Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T21:50:26.569Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 17 - Alexithymia and Body Awareness

from Part IV - Biological Considerations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 September 2018

Olivier Luminet
Affiliation:
Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium
R. Michael Bagby
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Graeme J. Taylor
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Alexithymia
Advances in Research, Theory, and Clinical Practice
, pp. 321 - 334
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainley, V., Maister, L., Brokfeld, J., et al. (2013). More of myself: Manipulating interoceptive awareness by heightened attention to bodily and narrative aspects of the self. Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 12311238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ainley, V., Tajadura-Jiminez, A., Fotopoulou, A., et al. (2012). Looking into myself: Changes in interoceptive sensitivity during mirror self-observation. Psychophysiology, 49, 16721676.Google Scholar
Ainley, V. and Tsakiris, M. (2013). Body conscious? Interoceptive awareness, measured by heartbeat perception, is negatively correlated with self-objectification. PLoS One, 8, e55568.Google Scholar
Bagby, R.M., Parker, J.D.A., and Taylor, G.J. (1994). The Twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale – I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 2332.Google Scholar
Baier, B. and Karnath, H.O. (2008). Tight link between our sense of limb ownership and self-awareness of actions. Stroke, 39, 486488.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. and Simmons, W.K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16, 419429.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L.W. (2007). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617645.Google Scholar
Bechara, A. and Naqvi, N. (2004). Listening to your heart: Interoceptive awareness as a gateway to feeling. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 102103.Google Scholar
Bergouignan, L., Nyberg, L., and Ehrsson, H.H. (2014). Out-of-body-induced hippocampal amnesia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 111, 44214426.Google Scholar
Betka, S., Pfeifer, G., Garfinkel, S., et al. (2017). How do self-assessment of alexithymia and sensivitity to bodily sensations relate to alcohol consumption? Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 42, 8188.Google Scholar
Bornemann, B., Herbert, B.M., Mehling, W.E., et al. (2015). Differential changes in self-reported aspects of interoceptive awareness through three months of contemplative training. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, e01504.Google Scholar
Bornemann, B. and Singer, T. (2017). Taking time to feel your body: Steady increase in heartbeat perception accuracy and decreases in alexithymia over 9 months of contemplative mental training. Psychophysiology, 54, 469482.Google Scholar
Cameron, O.G. (2001). Interoception: The inside story – a model for psychosomatic processes. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63, 697710.Google Scholar
Craig, A.D. (2003). Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Current Opinion in Neurology, 13, 500505.Google Scholar
Craig, A.D. (2004). Human feelings: Why are some more aware than others? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 239241.Google Scholar
Craig, A.D. (2008). Interoception and emotion: A neuroanatomical perspective. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J.M., and Felman Barrett, L. (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions, 3rd edition, pp. 272290. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Craig, A.D. (2009). How do you feel – now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 5970.Google Scholar
Critchley, H.D., Corfield, D.R., Chandler, M.P., et al. (2000). Cerebral correlates of autonomic cardiovascular arousal: A functional neuroimaging investigation in humans. Journal of Physiology, 523, 259270.Google Scholar
Critchley, H.D. and Garfinkel, S.N. (2017). Interoception and emotion. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 714.Google Scholar
Critchley, H.D., Mathias, C.J., and Dolan, R.J. (2001). Neuroanatomical basis for first- and second-order representations of bodily states. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 207212.Google Scholar
Critchley, H.D., Mathias, C.J., Josephs, O., et al. (2003). Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control: Converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence. Brain, 126, 21392152.Google Scholar
Critchley, H.D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., et al. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 189195.Google Scholar
Damasio, A.R. (1999). The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace & Company.Google Scholar
Dunn, B.D., Dalgleish, T., Ogilvie, A.D., et al. (2007). Heartbeat perception in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 19211930.Google Scholar
Dunn, B.D., Galton, H.C., Morgan, R., et al. (2010). Listening to your heart: How interoception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision making. Psychological Science, 21, 18351844.Google Scholar
Durlik, C., Brown, G., and Tsakiris, M. (2014). Enhanced interoceptive awareness during anticipation of public speaking is associated with fear of negative evaluation. Cognition and Emotion, 28, 530540.Google Scholar
Ernst, J., Böker, H., Hättenschwiler, J., et al. (2014). The association of interoceptive awareness and alexithymia with neurotransmitter concentrations in insula and anterior cingulate. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 857863.Google Scholar
Eshkevari, E., Rieger, E., Longo, M.R., et al. (2014). Persistent body image disturbance following recovery from eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 47, 400409.Google Scholar
Fischer, D., Berberich, G., Zaudig, M., et al. (2016). Interoceptive processes in anorexia nervosa in the time course of cognitive-behavioral therapy: A pilot study. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 7, 199.Google Scholar
Fischer, D., Messner, M., and Pollatos, O. (2017). Improvement of interoceptive processes after an 8-week body scan intervention. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 452.Google Scholar
Friston, K., Schwartenbeck, P., Fitzgerald, T., et al. (2013). The anatomy of choice: Active inference and agency. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 598.Google Scholar
Friston, K.J., Stephan, K.E., Montague, R., et al. (2014). Computational psychiatry: The brain as a phantastic organ. The Lancet Psychiatry, 1, 148158.Google Scholar
Fuchs, T. and Schlimme, J.E. (2009). Embodiment and psychopathology: A phenomenological perspective. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 22, 570575.Google Scholar
Garfinkel, S.N. and Critchley, H.D. (2013). Interoception, emotion and brain: New insights link internal physiology to social behaviour. Commentary on: Anterior insular cortex mediates bodily sensibility and social anxiety by Terasawa et al. (2012). Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 231234.Google Scholar
Garfinkel, S.N., Critchley, H.D., and Pollatos, O. (2015a). The interoceptive system: Implications for cognition, emotion, and health. In Cacioppo, J.T., Tassinary, L.G., and Berntson, G.G. (Eds.), Handbook of Psychophysiology, 4th edition, pp. 427443. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Garfinkel, S.N., Manassei, M.F., Hamilton-Fletcher, G., et al. (2016). Interoceptive dimensions across cardiac and respiratory axes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371, 1708.Google Scholar
Garfinkel, S.N., Seth, A.K., Barrett, A.B., et al. (2015b). Knowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biological Psychology, 104, 6574.Google Scholar
Georgiou, E., Mai, S., and Pollatos, O. (2016). Describe your feelings: Body illusion related to alexithymia in adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1690.Google Scholar
Grynberg, D. and Pollatos, O. (2015). Alexithymia modulates the experience of the rubber hand illusion. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 357.Google Scholar
Herbert, B.M., Blechert, J., Hautzinger, M., et al. (2013). Intuitive eating is associated with interoceptive sensitivity. Effects on body mass index? Appetite, 70, 2230.Google Scholar
Herbert, B.M., Herbert, C., and Pollatos, O. (2011). On the relationship between interoceptive awareness and alexithymia: Is interoceptive awareness related to emotional awareness. Journal of Personality, 79, 11491175.Google Scholar
Herbert, B.M., Herbert, C., Pollatos, O., et al. (2012a). Effects of short-term food deprivation on interoceptive awareness, feelings and autonomic cardiac activity. Biological Psychology, 89, 7179.Google Scholar
Herbert, B.M., Muth, E.R., Pollatos, O., et al. (2012b). Interoception across modalities: On the relationship between cardiac awareness and the sensitivity for gastric functions. PLoS One, 7, e36646.Google Scholar
Herbert, B.M. and Pollatos, O. (2012). The body in the mind: On the relationship between interoception and embodiment. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4, 692704.Google Scholar
Herbert, B.M., Pollatos, O., Flor, H., et al. (2010). Cardiac awareness and autonomic cardiac reactivity during emotional picture viewing and mental stress. Psychophysiology, 47, 342354.Google Scholar
Heydrich, L., Dodds, T.J., Aspell, J.E., et al. (2013). Visual capture and the experience of having two bodies – evidence from two different virtual reality techniques. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 946.Google Scholar
Hogeveen, J., Bird, G., Chau, A., et al. (2016). Acquired alexithymia following damage to the anterior insula. Neuropsychologia, 82, 142148.Google Scholar
James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, 188205.Google Scholar
Liang, C., Chang, S.-Y., Chen, W.-Y., et al. (2015). Body ownership and experiential ownership in the self-touching illusion. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, e1591.Google Scholar
Longarzo, M., D’Olimpio, F., Chiavazzo, A., et al. (2015). The relationship between interoception and alexithymic trait. The Self-Awareness Questionnaire in healthy subjects. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, e1149.Google Scholar
Longo, M.R., Schüür, F., Kammers, M.P.M., et al. (2009). Self awareness and the body image. Acta Psychologica, 132, 166172.Google Scholar
Matthias, E., Schandry, R., Duschek, S., et al. (2007). Interoceptive awareness modulates attentional processing of visual stimuli. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72, 154159.Google Scholar
Mehling, W.E., Gopisetty, V., Daubenmier, J., et al. (2009). Body awareness: Construct and self-report measures. PLoS One, 4, e5614.Google Scholar
Mehling, W.E., Wrubel, J., Daubenmier, J.J., et al. (2011). Body awareness: A phenomenological inquiry into the common ground of mind-body therapies. Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine, 6, 6.Google Scholar
Murphy, J., Brewer, R., Catmur, C., and Bird, G. (2017). Interoception and psychopathology: A developmental neuroscience perspective. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 4556.Google Scholar
Niedenthal, P.M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science, 316, 10021005.Google Scholar
Niedenthal, P.M., Barsalou, L.W., Winkielman, P., et al. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184211.Google Scholar
Palmer, C.J., Paton, B., Hohwy, J., et al. (2013). Movement under uncertainty: The effects of the rubber-hand illusion vary along the nonclinical autism spectrum. Neuropsychologia, 51, 19421951.Google Scholar
Paul, T. and Thiel, A. (2005). Eating Disorder Inventory – 2, Deutsche Version. Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Petersen, S., Van Staeyen, K., Vögele, C., et al. (2015). Interoception and symptom reporting: Disentangling accuracy and bias. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 732.Google Scholar
Phan, K.L., Wager, T., Taylor, S.F., et al. (2002). Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: A meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. NeuroImage, 16, 331348.Google Scholar
Pollatos, O. and Georgiou, E. (2016). Normal interoceptive accuracy in women with bulimia nervosa. Psychiatry Research, 240, 328332.Google Scholar
Pollatos, O., Gramann, K., and Schandry, R. (2007a). Neural systems connecting interoceptive awareness and feelings. Human Brain Mapping, 28, 918.Google Scholar
Pollatos, O., Herbert, B.M., Berberich, G., et al. (2016a). Atypical self-focus effect on interoceptive accuracy in anorexia nervosa. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 484.Google Scholar
Pollatos, O., Herbert, B.M., Kaufmann, C., et al. (2007b). Interoceptive awareness, anxiety and cardiovascular reactivity to isometric exercise. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 65, 167173.Google Scholar
Pollatos, O., Herbert, B.M., Mai, S., et al. (2016b). Changes in interoceptive processes following brain stimulation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371, 1708.Google Scholar
Pollatos, O., Herbert, B.M., Wankner, S., et al. (2011). Autonomic imbalance is associated with reduced facial recognition in somatoform disorders. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 71, 232239.Google Scholar
Pollatos, O., Kirsch, W., and Schandry, R. (2005a). Brain structures involved in interoceptive awareness and cardioafferent signal processing: A dipole source localization study. Human Brain Mapping, 26, 5464.Google Scholar
Pollatos, O., Kirsch, W., and Schandry, R. (2005b). On the relationship between interoceptive awareness, emotional experience, and brain processes. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 948962.Google Scholar
Pollatos, O., Kurz, A.L., Albrecht, J., et al. (2008a). Reduced perception of bodily signals in anorexia nervosa. Eating Behaviors, 9, 381388.Google Scholar
Pollatos, O. and Schandry, R. (2004). Accuracy of heartbeat perception is reflected in the amplitude of the heartbeat-evoked brain potential. Psychophysiology, 41, 476482.Google Scholar
Pollatos, O., Schandry, R., Auer, D.P., et al. (2007c). Brain structures mediating cardiovascular arousal and interoceptive awareness. Brain Research, 1141, 178187.Google Scholar
Pollatos, O., Schubö, A., Herbert, B.M., et al. (2008b). Deficits in early emotional reactivity in alexithymia. Psychophysiology, 45, 839846.Google Scholar
Porges, S. (1993). Body Perception Questionnaire. Laboratory of Development Assessment, University of Maryland.Google Scholar
Preuschoff, K., Quartz, S.R., and Bossaerts, P. (2008). Human insula activation reflects risk prediction errors as well as risk. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 27452752.Google Scholar
Quattrocki, E. and Friston, K. (2014). Autism, oxytocin and interoception. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, 410430.Google Scholar
Ronchi, R., Bello-Ruiz, J., Lukowska, M., et al. (2015). Right insular damage decreases heartbeat awareness and alters cardio-visual effects on bodily self-consciousness. Neuropsychologia, 70, 1120.Google Scholar
Schandry, R. (1981). Heart beat perception and emotional experience. Psychophysiology, 18, 483488Google Scholar
Schauder, K.B., Mash, L.E., Bryant, L.K., et al. (2015). Interoceptive ability and body awareness in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 131, 193200.Google Scholar
Seth, A.K., Suzuki, K., and Critchley, H.D. (2012). An interoceptive predictive coding model of conscious presence. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 395.Google Scholar
Shah, P., Hall, R., Catmur, C., et al. (2016). Alexithymia, not autism, is associated with impaired interoception. Cortex, 81, 215220.Google Scholar
Simmons, W.K., Avery, J.A., Barcalow, J.C., et al. (2013). Keeping the body in mind: Insula functional organization and functional connectivity integrate interoceptive, exteroceptive, and emotional awareness. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 29442958.Google Scholar
Smith, R. and Lane, R.D. (2015). The neural basis of one’s own conscious and unconscious emotional states. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 57, 129.Google Scholar
Suzuki, K., Garfinkel, S.N., Critchley, H.D., et al. (2013). Multisensory integration across exteroceptive and interoceptive domains modulates self-experience in the rubber-hand illusion. Neuropsychologia, 51, 29092917.Google Scholar
Taylor, G.J. (1984). Alexithymia: Concept, measurement, and implications for treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 725732.Google Scholar
Taylor, G.J., Bagby, R.M., and Parker, J.D.A. (2016). What’s in the name “alexithymia”? A commentary on “Affective agnosia: Expansion of the alexithymia construct and a new opportunity to integrate and extend Freud’s legacy.” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 68, 10061020.Google Scholar
Terasawa, Y., Fukushima, H., and Umeda, S. (2013). How does interoceptive awareness interact with the subjective experience of emotion? An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 598612.Google Scholar
Terasawa, Y., Kurosaki, Y., Ibata, Y., et al. (2015). Attenuated sensitivity to the emotions of others by insular lesion. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1314.Google Scholar
Tsakiris, M. (2010). My body in the brain: A neurocognitive model of body-ownership. Neuropsychologia, 48, 703712.Google Scholar
Tsakiris, M. (2017). The multisensory basis of the self: From body to identity to others. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 70, 597609.Google Scholar
Tsakiris, M., Hesse, M.D., Boy, C., et al. (2006). Neural signatures of body ownership: A sensory network for bodily self-consciousness. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 22352244.Google Scholar
Tsakiris, M., Jimenez, A.T., and Costantini, M. (2011). Just a heartbeat away from one’s body: Interoceptive sensitivity predicts malleability of body-representations. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278, 24702476.Google Scholar
Wiens, S. (2005). Interoception in emotional experience. Current Opinion in Neurology, 18, 442447.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×