Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-r5zm4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-16T08:21:55.153Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Language contact as a possible trigger of change

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 December 2017

Jurgen Meisel
Affiliation:
University of Hamburg, Germany and University of Calgary, Canada
Martin Elsig
Affiliation:
Goethe-University Frankfurt, Germany
Esther Rinke
Affiliation:
Goethe-University Frankfurt, Germany
Get access

Summary

The discussion in the previous chapter has cast doubt on the plausibility of a scenario according to which structural ambiguity, in the sense of P-ambiguity, can be held responsible for grammatical restructuring in the process of L1 acquisition. In this chapter, we will examine whether language contact is a more likely candidate to induce grammatical change. Our focus is, again, on phenomena related to morphosyntax.

The chapter is introduced by Section 5.1, where we will discuss general assumptions concerning contact-induced diachronic change, focusing on the concepts of structural borrowing, convergence and shift-induced interference. Whereas many scholars assume a rather sceptical view with respect to cross-linguistic influence at the level of the grammatical system (Meillet 1921, 1938; Sapir 1927; Weinreich 1979), Thomason and Kaufman (1988: 14) state that ‘any linguistic feature can be transferred from any language to any other language’. They propose two main mechanisms, namely structural borrowing and shift-induced interference, which we will discuss in more detail. We will argue that direct structural borrowing is a problematic concept because in the relevant contexts where it has been proposed that this happens, alternative explanations are equally plausible. On the one hand, it has been proposed that structural similarity between contact languages may arise not only as a consequence of structural borrowing, but also due to universal processes of grammaticalization which operate on superficially similar or equivalent constructions (Heine and Kuteva 2003). On the other hand, as proposed by Silva-Corvalan (1998), language contact can lead to changes of an already existing form within a language, either with regard to its frequency of use or concerning its linguistic and pragmatic contexts of occurrence. In view of these alternative mechanisms which may lead to the same surface effects, it is far from being obvious how structural borrowing could possibly be identified and whether it is at all possible to do so (see also Poplack and Levey 2010). The second mechanism mentioned above, shift-induced interference, however, which consists in changes brought about by an imperfect learning of a language by a group of speakers with a different native language background (cf. also Thomason 2001), provides a more plausible scenario of contactinduced interference and has been discussed as a possible causal factor of language change in generative research (see e.g. Kroch and Taylor 1997; Weerman 1993; Meisel 2011b); we will return to this issue in the following chapter (Section 6.2).

Type
Chapter
Information
Language Acquisition and Change
A Morphosyntactic Perspective
, pp. 96 - 136
Publisher: Edinburgh University Press
Print publication year: 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×