Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-wq484 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T11:22:45.991Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Associative modulation of US processing: implications for understanding of habituation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2011

Nestor Schmajuk
Affiliation:
Duke University Medical Center, Durham
Get access

Summary

Abstract

Considerable data from Pavlovian conditioning indicate that events that are associatively signaled by discrete cues are not as effectively processed as they otherwise would be. Extrapolating from early evidence of this phenomenon, especially so-called conditioned diminution of the unconditioned response (CDUR), Wagner (1976, 1979) suggested that a similar effect might be responsible for long-term habituation, as stimuli come to be “expected” in the context in which they have been exposed. In this paper we will reflect upon this reasoning in the light of more recent evidence from our laboratory and elsewhere. One major complication is that extended contexts (as well as discrete cues) can control response-potentiating, conditioned-emotional tendencies, in addition to the presumed decremental effects. Experiments that separate these effects will be exemplified, and one theoretical approach, through the models SOP and AESOP (Wagner, 1981; Wagner & Brandon, 1989) will be illustrated, with some implications for our further understanding of habituation.

In the late 1970s, Wagner (1976; 1979) presented some views about associative learning that were centered upon the notion that events that are already represented, or “primed” in active memory, are not as effectively processed as they otherwise would be. One of several forms of evidence for this supposition was the so-called conditioned diminution of the UR, a phenomenon originally reported by Kimble and Ost (1961), in the context of human eyeblink conditioning.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bombace, J. C., Brandon, S. E. & Wagner, A. R. (1991). Modulation of a conditioned eyeblink response by a putative emotive stimulus conditioned with hindleg shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, 323–332.Google ScholarPubMed
Borszcz, G. S., Cranney, J. & Leaton, R. N. (1989). Influence of long-term sensitization on long-term habituation of the acoustic startle response in rats: central gray lesions, preexposure, and extinction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 15, 54–64.Google ScholarPubMed
Brandon, S. E. & Wagner, A. R. (1991). Modulation of a discrete Pavlovian conditioning reflex by a putative emotive Pavlovian conditioned stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, 299–311.Google ScholarPubMed
Brandon, S. E. & Wagner, A. R. (1998). Occasion setting: influences of conditioned emotional responses and configural cues. In Schmajuk, N., ed., Occasion Setting: Associative Learning and Cognition in Animals. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association, pp. 343–382.Google Scholar
Brandon, S. E., Betts, S. L. & Wagner, A. R. (1994). Discriminated, lateralized eyeblink conditioning in the rabbit: an experimental context for separating specific and general associative influences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 292–307.Google ScholarPubMed
Brandon, S. E., Bombace, J. C., Falls, W. T. & Wagner, A. R. (1991). Modulation of unconditioned defensive reflexes via an emotive Pavlovian conditioned stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, 312–322.Google Scholar
Davis, M. (1992). The role of the amygdala in conditioned fear. In Aggleton, J., ed., The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspect of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction. New York: Wiley-Liss, pp. 255–305.Google Scholar
Davis, M. & Wagner, A. R. (1969). Habituation of startle response under incremental sequence of stimulus intensities. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 67(4), 486–492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donegan, N. H. (1981). Priming-produced facilitation or diminution of responding to a Pavlovian unconditioned stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 7, 295–312.Google ScholarPubMed
Gerwirtz, J. C., Brandon, S. E., Wagner, A. R. (1998). Modulation of the acquisition of the rabbit eye blink conditioned response by conditioned contextual stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 24, 106–117.
Groves, P. M. & Thompson, R. F. (1970). Habituation: a dual-process theory. Psychological Review, 77, 419–450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, G. (1991). Habituation. Perceptual and Associative Learning, 40, 29–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, G. & Channell, S. (1985). Differential-effects of contextual change on latent inhibition and on habituation of an orienting response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 470–481.Google Scholar
Hall, G. & Honey, R. C. (1989). Contextual effects in conditioning, latent inhibition, and habituation: associative and retrieval functions of contextual cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 15, 232–241.Google Scholar
Hinson, R. E. (1982). Effects of UCS preexposure on excitatory and inhibitory rabbit eyelid conditioning: an associative effect of conditioned contextual stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 8, 49–61.Google ScholarPubMed
Honey, R. C. & Good, M. (2000). Associative modulation of the orienting response: distinct effects revealed by hippocampal lesions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 26(1), 3–14.Google ScholarPubMed
Honey, R. C., Good, M. & Manser, K. L. (1998). Negative priming in associative learning: evidence form a serial-habituation procedure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 24(1), 229–237.Google Scholar
Jordan, W. P. & Leaton, R. N. (1982). The effects of mesencephalic reticular formation lesions on habituation of the startle and lick suppression responses in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 96, 170–183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jordan, W. P., Strasser, H. C. & McHale, L. (2000). Contextual control of long-term habituation in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 26, 323–339.Google ScholarPubMed
Kamin, L. J. (1968). “Attention-like” processes in classical conditioning. In Jones, M. R., ed., Symposium on the Prediction of Behavior: Aversive Stimulation. Miami, FL: University of Miami Press, pp. 9–31.Google Scholar
Kimble, G. A. & Ost, J. W. P. (1961). A conditioned inhibitory process in eyelid conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(2), 150–156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leaton, R. N. & Cranney, J. (1990) Potentiation of the acoustic startle response by a conditioned stimulus paired with acoustic startle stimulus in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 169–180.Google ScholarPubMed
Mackintosh, N. J. (1987). Neurobiology, psychology and habituation. Behavioral Research Theory, 25(2), 81–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marlin, N. A. & Miller, R. R. (1981). Associations to contextual stimuli as a determinant of long-term habituation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 7, 313–333.Google ScholarPubMed
Rankin, C. H. (2000). Context conditioning in habituation in the nematode, C. elegans. Behavioral Neuroscience, 114, 496–505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. & Solomon, R. L. (1967). Two-Process learning theory: relationship between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. Psychological Review, 74, 151–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and non reinforcement. In Black, A. H. and Proasky, W. F., eds., Classical Conditioning ii: Current Theory and Research. New York: Appleton–Century–Crofts, pp. 64–99.Google Scholar
Schneiderman, N. (1972). Response system divergencies in aversive classical conditioning. In Black, A. H. and Proasky, W. F., eds., Classical Conditioning ii: Current Theory and Research. New York: Appleton–Century–Crofts, pp. 313–376.Google Scholar
Siegel, S. (1975). Evidence from rats that morphine tolerance is a learned response. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 89, 498–506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, S., Hinson, E. E. & Krank, M. D. (1979). The role of predrug signals in morphine analgesic tolerance: support for a Pavlovian conditioning model of tolerance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 4, 188–196.Google Scholar
Terry, W. S. & Wagner, A. R. (1975). Short-term memory for “surprising” vs. “expected” unconditioned stimuli in Pavlovian conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1, 122–133.Google Scholar
Tomsik, D., Pedreira, M. E., Romano, A., Hermitte, G. & Maldonado, H. (1998). Context- US association as a determinant of long-term habituation in the crabChasmagnathus. Animal Learning and Behavior, 26, 196–209.Google Scholar
Vogel, E. H. & Wagner, A. R. (2005). Stimulus specificity in the habituation of startle response in rat. Physiology and Behavior, 86, 516–525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, A. R. (1969). Stimulus validity and stimulus selection in associative learning. In Mackintosh, N. J. and Honig, W.K., eds., Fundamental Issues in Associative Learning. Halifax, Canada: Dalhousie University Press, pp. 90–122.Google Scholar
Wagner, A. R. (1976). Priming in STM: an information-processing mechanism for self-generated or retrieval-generated depression in performance. In Tighe, T. J. and Leaton, R. N., eds., Habituation: Perspectives from Child Development, Animal Behavior and Neurophysiology. Hillsdale, NJ: Elrbaum, pp. 95–118.Google Scholar
Wagner, A. R. (1979). Habituation and memory. In Dickinson, A. and Boakes, R. A., eds., Mechanisms of Learning and Motivation: A Memorial Volume for Jerzy Konorski. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates, pp. 53–82.Google Scholar
Wagner, A. R. (1981). SOP: a model of automatic memory processing in animal behavior. In Spear, N. E. and Miller, R. R., eds., Information Processing in Animals: Memory Mechanisms. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 5–47.Google Scholar
Wagner, A. R. & Brandon, S. E. (1989). Evolution of a structured connectionist model of Pavlovian conditioning (AESOP). In Klein, S.B. and Mowrer, R.R., eds., Contemporary Learning Theories: Pavlovian Conditioning and the Status of Traditional Learning Theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 149–189.Google Scholar
Wagner, A. R. & Rescorla, R. A. (1972) Inhibition in Pavlovian conditioning: application of a theory. In Halliday, M. S. and Boakes, R. A., eds., Inhibition and Learning. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, pp. 301–335.Google Scholar
Wagner, A. R., Rudy, J. W. & Whitlow, J. W. (1973). Rehearsal in animal conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 97, 407–426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, A. R., Thomas, E. & Norton, T. (1967). Conditioning with electrical stimulation of motor cortex: evidence of a possible source of motivation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 64, 191–199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, B. J. & Leaton, R. N. (1994) Fear potentiation of acoustic startle stimulus-evoked heart rate changes in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 1065–1079.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×