170). Washington, DC: National Research Council.
Warm, J. S. and Alluisi, E. A. (1971). Influence of temporal uncertainty and sensory modality of signals on watchkeeping performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 87, 303–308.
Warm, J. S. and Dember, W. N. (1998). Tests of a vigilance taxonomy. In R. R. Hoffman, M. F. Sherrick and J. S. Warm (Eds.), Viewing psychology as a whole: The integrative science of William N. Dember (pp. 87–112). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N. and Parasuraman, R. (1991). Effects of olfactory stimulation on performance and stress in a visual sustained attention task. Journal of the Society of Cosmetic Chemists, 42, 199–210.
Warm, J. S., Howe, S. R., Fishbein, H. D., Dember, W. N. and Sprague, R. L. (1984). Cognitive demand and the vigilance decrement. In A. Mital, (Ed.), Trends in ergonomics/human factors I (pp. 15–20). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Warm, J. S. and Jerison, H. J. (1984). The psychophysics of vigilance. In J. S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 15–60). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Warm, J. S., Kanfer, F. H., Kuwada, S. and Clark, J. L. (1972). Motivation in vigilance: Effects of self-evaluation and experimenter controlled feedback. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 92, 123–127.
Warm, J. S., Loeb, M. and Alluisi, E. A. (1970). Variations in watchkeeping performance as a function of the rate and duration of visual signals. Perception & Psychophysics, 7, 97–99.
Warm, J. S, Matthews, G. and Finomore, V. S. (2008). Workload, stress, and vigilance. In P. A. Hancock and J. L. Szalma (Eds.), Performance under stress (pp. 115–141). Brookfield, VT: Ashgate.
Warm, J. S., Matthews, G. and Parasuraman, R. (2009). Cerebral hemodynamics and vigilance performance. Military Psychology, 21, S75–S100.
Warm, J. S. and Parasuraman, R. (Eds.) (1987). Vigilance: Basic and applied research [Special issue]. Human Factors, 29, 623–740.
Warm, J. S. and Parasuraman, R. (2007). Cerebral hemodynamics and vigilance. In R. Parasuraman and M. Rizzo (Eds.), Neuroergonomics: The brain at work (pp. 146–158). New York: Oxford University Press.
Warm, J. S., Parasuraman, R. and Matthews, G. (2008). Vigilance requires hard mental work and is stressful. Human Factors, 50, 433–441.
Warm, J. S., Rosa, R. R. and Colligan, M. J. (1989). Effects of auxiliary load on vigilance performance in a simulated work environment. Proceedings of the Human Factors Society Annual Meeting, 33, 1419–1421.
Warm, J. S., Tripp, L. D., Matthews, G. and Helton, W. S. (2012). Cerebral hemodynamic indices of operator fatigue in vigilance. In G. Matthews, P. A. Desmond, C. Neubauer and P. A. Hancock (Eds.), The handbook of operator fatigue (pp. 197–207). Brookfield, VT: Ashgate.
Warm, J. S. Wait, R. G. and Loeb, M. (1976). Head restraint enhances visual monitoring performance. Perception & Psychophysics, 20, 281–286.
Weirwille, W. W., Ellsworth, L. A., Wreggit, S. S., Fairbanks, R. J. and Kim, C. L. (1994). Research on vehicle based driver status/performance monitoring: Development, validation, and refinement of algorithms for detection of driver drowsiness, (Tech. Rep. DOT-HS-808–247). Washington, D. C.: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration Final Report.
Wickens, C. D. (1984). Processing resources in attention. In R. Parasuraman and D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 63–102). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Wickens, C. D., Hollands, J. G., Banbury, S., and Parasuraman, R. (2013). Engineering psychology and human performance (4th ed.). Boston: Pearson.
Wiener, E. L. (1984). Vigilance and inspection. In J. S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 207–246). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Wiggins, M. W. (2011). Vigilance during a simulated general aviation flight. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 229–235.
Wolfe, J. M., Kluender, K. R., Levi, D. M., Bartoshuk, L. M., Herz, R. S., Klatzky, R. L. et al. (2012). Sensation and Perception (3rd ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.
Wyatt, S. and Langdon, J. N. (1932). Inspection processes in industry. Medical Research Council Industrial Health Research Board Report NO. 63. London: H. M. Stationery Office.
Yamaguchi, S., Yamagata, S. and Kobayashi, S. (2000). Cerebral asymmetry of the ‘‘top-down’’ allocation of attention to global and local features. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 1–5.
Yoshitake, H. (1978). Three characteristic patterns of subjective fatigue symptoms. Ergonomics, 21, 231–233.