Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
×
Home
  • Online publication date: July 2015

17 - Haptic Perception

from Part III - Perception and Modality
Recommend this book

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this book to your organisation's collection.

The Cambridge Handbook of Applied Perception Research
  • Online ISBN: 9780511973017
  • Book DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511973017
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to *
×

References

Ackerley, R., McGlone, F., Olausson, H. and Wessberg, J. (2010). Mapping the sensation of wetness across the body. In Society for Neuroscience Abstracts (Program No. 285.4).
Basdogan C., De, S., Kim, J., Muniyandi, M., Kim, H. and Srinivasan, M. A. (2004). Haptics in minimally invasive surgical simulation and training. IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications, 24(2), 56–64.
Benali-Khoudja, M., Hafez, M., Alexandre, J.-M., Benachour, J. and Kheddar, A. (2003). Thermal feedback model for virtual reality. In Proceedings of the International Symposium on Micromechatronics and Human Science, pp. 153–158.
Bensmaïa, S. and Hollins, M. (2005). Pacinian representations of fine surface texture. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 842–854.
Bensmaïa, S. J. and Hollins, M. (2003). The vibrations of texture. Somatosensory & Motor Research, 20, 33–43.
Bergmann Tiest, W. M. (2007). An experimentally verified model of the perceived ‘coldness’ of objects. In Proceedings of the 2nd Joint Eurohaptics Conference and Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems. Tsukuba, Japan: IEEE Computer Society, pp. 61–65.
Bergmann Tiest, W. M. and Kappers, A. M. L. (2006). Analysis of haptic perception of materials by multidimensional scaling and physical measurements of roughness and compressibility. Acta Psychologica, 121, 1–20.
Bergmann Tiest, W. M. and Kappers, A. M. L. (2008). Thermosensory reversal effect quantified. Acta Psychologica, 127, 46–50.
Bergmann Tiest, W. M. and Kappers, A. M. L. (2009a). Cues for haptic perception of compliance. IEEE Transactions on Haptics, 2, 189–199.
Bergmann Tiest, W. M. and Kappers, A. M. L. (2009b). Tactile perception of thermal diffusivity. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 71, 481–489.
Bergmann Tiest, W. M., Kosters, N. D., Kappers, A. M. L. and Daanen, H. A. M. (2012). Haptic perception of wetness. Acta Psychologica, 141, 159–163.
Bergmann Tiest, W. M., Vrijling, A. C. L. and Kappers, A. M. L. (2013). Haptic discrimination and matching of viscosity. IEEE Transactions on Haptics, 6, 24–34.
Bicchi, A., Scilingo, E. P. and De Rossi, D. (2000). Haptic discrimination of softness in teleoperation: The role of the contact area spread rate. IEEE Transactions on Robotics and Automation, 16, 496–504.
Blumenfeld, W. (1937). The relationship between the optical and haptic construction of space. Acta Psychologica, 2, 125–174.
Cadoret, G. and Smith, A. M. (1996). Friction, not texture, dictates grip forces used during object manipulation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 75, 1963–1969.
Christensen, C. M. and Casper, L. M. (1987). Oral and nonoral perception of solution viscosity. Journal of Food Science, 52, 445–447.
Cornsweet, T. N. (1962). The staircase-method in psychophysics. The American Journal of Psychology, 75, 485–491.
Davidson, P. W. (1972). Haptic judgments of curvature by blind and sighted humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 43–55.
Ekman, G., Hosman, J. and Lindström, B. (1965). Roughness, smoothness and preference: A study of quantitative relations in individual subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 18–26.
Fernández-Díaz, M. and Travieso, D. (2011). Performance in haptic geometrical matching tasks depends on movement and position of the arms. Acta Psychologica, 136, 382–389.
Fleischer, C., Kondak, K., Wege, A. and Kossyk, I. (2009). Research on exoskeletons at the TU Berlin. In Advances in Robotics Research (pp. 335–346). Berlin: Springer.
Freyberger, F. K. B. and Färber, B. (2006). Compliance discrimination of deformable objects by squeezing with one and two fingers. In Proceedings of EuroHaptics 2006, pp. 271–276.
Gescheider, G. A. (1988). Psychophysical scaling. Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 169–200.
Gibson, J. J. (1963). The useful dimensions of sensitivity. American Psychologist, 18, 1–15.
Goodwin, A. W., John, K. T. and Marceglia, A. H. (1991). Tactile discrimination of curvature by humans using only cutaneous information from the fingerpads. Experimental Brain Research, 86, 663–672.
Goodwin, A. W. and Wheat, H. E. (1992). Human tactile discrimination of curvature when contact area with the skin remains constant. Experimental Brain Research, 88, 447–450.
Gordon, I. A. and Morison, V. (1982). The haptic perception of curvature. Perception & Psychophysics, 31, 446–450.
Greenspan, J. D., Roy, E. A., Caldwell, P. A. and Farooq, N. S. (2003). Thermosensory intensity and affect throughout the perceptible range. Somatosensory & Motor Research, 20, 19–26.
Grierson, L. E. M. and Carnahan, H. (2006). Manual exploration and the perception of slipperiness. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 1070–1081.
Hamza-Lup, F. G., Bogdan, C. M., Popovici, D. M. and Costea, O. D. (2011) A survey of visuo-haptic simulation in surgical training, In The third international conference on mobile, hybrid, and on-line learning. Gosier, Guadeloupe, France, pp. 57–62.
Harper, R. and Stevens, S. S. (1964). Subjective hardness of compliant materials. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 16, 204–215.
Ho, H. and Jones, L. A. (2006a). Contribution of thermal cues to material discrimination and localization. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 118–128.
Ho, H. and Jones, L. A. (2006b). Thermal model for hand-object interactions. In Proceedings of the 14th Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator System. Alexandria, VA: IEEE, pp. 461–467
Hogema, J. H., De Vries, S. C., Van Erp, J. B. F. and Kiefer, R. J. (2009). A tactile seat for direction coding in car driving: Field evaluation. IEEE Transactions on Haptics, 2(4), 181–188.
Hollins, M., Bensmaïa, S., Karlof, K. and Young, F. (2000). Individual differences in perceptual space for tactile textures: Evidence from multidimensional scaling. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 1534–1544.
Hollins, M., Bensmaïa, S. J. and Washburn, S. (2001). Vibrotactile adaptation impairs discrimination of fine, but not coarse, textures. Somatosensory & Motor Research, 18, 253–262.
Hollins, M., Faldowski, R., Rao, S. and Young, F. (1993). Perceptual dimensions of tactile surface texture: A multidimensional scaling analysis. Perception & Psychophysics, 54, 697–705.
Hollins, M. and Risner, S. R. (2000). Evidence for the duplex theory of tactile texture perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 695–705.
Hunter, I. M. L. (1954). Tactile-kinaesthetic perception of straightness in blind and sighted humans. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 6, 149–154.
Johansson, R. S. and Westling, G. (1984). Roles of glabrous skin receptors and sensorimotor memory in automatic control of precision grip when lifting rougher or more slippery objects. Experimental Brain Research, 56, 550–564.
Jones, L. A. (1986). Perception of force and weight: Theory and research. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 29–42.
Jones, L. A. and Ho, H.-N. (2008). Warm or cool, large or small? The challenge of thermal displays. IEEE Transactions on Haptics, 1, 53–70.
Kaas, A. and Van Mier, H. (2006). Haptic spatial matching in near peripersonal space. Experimental Brain Research, 170, 403–413.
Kahrimanovic, M., Bergmann Tiest, W. M. and Kappers, A. M. L. (2010). Haptic perception of volume and surface area of 3-D objects. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72(2), 517–527.
Kahrimanovic, M., Bergmann Tiest, W. M. and Kappers, A. M. L. (2011). Characterization of the haptic shape-weight illusion with 3-dimensional objects. IEEE Transactions on Haptics, 4, 316–320.
Kappers, A. M. L. (1999). Large systematic deviations in the haptic perception of parallelity. Perception, 28(8), 1001–1012.
Kappers, A. M. L. (2007). Haptic space processing – allocentric and egocentric reference frames. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(3), 208–218.
Kappers, A. M. L. and Koenderink, J. J. (1999). Haptic perception of spatial relations. Perception, 28(6), 781–795.
Kappers, A. M. L., Koenderink, J. J. and Lichtenegger, I. (1994). Haptic identification of curved surfaces. Perception & Psychophysics, 56(1), 53–61.
Kenshalo, D. R., Holmes, C. E. and Wood, P. B. (1968). Warm and cool thresholds as a function of rate of stimulus temperature change. Perception & Psychophysics, 3, 81–84.
Klatzky, R. L. and Lederman, S. J. (1995). Identifying objects from a haptic glance. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 1111–1123.
Klatzky, R. L., Lederman, S. J. and Metzger, V. A. (1985). Identifying objects by touch: An “expert system.” Perception & Psychophysics, 37, 299–302.
Krishna, A. (2006). Interaction of senses: The effect of vision versus touch on the elongation bias. The Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 557–566.
Lederman, S. J. and Klatzky, R. L. (1987). Hand movements: A window into haptic object recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 342–368.
Lederman, S. J. and Klatzky, R. L. (1997). Relative availability of surface and object properties during early haptic processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1680–1707.
Lederman, S. J. and Klatzky, R. L. (2009). Haptic perception: A tutorial. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 71, 1439–1459.
Li, Y. (2005). Perceptions of temperature, moisture and comfort in clothing during environmental transients. Ergonomics, 48, 234–248.
Li, Y., Plante, A. M. and Holcombe, B. V. (1993). The physical mechanisms of the perception of dampness in fabrics. Journal of Thermal Biology, 18, 417–419.
Loomis, J. M. and Lederman, S. J. (1986). Tactual perception. In K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman and J. P. Thomas (Eds.), Cognitive Processes and Performance, Vol. 2, (pp. 31.1–31.41). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Louw, S., Kappers, A. M. L. and Koenderink, J. J. (2000). Haptic detection thresholds of Gaussian profiles over the whole range of spatial scales. Experimental Brain Research, 132, 369–374.
Moskowitz, H. R. (1972). Scales of subjective viscosity and fluidity of gum solutions. Journal of Texture Studies, 3, 89–100.
Murray, D., Ellis, R., Bandomir, C. and Ross, H. (1999). Charpentier (1891) on the size-weight illusion. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 1681–1685.
Plaisier, M. A., Bergmann Tiest, W. M. and Kappers, A. M. L. (2008). Haptic pop-out in a hand sweep. Acta Psychologica, 128, 368–377.
Plaisier, M. A., Bergmann Tiest, W. M. and Kappers, A. M. L.(2009). Salient features in three-dimensional haptic shape perception. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 71, 421–430.
Pont, S. C., Kappers, A. M. L. and Koenderink, J. J. (1997). Haptic curvature discrimination at several regions of the hand. Perception & Psychophysics, 59(8), 1225–1240.
Pont, S. C., Kappers, A. M. L. and Koenderink, J. J. (1999). Similar mechanisms underlie curvature comparison by static and dynamic touch. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 874–894.
Proske, U. and Gandevia, S. C. (2009). The kinaesthetic senses. The Journal of Physiology, 587, 4139–4146.
Provancher, W. R. and Sylvester, N. D. (2009). Fingerpad skin stretch increases the perception of virtual friction. IEEE Transactions on Haptics, 2(4), 212–223.
Sarda, A., Deterre, R. and Vergneault, C. (2004). Heat perception measurements of the different parts found in a car passenger compartment. Measurement, 35, 65–75.
Scilingo, E. P., Bianchi, M., Grioli, G. and Bicchi, A. (2010). Rendering softness: Integration of kinaesthetic and cutaneous information in haptic devices. IEEE Transactions on Haptics, 3, 109–118.
Scott Blair, G. W. and Coppen, F. M. V. (1939). The subjective judgments of the elastic and plastic properties of soft bodies; the “differential thresholds” for viscosities and compression moduli. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 128, 109–125.
Smith, A. M. and Scott, S. H. (1996). Subjective scaling of smooth surface friction. Journal of Neurophysiology, 75, 1957–1962.
Smith, C. H., Logemann, J. A., Burghardt, W. R., Carrell, T. D. and Zecker, S. G. (1997). Oral sensory discrimination of fluid viscosity. Dysphagia, 12, 68–73.
Srinivasan, M. A. and LaMotte, R. H. (1995). Tactual discrimination of softness. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73, 88–101.
Steele, C. M., Van Lieshout, P. H. H. M. and Goff, H. (2003). The rheology of liquids: A comparison of clinicians’ subjective impressions and objective measurement. Dysphagia, 18(3), 182–195.
Stevens, J. C. and Choo, K. K. (1998). Temperature sensitivity of the body surface over the life span. Somatosensory & Motor Research, 15, 13–28.
Stevens, J. C. and Marks, L. E. (1979). Spatial summation of cold. Physiology & Behavior, 22, 541–547.
Stevens, S. S. (1957). On the psychophysical law. Psychological Review, 64, 153–181.
Stevens, S. S. and Guirao, M. (1964). Scaling of apparent viscosity. Science, 144, 1157–1158.
Stevens, S. S. and Harris, J. R. (1962). The scaling of subjective roughness and smoothness. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 489–494.
Stone, L. A. (1967). Subjective roughness and smoothness for individual judges. Psychonomic Science, 9, 347–348.
Treisman, A. and Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological Review, 95, 15–48.
Tritsch, M. F. (1988). The veridical perception of object temperature with varying skin temperature. Perception & Psychophysics, 43, 531–540.
Tsagarakis, N. G., Horne, T. and Caldwell, D. G. (2005). Slip aesthesis: A portable 2D slip/skin stretch display for the fingertip. In Proceedings of the 1st Joint Eurohaptics Conference and Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems. Pisa, Italy: IEEE Computer Society, pp. 214–219.
Van Erp, J. B. F. and Van Veen, H. A. H. C. (2006). Touch down: The effect of artificial touch cues on orientation in microgravity. Neuroscience Letters, 404(1–2), 78–82.
Verrillo, R. T., Bolanowski, S. J. and McGlone, F. P. (1999). Subjective magnitude of tactile roughness. Somatosensory & Motor Research, 16, 352–360.
Van Polanen, V., Bergmann Tiest, W. M., and Kappers, A. M. L. (2012a) Haptic pop-out of movable stimuli, Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 74, 204–215.
Van Polanen, V., Bergmann Tiest, W. M., and Kappers, A. M. L. (2012b) Haptic search for hard and soft spheres, PLOS ONE, 7(10), e45398.
Vogels, I. M. L. C., Kappers, A. M. L. and Koenderink, J. J. (1996). Haptic aftereffect of curved surfaces. Perception, 25, 109–119.
Wang, Q. and Hayward, V. (2006). Compact, portable, modular, high-performance, distributed tactile transducer device based on lateral skin deformation. In Proceedings of the 14th International Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems. Arlington, VA: IEEE Computer Society, pp. 67–72.
Wang, Z., Li, Y., Kowk, Y. and Yeung, C. (2002). Mathematical simulation of the perception of fabric thermal and moisture sensations. Textile Research Journal, 72, 327–334.
Watson, A. and Fitzhugh, A. (1990). The method of constant stimuli is inefficient. Perception & Psychophysics, 47, 87–91.
Wijntjes, M. W. A., Sato, A., Hayward, V. and Kappers, A. M. L. (2009). Local surface orientation dominates haptic curvature discrimination. IEEE Transactions on Haptics, 2, 94–102.
Yamakawa, M. and Isaji, S. (1987). Factors affecting the clamminess. Journal of the Textile Machinery Society of Japan, 33, 9–15.
Zigler, M. J. (1923). An experimental study of the perception of clamminess. American Journal of Psychology, 34, 550–561.