Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-g5fl4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-30T22:11:01.429Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Christina E. Erneling
Affiliation:
Lunds Universitet, Sweden
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Towards Discursive Education
Philosophy, Technology, and Modern Education
, pp. 183 - 194
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Antaki, C. 2004. Reading minds or dealing with interactional implications?Theory & Psychology, 14 (5), 667–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A. 2005. From monkey-like action recognition to human language: an evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28 (2), 105–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Armstrong, A. and Casement, C.. 1998. The Child and the Machine: Why Computers May Put Our Children's Education at Risk. Beltsville, MD: Robins Lane Press.Google Scholar
Astington, J. W. and Baird, J. A. (eds.). 2005. Why Language Matters for Theory of Mind. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
Bailey, R. 2000. Education in the Open Society: Karl Popper and Schooling. Aldershot: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Baldwin, D. 2000. Interpersonal understanding fuels knowledge acquisition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9 (2), 40–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. 1995. Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Berkson, W. and Wetterstein, J.. 1984. Learning from Error: Karl Popper's Psychology of Learning. La Salle, IL: Open Court.Google Scholar
Berliner, D. and Calfee, R.. 1996. Handbook of Educational Psychology. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Beth, E. W., Mays, W., and Piaget, J.. 1957. Epistémologie génétique et recherche psychologique. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Bijeljac-Babic, B. J., Bertocini, J. and Mehler, J.. 1993. How do 4-day-old infants categorize multisyllabic utterances?Developmental Psychology, 29 (4), 711–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binder, C. 1993. Behavioral fluency: a new paradigm. Educational Technology, October, 8–14.Google Scholar
Blaffer Hrdy, S. 2009. Mothers and Others: Evolutionary Origins of Mutual Understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. 2004. Descartes' Baby: How the Science of Child Development Explains What Makes Us Human. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bloor, D. 1983. Wittgenstein: A Social Theory of Knowledge. New York: Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowd, A., McDougall, D. and Yewchuck, C.. 1997. Educational Psychology for Canadian Teachers. Toronto: Harcourt and Brace.Google Scholar
Bower, T. G. R. 1977. The Perceptual World of the Child. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bringuier, J-C. 1980. Conversations with Jean Piaget. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. 1971. The Relevance of Education. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. 1976. Peek-a-boo and the learning of rule structures. In Bruner, J., Jolly, A., and Sylvia, K. (eds.), Play: Its Role in Development and Evolution. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. 1983. Child's Talk: Learning to Use Language. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. 1990. Acts of Meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. 1996. The Culture of Education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. 2004. A short history of psychological theories of learning. Daedalus, Winter, 13–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burbules, N. and Callister, Jr T.., 2000. Watch IT. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. 1999. Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (ed.). 2005. Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Campbell, R. J. 1998. Hyperminds for hyper times: the demise of rational, logical thought?Educational Technology, January–February, 24–31.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P. and Smith, P. (eds.). 1996. Theories of Theories of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Changeux, J-P. 1985. Neuronal Man. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Chapman, M. 1988. Constructive Evolution: Origins and Development of Piaget's Thought. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Chiou, G-F. 1992. Situated learning, metaphors and computer-based learning environments. Educational Technology, August, 7–11.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1959. Review of B. F. Skinner's ‘Verbal Behavior’. Language, 35, 26–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1968. Language and Mind. New York: Harcourt, Brace, and World.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1975. Reflections on Language. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1980. Rules and Representations. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1997a. Language and cognition. In Johnson, D. M. and Erneling, C. E. (eds.), The Future of the Cognitive Revolution. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1997b. Language from an internalist perspective. In Johnson, D. M. and Erneling, C. E. (eds.), Reassessing the Cognitive Revolution: Alternative Futures. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cook, D. 1993. Behaviorism evolves. Educational Technology, October, 62–77.Google Scholar
Cooper, P. 1993. Paradigm shifts in designed instruction: from behaviorism to cognitivism to constructivism. Educational Technology, May, 12–19.Google Scholar
Costall, A. and Leuder, I.. 2004. Where is the ‘theory’ in the theory of mind?Theory & Psychology, 14 (5), 623–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crook, C. 1998. Children as computer users: the case of collaborative learning. Computer and Education, 30 (3), 237–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuban, L. 1986. Teachers and Machines: The Classroom Use of Technology Since 1920. New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
Cuban, L. 2001. Oversold and Underused: Computers in the Classroom. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Danziger, K. 1985. The methodological imperative in psychology. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 15, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. 1877/1974. A biographical sketch of an infant. In Gruber, H., Darwin on Man: A Psychological Study of Scientific Creativity. London: Wildwood House.Google Scholar
Boysson-Bardies, B., Vihman, M., Roug-Hellichius, L., Durand, C., Landberg, I., and Arao, F., 1993. Material evidence of infant selection from target language: a cross-linguistic study. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L., and Stoel-Gammon, C. (eds.), Phonological Development: Models, Research, Implications. Parkton, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Corte, E. 1990. Learning with new information technologies in schools: perspectives from the psychology of learning and instruction. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 6 (2), 69–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewey, J. 1916/1966. Democracy and Education. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. 1990. The School and Society: The Child and the Curriculum. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Dilthey, W. 1914–36/1985. Introduction to the Human Sciences: Wilhelm Dilthey. Selected Works. Vol. 1. Trans. Makkreel, R. A. and Rodi, F., Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Doherty, M. 2008. Theory of Mind: How Children Understand Others' Thoughts and Feelings. Howe, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Donald, M. 1991. Origins of the Modern Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. 1978. Children's Minds. London: Fontana.Google Scholar
Durkheim, E. 1922/1956. Education and Sociology. Trans. Fox, Sherwood D.. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.Google Scholar
Eimas, P. D. 1985. The perception of speech in early infancy. Scientific American, January, 46–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elman, J., Bates, E., Johnson, M., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., and Plunkett, K.. 1996. Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Erneling, C. E. 1993. Understanding Language Acquisition: The Framework of Learning. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Erneling, C. E. 1997. Afterword. In Johnson, D. M. and Ernling, C. E. (eds.), The Future of the Cognitive Revolution. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Erneling, C. E. 2007. The primacy of social interaction. American Journal of Psychology, 120 (2), 334–8.Google Scholar
Erneling, C. E. in press. The limits of mindreading. Philosophy of the Social Sciences.
Erneling, C. E. and Johnson, D. M. (eds.). 2005. The Mind as a Scientific Object: Between Brain and Culture. New York: Oxford University Press.
Evans, C. 1979. The Mighty Micro: The Impact of the Computer Revolution. London: Victor Gollancz.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. 1975/1979. The Language of Thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. 1980. Methodological solipsism considered as a research strategy in cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 63–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. 1983. The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. 1987. Psychosemantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fuller, S. 2003. Popper vs. Kuhn: The Struggle for the Soul of Science. London: Icon Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. 1983. Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. 1985. The Mind's New Science. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. 2004. What we do and don't know about learning. Daedalus, Winter, 5–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geary, D. C. 1995. Reflections on evolution and culture in children's cognition: implications for mathematical development and instruction. American Psychologist, 50 (1), 24–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gellner, E. 1985. Relativism and the Social Sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gervain, J. and Werker, J.. 2008. How infant speech perception contributes to language acquisition. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2 (6), 1149–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goffman, E. 1974. Frame Analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goodman, N. 1972. Problems and Projects. Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merill.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. 2004. Finding our inner scientist. Daedalus, Winter, 21–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. 2009. The Philosophical Baby: What Children's Minds Tell Us About Truth, Love, and the Meaning of Life. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. and Meltzoff, A. N.. 1997. Words, Thoughts, and Theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N., and Kuhl, P., 1999. The Scientist in the Crib: Minds, Brains, and How Children Learn. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
Gray, H. 1978. Learning to take an object from the mother. In Lock, A. (ed.), Action, Gesture, and Symbol: The Emergence of Language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Greenspan, S. and Shanker, S.. 2004. The First Idea: How Symbols, Language, and Intelligence Evolved From Our Early Primate Ancestors to Modern Humans. Cambridge, MA: Da Capo Press.Google Scholar
Hacking, I. 2002. Historical Ontology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamlyn, D. W. 1978. Experience and the Growth of Understanding. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Harré, R. 1997. ‘Berkeleyan’ arguments and the ontology of cognitive science. In Johnson, D. M. and Erneling, C. E. (eds.), The Future of the Cognitive Revolution. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Harré, R. 2000. The rediscovery of the human mind. http://www.massey.ac.nz/xxxAlock/virtual/korea%206/26/00.
Harré, R. 2001. Norms in life: problems in the representation of rules. In Bakhurst, D. and Shanker, S. (eds.), Jerome Bruner: Language, Culture, Self. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Harré, R. 2002. Cognitive Psychology: A Philosophical Introduction. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Harré, R. and Gillett, G.. 1994. The Discursive Mind. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Harré, R. and Moghaddam, F. M. (eds.). 2003. The Self and Others: Positioning Individuals and Groups in Personal, Political and Cultural Contexts. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Harré, R. and Stearns, P. (eds.). 1995. Discursive Psychology in Practice. London: Sage.
Harré, R. and Langehove, L. (eds.). 1999. Positioning Theory. Oxford: Blackwell.
Hauser, M. 2000. Wild Minds: What Animals Really Think. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Hazzan, O. 1999. Information technologies and objects to learn with. Educational Technology, May–June, 55–9.Google Scholar
Hergenhan, B. R. and Olson, M. H.. 1997. An Introduction to Theories of Learning. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Hill, M. 1992. The new literacy beyond the three R's. Electronic Learning, September, 28–34.Google Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. 1997. Language Development. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
Hudspeth, D. 1992. Just-in-time education. Educational Technology, June, 7–11.Google Scholar
Hurford, J. R. 2007. The Origins of Meaning: Language in the Light of Evolution. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D. 2008. Folk Psychological Narratives: The Sociocultural Basis of Understanding Reasons. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jarvie, I. C. 2005. Workshop rationality, dogmatism, and models of mind. In Erneling, C. E. and Johnson, D. M. (eds.), The Mind as a Scientific Object: Between Brain and Culture. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, D. M. and Erneling, C. E. (eds.). 1997. The Future of the Cognitive Revolution. New York: Oxford University Press.
Johnson, J. K. 1992. Advancing by the degrees: trends in master's and doctoral programs in educational communication technology. Tech Trends, 37 (2), 13–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jonassen, D. H. (ed.). 1996. Handbook of Research for Educational Communications and Technology. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Jonassen, D. H., Carr, C., and Yueh, H-P.. 1998. Computers as mindtools for engaging learners in critical thinking. Tech Trends, March, 24–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juszcyk, P. W. and Derrah, C.. 1987. Representation of speech sounds by young infants. Developmental Psychology, 23, 648–54.Google Scholar
Juszcyk, P. W., Friederici, A. D., Wessels, J. M., Svenkerud, V. Y., and Juszcyk, A. M.. 1993. Infants' sensitivity to the sound pattern of native language words. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 402–20.Google Scholar
Kafai, Y. 1995. Minds in Play: Computer Game Design as a Context for Children's Learning. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. 1998. Three Seductive Ideas. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kearsky, G. 1998. Educational technology: a critique. Educational Technology, March–April, 47–51.Google Scholar
Kimbrough Oller, D. 2000. The Emergence of Speech Capacity. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Konner, M. J. 1972. Aspects of the developmental ethology of a foraging people. In Blurton Jones, N. J. (ed.). Ethological Studies of Child Behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. 2004. Early language acquisition: cracking the code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5 (11), 831–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. 1962. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Leggett, W. P. and Persichitte, K. A.. 1998. Blood, sweat and TEARS: 50 years of technology implementation obstacles. Tech Trends, April, 33–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leudar, I. and Costall, A.. 2004. On persistence of the ‘problem of “other minds”’ in psychology: Chomsky, Grice and theory of mind. Theory & Psychology, 14 (5), 601–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leudar, I., Costall, A., and Francis, D.. 2004. Theory of mind: a critical assessment. Theory & Psychology, 14 (5), 571–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, P. 1984. The Biology and Evolution of Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. 1985. On the evolution of human syntactic ability: its preadaptive bases – motor control and speech. Journal of Human Evolution, 14, 657–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, P. and Blumstein, S. E.. 1988. Speech Physiology, Speech Perception, and Acoustic Phonetics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linard, M. 1995. New debates on learning support. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 11 (4) 239–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lock, A. (ed.). 1978. Action, Gesture, and Symbol: The Emergence of Language. New York: Academic Press.
Macnamara, J. 1976. Stomachs assimilate and accommodate, don't they?Canadian Psychological Review, 17, 167–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F. 2008. The Origin of Speech. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Malle, B. F. 2004. How the Mind Explains Behavior: Folk Explanations, Meaning, and Social Interaction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Malle, B. F. and Hodges, S.. 2005. Other Minds: How Humans Bridge the Divide Between Self and Others. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mandelbaum, M. 1971. History, Man, and Reason: A Study in Nineteenth-Century Thought. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Mayer, R. E., Schustack, M. W. and Blanton, W. E.. 1999. What do children learn from using computers in an informal, collaborative setting?Educational Technology, 39, 27–31.Google Scholar
McCown, R. R. 1999. Educational Psychology: A Learning-Centred Approach to Classroom Practice. Scarborough, ONT: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
McGrath, B. 1998. Partners in learning: twelve ways technology changes the teacher–student relationship. THE Journal, 25, 58–61.Google Scholar
McLellan, H. 1994. Situated learning: continuing the conversation. Educational Technology, October, 7–8.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. and Prinz, W. (eds.). 2002. The Imitative Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Menn, L. and Stoel-Gammon, C.. 1993. Phonological development: learning sounds and sound patterns. In Gleason, J. B. (ed.), The Development of Language. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Merrill, P., Hammons, K., Tolman, M., Cristensen, L., Vincent, B. and Reynolds, P.. 1992. Computers in Education. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Messerly, J. G. 1996. Piaget's Conception of Evolution: Beyond Darwin and Lamarck. Lanham, MD: Rowman and Littlefield.Google Scholar
Moghaddam, F. M. 2002. The Individual and Society: A Cultural Integration. New York: Worth.Google Scholar
Morss, J. R. 1990. The Biologising of Childhood: Developmental Psychology and the Darwinian Myth. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Munz, P. 2004. Beyond Wittgenstein's Poker: New Light on Wittgenstein and Popper. Burlington, VT: AshgateGoogle Scholar
Nagel, T. 1995. Other Minds: Critical Essays 1969–1994. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Neisser, U. 1988. Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philosophical Psychology, 1 (1), 35–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, G. 1998. Internet/web-based instruction and multiple intelligence. Educational Media International, 35 (2), 90–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newson, J. 1978. Dialogue and development. In Lock, A. (ed.), Action, Gesture, and Symbol: The Emergence of Language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Olson, D. R. 1994. The World on Paper. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Olson, D. R. 2003. Psychological Theory and Educational Reform. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Palermo, D. S. 1971. Is a scientific revolution taking place in psychology?Science Studies, 1, 135–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papert, S. 1980/1993. Mindstorms: Children, Computers, and Powerful Ideas. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Papert, S. 1993. The Children's Machine: Rethinking School in the Age of the Computer. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1926/1955. The Language and Thought of the Child. New York: Meridian Books.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1928/1976. Judgement and Reasoning in the Child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. 1936/1963. The Origins of Intelligence in Children. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1937/1954. The Construction of Reality in the Child. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1945/1962. Play, Dreams, and Imagination in Childhood. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1947/1976. The Psychology of Intelligence. Totowa, NJ: Littlefield.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. 1948/1973. To Understand is to Invent: The Future of Education. New York: Grossman.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1950/1965/1995. Sociological Studies. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1964/1968. Six Psychological Studies. New York: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1964/1993. Development and learning. In Gauvin, M. and Cole, M. (eds.), Readings in the Development of Children. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1967/1971. Biology and Knowledge. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1969/1970. The Science of Education and the Psychology of the Child. New York: Grossman.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1970. Genetic Epistemology. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1970/1971. Psychology and Genetic Epistemology. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1977/1978. The Development of Thought. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1980. The psychogenesis of knowledge and its epistemological significance. In Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (ed.), Language and Learning: The Debate Between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. and Beth, E. W.. 1961/1966. Mathematical Epistemology and Psychology. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (ed.). 1980. Language and Learning: The Debate Between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Pinker, S. 1984. Language Learnability and Language Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. 1997. How the Mind Works. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. 2002. The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. 2007. The Stuff of Thought: Language as a Window into Human Nature. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Plato, . 1974. The Republic. Trans. Grube, G. M. A.. Indianapolis: Hackett.Google Scholar
Plato, . 1981. Plato: Five Dialogues. Trans. Grube, G. M. A.. Indianapolis: Hackett.Google Scholar
Popper, K. 1957. The Poverty of Historicism. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Popper, K. 1963. Back to the Pre-Socratics. In Conjectures and Refutations: The Growth of Scientific Knowledge. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Popper, K. 1972. Objective Knowledge: An Evolutionary Approach. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Postman, N. 1996. The End of Education: Redefining the Value of School. New York: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Postman, N. 1999. Building a Bridge to the Eighteenth Century. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
Premack, D. 1976. Intelligence in Ape and Man. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Prensky, M. 2001. Digital Game-Based Learning. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Putnam, H. 1981. Reason, Truth, and History. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reddy, V. and Morris, P.. 2004. Participants don't need theories: knowing minds in engagement. Theory & Psychology, 14 (5), 647–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Relan, A. and Gillini, B. B.. 1997. Web-based instruction and the traditional classroom: similarities and differences. In Khan, B. H. (ed.), Web-Based Instruction. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Educational Technologies.Google Scholar
Rich Harris, J. 1998. The Nurture Assumption: Why Children Turn Out the Way They Do. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Richards, M. (ed.). 1974. The Integration of a Child into a Social World. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Rizzolatti, R., Fogassi, L. and Gallese, V.. 2001. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 661–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, D. 1998. The New Renaissance: Computers and the Next Level of Civilization. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B. 1990. Apprenticeship in Thinking. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B. 2003. The Culture of Human Development. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Romiszowski, A. J. 1997. Web-based distance learning and teaching: revolutionary innovation or reaction to necessity. In Khan, B. H. (ed.), Web-Based Instruction. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Educational Technologies.Google Scholar
Rose, E. 1998. Taking Turing: how the imitation game plays out in the classroom. Educational Technology, May–June, 56–61.Google Scholar
Rose, E. 1999. Deconstructing interactivity in educational computing. Educational Technology, January–February, 43–9.Google Scholar
Roszak, T. 1986. The Cult of Information: The Folklore of Computers and the True Art of Thinking. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Rotman, B. 1977. Jean Piaget: Psychologist of the Real. Hassocks: Harvester Press.Google Scholar
Russell, J. 1978. The Acquisition of Knowledge. London: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, J. 1974. Early language development: towards a communicational analysis. In Richards, M. (ed.), The Integration of a Child into a Social World. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ryle, G. 1949. The Concept of Mind. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Ryle, G. 1967. Teaching and training. In Peters, R. S. (ed.), The Concept of Education. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Sachs, J. 1993. The emergence of intentional communication. In Gleason, J. B. (ed.), The Development of Language. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C. and Cleary, C.. 1995. Engines for Education. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schoenfeld, W. 1993. The necessity of ‘behaviorism’. Educational Technology, October, 5–7.Google Scholar
Scott Gordon, H. 1991. The History and Philosophy of the Social Sciences. New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. 1983. Intentionality: An Essay in the Philosophy of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shanker, S. 2004. The roots of mindblindness. Theory & Psychology, 14 (5), 685–703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shanker, S. and Taylor, T.. 2005. The significance of ape language research. In Erneling, C. E. and Johnson, D. M. (eds.), The Mind as a Scientific Object: Between Brain and Culture. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sharrock, W. and Coulter, J.. 2004. ToM: a critical commentary. Theory & Psychology, 14 (5), 579–600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shotter, J. 1974. The development of personal powers. In Richards, M. (ed.), The Integration of a Child into a Social World. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Shotter, J. 1976. Acquired powers: the transformation of the natural into social powers. In Harré, R. (ed.), Personality. Totowa, NJ: Rowman and Littlefield.Google Scholar
Shute, V. J. and Gawlick-Grendell, L. A.. 1994. What does the computer contribute to learning?Computers in Education, 23 (3), 177–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinclair, H. 1971. Sensorimotor action pattern as a condition for the acquisition of syntax. In Ingram, E. and Huxley, R. (eds.), Language Acquisition: Models and Methods. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sinnott, J. M., Pison, D. P., and Askin, R. N.. 1983. A comparison of pure auditory thresholds in human infants and adults. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 3–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. 1974. About Behaviorism. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Sykes, W. and Reid, R.. 1999. Virtual reality in schools: the ultimate educational technology. THE Journal, 26, 61–3.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. 1999. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. 2000. Culture and cognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 37–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. 2008. Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Toulmin, S. 1961. Foresight and Understanding: An Enquiry into the Aims of Science. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Gelder, T. 2005. Beyond the mind–body problem. In Erneling, C. E. and Johnson, D. M. (eds.), The Mind as a Scientific Object: Between Brain and Culture. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Vargas, E. 1993. From behaviorism to selectionism. Educational Technology, October, 46–51.Google Scholar
Vasta, R., Haith, M., and Miller, S.. 1999. Child Psychology: The Modern Science. Second edition. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Veenema, S. and Gardner, H.. 1996. Multimedia and multiple intelligences. American Prospect, 27, 69–75.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. 1934/1994. Thought and Language. Rev. and ed. Kozulin, A.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. 1978. Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Eds. Cole, M., John-Steiner, V., Scriber, S., and Souberman, E.. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Walker, S. 1987. The evolution and dissolution of language. In Ellis, A. W. (ed.), Progress in Psychology of Language. Vol. 3. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wallace, P. 1999. The Psychology of the Internet. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wanner, E. and Gleitman, L. R.. 1982. Language Acquisition: The State of the Art. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wells, G. 1999. Dialogic Inquiry: Towards a Sociocultural Practice and Theory of Education. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. F. and Tees, R. C.. 1984. Cross-linguistic speech perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 49–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westera, W. 1999. Paradoxes in open, networked learning environments: towards a paradigm shift. Educational Technology, January–February, 17–23.Google Scholar
Wexler, B. E. 2006. Brain and Culture: Neurobiology, Ideology, and Social Change. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Williams, E. 2004. Who really needs a ‘theory of mind’? An interpretative phenomenological analysis of the autobiographical writings of ten high-functioning individuals with an autism spectrum disorder. Theory & Psychology, 14 (5), 704–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winn, W. 1993. Instructional design and situated learning: paradox or partnership?Educational Technology, March, 16–21.Google Scholar
Winn, W. and Jackson, R.. 1999. Fourteen propositions about educational uses of virtual reality. Educational Technology, July–August, 5–14.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. 1953. Philosophical Investigations. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. 1961. Tractatus Logico-Philosophicus. New York: Humanities Press.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. 1916. Elements of Folk Psychology: Outlines of a Psychological History of the Development of Mankind. London: Allen & Unwin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yarusso, L. 1992. Constructivism vs. objectivism. Performance & Instruction, April, 7–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Christina E. Erneling, Lunds Universitet, Sweden
  • Book: Towards Discursive Education
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511761799.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Christina E. Erneling, Lunds Universitet, Sweden
  • Book: Towards Discursive Education
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511761799.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Christina E. Erneling, Lunds Universitet, Sweden
  • Book: Towards Discursive Education
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511761799.010
Available formats
×