Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ttngx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-11T11:39:00.055Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - How Does Collective Memory Create a Sense of the Collective?

Social and Psychological Processes Underlying Collective Memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Pascal Boyer
Affiliation:
Washington University, St. Louis
James V. Wertsch
Affiliation:
Washington University, St Louis
Get access

Summary

Where were you when you first heard about the 9/11 attacks? Do you remember what you were doing at the time? How did you feel when you first heard the news, and how does thinking about this event make you feel now? As of this writing, many Americans over the age of 15 most likely find these questions remarkably easy to answer. The fact that a singular event in history could be remembered so well, and by so many people, is relevant to at least two lines of research in the memory literature.

First, the very fact that people can answer such questions so easily is relevant to research and theory on “flashbulb memories” (Brown & Kulik, 1977; Kvavilashvili, Mirani, Schlagman, & Kornbrot, 2004; Sharot, Martorella, Delgado, & Phelps, 2006). As originally conceptualized, flashbulb memories were regarded as extraordinarily detailed, long-lasting, and unusually accurate “snapshots” of the specific context in which an unexpected, emotion-laden event occurred (Brown & Kulik, 1977). Over the years, research has qualified some of the early claims regarding such memories. For example, even though people often perceive that such memories are accurate, such recollections can contain the same sorts of distortions one finds with other types of memories (Neisser & Harsch, 1992). Nevertheless, more recent work has found that flashbulb memories can sometimes be extraordinarily accurate and detailed, provided that the event has direct, personal relevance to the perceiver (Bernsten & Thomsen, 2005).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baker, W. D., & O'Neal, J. R. (2001). Patriotism or opinion leadership?:The nature and origins of the “Rally ‘Round the Flag’ Effect. Journal of Conflict Resolution,” 45, 661–687.Google Scholar
Balkin, J. M. (1999). The Declaration and the promise of a democratic culture. Unpublished manuscript. Yale University: New Haven, CT.Google Scholar
Bernsten, D., & Thomsen, D. (2005). Personal memories for remote historical events: Accuracy and clarity of flashbulb memories related to World War II. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 134, 242–257.Google Scholar
Beschloss, M. R. (1997). Taking charge: The Johnson white house tapes, 1963–1964. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (1992). Tradition as truth and communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brody, R., & Shapiro, (1989). Policy failure and public support: The Iran-ContraPolicy Failure and Public Support: The Iran-Contra affair and assessments of President Reagan. Political Behavior, 11, 353–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R., & Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition, 5, 73–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohn, M. A., Mehl, M. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2004). Linguistic markers of psychological change surrounding September 11, 2001. Psychological Science, 15(10), 687–693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clore, G. L. (1994). Why emotions are never unconscious. In Ekman, P. & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 285–290). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ewen, S. (1996). A social history of spin. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Foote, K. (1990). To remember and forget: Archives, memory, and culture. American Archivist, 53, 378–392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forgas, J. P. (2002). Feeling and thinking: The influence of affect on social cognition and behavior. In Bäckman, L., Lars, E., & Hofsten, C. (Eds.), Psychology at the turn of the millennium, Vol. 1: Cognitive, biological, and health perspectives (pp. 455–480). Hove, England: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H., & Mesquita, B. (1994). The social roles and functions of emotions. In Kitayama, S. & Markus, H. (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence (pp. 51–87). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Halbwachs, M. (1992). On collective memory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Harber, K., & Pennebaker, J. (1992) Overcoming traumatic memories. In Christianson, S. (Ed.), The handbook of emotion and memory: Research and theory (pp. 359–387). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Harber, K. D., & Cohen, D. J. (2005). The emotional broadcaster theory of social sharing. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 24, 382–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmon-Jones, E. (2004). From cognitive dissonance to the motivational functions of emotions. In Wright, R. A., Greenberg, J., & Brehm, S. S. (Eds.), Motivation and emotion in social contexts: Jack Brehm's influence on contemporary psychological thought (pp. 39–55). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., & Gross, J. J. (1999). Functional accounts of emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 467–480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kvavilashvili, L., Mirani, J., Schlagman, S., & Kornbrot, D. (2004). Comparing flashbulb memories of September 11 and the death of Princess Diana: Effects of time delays and nationality. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 1017–1031.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunst-Wilson, W. R., & Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207, 557–558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambert, A. J., & Wyer, R. S. (1990). Stereotypes and social judgment: The effects of typicality and group heterogeneity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 676–691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambert, A. J., Nesse, L., Olson, K., Andrews, R., Zisser, A., & Schott, J. P. (2008). A dual-emotion framework of threat and social attitudes. Manuscript under review.
Lambert, A. J., Barton, L., Lickel, B. A., & Wells, J. (1998). The influence of group variability and processing goals on the ease of making decisions about social categories. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 807–820.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 146–159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bon, G. (1895) The crowd: A study of the popular mind. Mineola, NY: Dover Publications. (Originally published as La psychologie des foule.Google Scholar
Levine, L. J., Whalen, C. K., Jamner, L. D., & Henker, B. (2005). Looking back on September 11, 2001: Appraised impact on memory for emotions in adolescents and adults. Journal of Adolescent Research, 20, 497–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levine, L. J., Safer, M. A., & Lench, H. C. (2006). Remembering and misremembering emotions. In Sanna, L. J. & Chang, E. C. (Eds.), Judgments over time: The interplay of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors (pp. 271–290). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lickel, B., Hamilton, D., Wieczorkowska, G., Lewis, A., Sherman, S., & Uhles, A. (2000). Varieties of groups and the perception of group entitativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 223–246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McConnell, A. R., Sherman, S. J., & Hamilton, D. L. (1997). Target entitativity: Implications for information processing about individual and group targets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 72(4), 750–762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons Inc.Google Scholar
Mueller, J. E. (1970). Presidential popularity from Truman to Johnson. The American Political Science Review, 64, 18–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neisser, U., & Harsch, N. (1992). Phantom flashbulbs: False recollections of hearing the news about Challenger. In Winograd, E. & Neisser, U. (Eds.), Affect and accuracy in recall: Studies of “flashbulb” memories (Vol. 4, pp. 9–31). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Newcombe, N. S., Lloyd, M. E., & Ratliff, K. R. (2007). Development of episodic and autobiographical memory: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. In Kail, R. V. (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 35, pp. 37–85). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Novick, P. (1999). The Holocaust in American life. New York: Houghton Mifflin Co.Google Scholar
Olson, K., (2003). Effects of priming 9–11 memories on social attitudes and values. Undergraduate honors thesis, Washington University.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Banasik, B. (1997). On the creation and maintenance of collective memories: History as social psychology. In Pennebaker, J. W., Paez, D., & Rimé, B. (Eds.), Collective memory of political events: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 3–20). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Reicher, S. (2004). The psychology of crowd dynamics. In Brewer, M. B. & Hewstone, M. (Eds.), Self and social identity (pp. 232–258). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Rime, B., & Christophe, V. (1997). How individual emotional episodes feed collective memory. In Pennebaker, J. W., Paez, D., & Rim´e, B. (Eds.), Collective memory of political events: Social and psychological perspectives (pp. 131–146). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ross, M., & Wilson, A. E. (2003). Autobiographical memory and conceptions of self: Getting better all the time. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 66–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The false consensus effect: An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 279–301CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Safer, M. A., Levine, L. J., & Drapalski, A. (2002). Distortion in memory for emotions: The contributions of personality and post-event knowledge. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1495–1507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Schwalbe, C. B. (2006). Remembering our shared past: Visually framing the Iraq war on U.S. news websites. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12(1), article 14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In Higgins, E. T. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 527–561). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Sharot, T., Martorella, E. A., Delgado, M. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2006). How personal experience modulates the neural circuitry of memories of September 11. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 389–394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R., (1994). Declarative and nondeclarative memory: Multiple brain systems supporting learning and memory. In Schacter, D. L. & Tulving, E. (Eds.), Memory systems 1994 (pp. 203–231). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Squire, L. R. & Kandel, E. R. (1999). Memory: From mind to molecules. New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Stryker, S. (2004). Integrating emotion into identity theory. In Turner, J. (Ed.), Advances in group processes, Vol 21: Theory and research on human emotions (pp. 1–23). US: Elsevier Science/JAI Press.Google Scholar
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1991). Concepts of human memory. In Squire, L., Lynch, G., Weinberger, N. M., & McGaugh, J. L. (Eds.), Memory: Organization and locus of change (pp. 3–32). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Velleman, D. (1997). How to share an intention. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 57, 29–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wertsch, J. V., & Roediger, H. L. (2008). Collective memory: Conceptual foundations and theoretical approaches. Memory, 16, 318–326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wicker, A. W. (1969). Attitudes versus actions: The relationship of verbal and overt behavioral responses to attitude objects. Journal of Social Issues, 25, 41–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, R. A. (2005). Collective memory, group minds, and the extended mind thesis. Cognitive Processing, 6, 227–236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, A. E., & Ross, M. (2001). From chump to champ: People's appraisals of their earlier and current selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 572–584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yzerbyt, C., Judd, C., & Corneille, O. (2003). The psychology of group perception: Contributions to the study of homogeneity, entitativity and essentialism. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking. Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zertal, I. (2005). Israel's holocaust and the politics of nationhood. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×