Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-22dnz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T09:42:56.696Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 6 - Acquired language disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2014

Louise Cummings
Affiliation:
Nottingham Trent University
Get access

Summary

Adults can sustain injuries and illnesses which cause disruption of previously intact language skills. Aphasia is the most prominent language disorder which is acquired in adulthood. Typically, it is caused by damage to the language centres in the left hemisphere of the brain following a cerebrovascular accident or stroke. Less common causes of aphasia are traumatic brain injury, brain tumours and cerebral infections. In rare cases, aphasia may result from damage to the right hemisphere of the brain. Aphasias are classified as fluent and non-fluent types. In fluent aphasia, there is effortless spoken output which exhibits the normal suprasegmental features of speech. However, this output often contains copious amounts of jargon (hence, the term ‘jargon aphasia’) in which neologisms are present. The client with fluent aphasia also has a severe auditory comprehension deficit and displays limited awareness of their communication problems. In non-fluent aphasia, there is reduced, effortful spoken output. The client is aware of, and is often distressed by, their problems with spoken language. Grammatical structure is markedly reduced with content words (e.g. nouns) retained, while function words (e.g. articles) are omitted. This gives speech the appearance of a telegram (hence, the use of the term ‘agrammatic aphasia’ to describe this type of aphasia).

Damage to the right hemisphere of the brain can cause a different type of language disorder in adults. In so-called right-hemisphere language disorder, structural language skills are largely intact while there are marked impairments of pragmatic and discourse skills. These impairments include the concrete, literal interpretation of figurative language, egocentric discourse which relates the speaker's personal experience, neglect of the hearer's perspective in formulating utterances, and problems with topic management and narrative structure. Many of these communication difficulties appear to be related to cognitive deficits such as executive dysfunction.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2014

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bastiaanse, R. and Prins, R. S. 2014. ‘Aphasia’, in Cummings, L. (ed.), Cambridge handbook of communication disorders, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 224–46.Google Scholar
Cummings, L. 2008. Clinical linguistics, Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press (section 5.4).Google Scholar
Cummings, L. 2014a. Communication disorders, Houndmills: Palgrave Macmillan (chapter 5).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joanette, Y., Ferré, P. and Wilson, M. A. 2014. ‘Right hemisphere damage and communication’, in Cummings, L. (ed.), Cambridge handbook of communication disorders, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 247–65.Google Scholar
Le, K., Mozeiko, J. and Coelho, C. 2010. ‘Traumatic brain injury and discourse’, in Cummings, L. (ed.), Routledge pragmatics encyclopedia, London and New York: Routledge, 475–8.Google Scholar
Maxim, J. and Bryan, K. 2006. ‘Language, communication and cognition in the dementias’, in Bryan, K. and Maxim, J. (eds.), Communication disability in the dementias, Chichester: Whurr, 73–124.Google Scholar
Monetta, L. and Champagne-Lavau, M. 2010. ‘Right-hemisphere damage and pragmatics’, in Cummings, L. (ed.), Routledge pragmatics encyclopedia, London and New York: Routledge, 408–9.Google Scholar
Orange, J. B. and Williams, L. J. 2010. ‘Dementia and conversation’, in Cummings, L. (ed.), Routledge pragmatics encyclopedia, London and New York: Routledge, 105–8.Google Scholar
Reilly, J. and Hung, J. 2014. ‘Dementia and communication’, in Cummings, L. (ed.), Cambridge handbook of communication disorders, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 266–83.Google Scholar
Togher, L. 2014. ‘Traumatic brain injury and communication’, in Cummings, L. (ed.), Cambridge handbook of communication disorders, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 284–99.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×