Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-t6hkb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-12T03:00:23.288Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 February 2010

David Lightfoot
Affiliation:
Georgetown University, Washington DC
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, C. 1997 Investigating the origins of the “group genitive” in English. Transactions of the Philological Society 95. 1: 111–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, C. 2002 Case and Middle English genitive noun phrases. In Lightfoot, ed. 57–80.
Anderson, S. R. & Lightfoot, D. W. 2002 The language organ: linguistics as cognitive physiology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, M. A. 2001 The atoms of language. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bean, M. 1983 The development of word order patterns in Old English. London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Bejar, S. 2002 Movement, morphology, and learnability. In Lightfoot, ed. 307–325.
Belletti, A. 1994 Verb positions: evidence from Italian. In Lightfoot & Hornstein, eds. 19–40.
Berlin, I. 1996 The sense of reality: studies in ideas and their history. London: Chatto & Windus.Google Scholar
Berwick, R. C. 1985 The acquisition of syntactic knowledge. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. 1984a The language bioprogram hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7: 173–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D. 1984b Creole is still king: response to commentary. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7: 212–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D. 2004 Reconsidering creole exceptionalism. Language 80. 4: 828–833.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloomfield, L. 1933 Language. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Bobaljik, J. D. 2001 The Rich Agreement Hypothesis in review. Ms, McGill University.
Briscoe, E. 2000 Grammatical acquisition: inductive bias and coevolution of language and the acquisition device. Language 76. 2: 245–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brugmann, K. 1918 Verschiedenheiten der Satzgestaltung nach Massgabe der seelischen Grundfunktionen in den indogermanischen Sprachen. Leipzig: Trübner.Google Scholar
Brugmann, K. 1925 Die Syntax des einfachen Satzes im Indogermanischen. Berlin: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Brugmann, K. & Delbrück, B. 1886–1900 Grundriss der vergleichenden Grammatik der indogermanischen Sprachen. Strassburg: Trübner.Google Scholar
Buck, L. & Axel, R. 1991 A novel multigene family may encode odorant receptors: a molecular basis for odor recognition. Cell 65: 175–187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, A. 1959 Old English grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, L. 1990 Syntactic reconstruction and Finno-Ugric. In Andersen, H. & Koerner, K., eds. Historical linguistics 1987: papers from the 8th International Conference on Historical Linguistics. Amsterdam: Benjamins. 51–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, L. 1997 Typological and areal issues in reconstruction. In Fisiak, J., ed. Linguistic reconstruction and typology (Trends in Linguistics: Studies and Monographs 96). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 49–72.Google Scholar
Campbell, L. & Harris, A. 2002 Syntactic reconstruction and demythologizing “Myths and the prehistory of grammars.” Journal of Linguistics 38: 599–618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canale, M. 1978 Word order change in Old English: base reanalysis in generative grammar. PhD dissertation, McGill University.
Chambers, J. K. 1992 Dialect acquisition. Language 68: 673–705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaudenson, R. 1992 Des îles, des hommes, des langues. Paris: L'Harmattan.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1957 Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1959 Review of B. F. SkinnerVerbal Behavior. Language 35: 26–57.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1965 Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1973 Conditions on transformations. In Anderson, S. R. & Kiparsky, P., eds. A festschrift for Morris Halle. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. 232–286.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1975 The logical structure of linguistic theory. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1980 On binding. Linguistic Inquiry 11: 1–46.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1981a Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1981b Principles and parameters in syntactic theory. In Hornstein, N. & Lightfoot, D. W., eds. Explanation in linguistics: the logical problem of language acquisition. London: Longman. 32–75.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1986 Knowledge of language: Its nature, origin and use. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 2002 On nature and language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cinque, G. 1999 Adverbs and functional heads: a cross-linguistic perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. & K.-D. Smolka 1986 Psycholinguistic evidence and the description of V2 in German. In Haider, H. & Prinzhorn, M., eds. Verb-second phenomena in Germanic languages. Dordrecht: Foris. 137–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, R. 1992 The selection of syntactic knowledge. Language Acquisition 2: 83–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, R. & Roberts, I. G. 1993 A computational model of language learnability and language change. Linguistic Inquiry 24: 299–345.Google Scholar
Cohen, G. A. 1978 Karl Marx's theory of history: a defense. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Crain, S. & Thornton, R. 1998 Investigations in Universal Grammar: a guide to experiments on the acquisition of syntax and semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Crystal, D. 2004 The stories of English. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Curtiss, S. 1977 Genie: a psycholinguistic study of a modern-day “wild child.”New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Davies, A. M. 1998 Nineteenth-century linguistics [History of Linguistics, ed. G. Lepschy, vol. 4]. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. 2004 The ancestor's tale: a pilgrimage to the dawn of life. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.Google Scholar
DeGraff, M., ed. 1998 Language creation and change: creolization, diachrony and development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
DeGraff, M. 2001 Morphology in creole genesis: linguistics and ideology. In Kenstowicz, M., ed. Ken Hale: a life in language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 53–121.Google Scholar
DeGraff, M. 2004 Against creole exceptionalism (redux). Language 80. 4: 834–839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, R. 1997 The rise and fall of languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dresher, B. E. 1999 Charting the learning path: cues to parameter setting. Linguistic Inquiry 30: 27–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dresher, B. E. & Kaye, J. 1990 A computational learning model for metrical phonology. Cognition 34: 137–195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekwall, E. 1943 Studies on the genitive of groups in English. Lund: Gleerup.Google Scholar
Fikkert, P. 1994 On the acquisition of prosodic structure. PhD dissertation, University of Leiden.
Fikkert, P. 1995 Models of acquisition: how to acquire stress. In Beckman, J., ed. Proceedings of NELS 25. GLSA, University of Massachusetts. 27–42.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. D. 1998 Unambiguous triggers. Linguistic Inquiry 29. 1: 1–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedrich, P. 1975 Proto-Indo-European syntax. Journal of Indo European Studies monograph no.1, Butte, Montana.Google Scholar
Frisch, K. 1967 The dance language and orientation of the bees (transl. L. E. Chadwick). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gelderen, E. 1997 Verbal agreement and the grammar behind its “breakdown”: Minimalist feature checking. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Getty, M. 2002 The metre of Beowulf: a constraint-based approach. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gianollo, C., C. Guardiano, & G. Longobardi 2004 Historical implications of a formal theory of syntactic variation. Paper presented at DIGS VIII, Yale University. Ms, University of Trieste.
Gibson, E. & Wexler, K. 1994 Triggers. Linguistic Inquiry 25. 3: 407–454.Google Scholar
Goethe, J. W. 1790 Versuch die Metamorphose der Pflanzen zu erklären. Gotha: C. W. Ettinger.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. 2003 The resilience of language: what gesture creation in deaf children can tell us about how all children learn language. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Mylander, C. 1990 Beyond the input given: the child's role in the acquisition of language. Language 66: 323–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, M. & Crago, M. 1991 Familial aggregation of a developmental language disorder. Cognition 39: 1–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gorrell, J. H. 1895 Indirect discourse in Anglo-Saxon. PMLA 10. 3: 342–485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gould, S. J. 1985 A clock of evolution. Natural History, April: 12–25.Google Scholar
Greenberg, J. H. 1966 Some universals of grammar with particular reference to the order of meaningful elements. In Greenberg, J. H., ed. Universals of language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 73–113.Google Scholar
Grimes, B. F. & Grimes, J. E. 2000 Ethnologue, vol. 1: Languages of the world; vol. 2: Maps and indexes. Dallas: SIL International.Google Scholar
Grimm, J. 1848 Geschichte der deutschen Sprache, vol.1. Leipzig: Weidmannsche Buchhandlung.Google Scholar
Haeberli, E. 2002 Inflectional morphology and the loss of V2 in English. In Lightfoot, ed. 88–106.
Haegeman, L. 1994 Introduction to government and binding theory (2nd edn). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hall, R. 1966 Pidgin and creole languages. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Harris, A. & Campbell, L. 1995 Historical syntax in cross-linguistic perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haspelmath, M. 1999a Are there principles of grammatical change?Journal of Linguistics 35. 3: 579–595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haspelmath, M. 1999b Why is grammaticalization irreversible?Linguistics37. 6: 1043–1068.Google Scholar
Hawkins, J. 1979 Implicational universals as predictors of word order change. Language 55. 3: 618–648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickey, R. ed. 2003 Motives for language change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hiltunen, R. 1983 The decline of the prefixes and the beginnings of the English phrasal verb. Turku: Turun yliopisto.Google Scholar
Hock, H. H. 1986 Principles of historical linguistics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hock, H. H. & Joseph, B. D. 1996 Language history, language change, and language relationship: an introduction to historical and comparative linguistics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hockett, C. F. 1965 Sound change. Language 41: 185–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoenigswald, H. 1978 The annus mirabilis 1876 and posterity. Transactions of the Philological Society: 17–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmberg, A. & Platzack, C. 1995. The role of inflection in Scandinavian syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hopper, P. & Traugott, E. 2003 Grammaticalization (2nd edn). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hornstein, N. 2001 Move! A minimalist theory of construal. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hróarsdóttir, . 2000a Word order change in Icelandic. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hróarsdóttir, þ.2000b Interacting movements in the history of Icelandic. In Pintzuk, Tsoulas, & Warner, eds. 296–321.
Hubel, D. 1978 Vision and the brain. Bulletin of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences 31. 7: 28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hubel, D. & Wiesel, T. 1962 Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. Journal of Physiology 160: 106–154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humboldt, W. 1836 Über die Verschiedenheit des menschlichen Sprachbaues undihren Einfluss auf die geistige Entwicklung des Menschengeschlechts. Royal Academy of Sciences of Berlin [Linguistic variability and intellectual development, transl. Buck, G. C. & Raven, F. A. 1971. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press].Google Scholar
Hyams, N. 1986 Language acquisition and the theory of parameters. Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyams, N. 1996 The underspecification of functional categories in early grammar. In Clahsen, H., ed. Generative perspectives on language acquisition. Amsterdam: Benjamins. 91–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janda, R. D. 2001 Beyond “pathways” and “unidirectionality”: on the discontinuity of language transmission and the counterability of grammaticalization. Language Sciences 23. 2–3: 265–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janda, R. D. & Joseph, B. D., eds. 2003 Handbook of historical linguistics. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Jeffers, R. 1976 Syntactic change and syntactic reconstruction. In Christie, W., ed. Current progress in historical linguistics. Amsterdam: North-Holland. 1–16.Google Scholar
Jerne, N. K. 1985 The generative grammar of the immune system. Science 229: 1057–1059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jespersen, O. 1909 Progress in language (2nd edn). London: Swan Sonnenschein.Google Scholar
Jonas, D. 2002 Residual V-to-I. In Lightfoot, ed. 251–270.
Joseph, B. D. 2001 Is there such a thing as grammaticalization?Language Sciences 23. 2–3: 163–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kato, M. & Negrão, E., eds. 2000 Brazilian Portuguese and the null subject parameter. Frankfurt: Vervürt.Google Scholar
Kayne, R. S. 1994 The antisymmetry of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kegl, J., A. Senghas, & M. Coppola 1998 Creation through contact: sign language emergence and sign language change in Nicaragua. In DeGraff, ed. 179–237.
Kellner, L. 1892 Historical outlines of English syntax. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kemenade, A. 1987 Syntactic case and morphological case in the history of English. Dordrecht: Foris.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemenade, A. & Vincent, N., eds. 1997 Parameters of morphosyntactic change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kerswill, P. 1996 Children, adolescents, and language change. Language Variation and Change 7: 177–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiparsky, P. 1996 The shift to head-initial VP in Germanic. In Thráinsson, H., Epstein, S., & Peters, S. eds. Studies in comparative Germanic syntax, vol. 2. Dordrecht: Kluwer. 140–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiparsky, P. 1997 The rise of positional licensing in Germanic. In van Kemenade & Vincent, eds. 460–494.
Klein, W. 1974 Word order, Dutch children and their mothers. Publikaties van het Instituut voor Algemene Taalwetenschap 9. University of Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Koeneman, O. 2000 The flexible nature of verb movement. PhD dissertation, Univerity of Utrecht.
Kohonen, V. 1978 On the development of English word order in religious prose around 1000 and 1200 A.D.: a quantitative study of word order in context. Åbo: Åbo Akademi Foundation.Google Scholar
Koopman, H. 1984 The syntax of verbs: from verb movement rules in the Kru languages to Universal Grammar. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Koopman, H. 1986 The genesis of Haitian: implications of a comparison of some features of the syntax of Haitian, French, and West-African languages. In Muysken, P. & Smith, N., eds. Substrata versus universals in creole languages. Amsterdam: Benjamins. 231–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kouwenberg, S. 1992 From OV to VO: linguistic negotiation in the development of Berbice Dutch Creole. Lingua 88: 263–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroch, A. 1989 Reflexes of grammar in patterns of language change. Language Variation and Change 1: 199–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroch, A. 1994 Morphosyntactic variation. In Beals, K. et al., eds. Papers from the 30th Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistics Society: Parasession on Variation and Linguistic Theory. Chicago: Chicago Linguistics Society. 180–201.Google Scholar
Kroch, A. & A. Taylor 1997 The syntax of verb movement in Middle English: dialect variation and language contact. In van Kemenade and Vincent, eds. 297–325.
Kroch, A. 2000 Verb–object order in early Middle English. In Pintzuk, Tsoulas, & Warner, eds. 132–163.
Kuhn, T. 1962 The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Labov, W. 1963 The social motivation of a sound change. Word 19: 273–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, R. T. 1975 Language and woman's place. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Lasnik, H. 1999 Minimalist analysis. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Lass, R. 1980 On explaining linguistic change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lass, R. 1997 Historical linguistics and language change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lees, R. 1957 Review of Syntactic Structures. Language 33. 3: 375–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehmann, W. P. 1967 A reader in nineteenth century historical Indo-European linguistics. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Lehmann, W. P. 1974 Proto-Indo-European syntax. Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Lehmann, W. P. & L. Zgusta 1979 Schleicher's tale after a century. In Brogyanyi, B., ed. Studies in diachronic, synchronic, and typological linguistics: festschrift for Oswald Szemerenyi on the occasion of his 65th birthday. Amsterdam: Benjamins. 455–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenneberg, E. H. 1967 The biological foundations of language. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Lewontin, R. C. 1972 The apportionment of human diversity. Evolutionary Biology 6: 381–398.Google Scholar
Li, C. N., ed. 1975 Word order and word order change. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Li, C. N.ed. 1977 Mechanisms of syntactic change. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Lightfoot, D. W. 1974 The diachronic analysis of English modals. In Anderson, J. M. & Jones, C., eds. Historical linguistics. Amsterdam: North Holland. 219–249.Google Scholar
Lightfoot, D. W. 1979 Principles of diachronic syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lightfoot, D. W. 1980 Sur la reconstruction d'une proto-syntaxe. Langages 60: 109–123. [English version in Ramat, P., ed. Linguistic reconstruction and Indo-European syntax. Amsterdam: Benjamins. 128–142. Reprinted in Rauch, I. & Carr, J., eds. Language change. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.]Google Scholar
Lightfoot, D. W. 1989 The child's trigger experience: degree-0 learnability. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12. 2: 321–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, D. W. 1991 How to set parameters: arguments from language change. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lightfoot, D. W. 1993 Why UG needs a learning theory: triggering verb movement. In Jones, C., ed. Historical linguistics: problems and perspectives. London: Longman. 190–214. [Reprinted in Battye, A. & Roberts, I. G., eds. Clause structure and language change. Oxford: Oxford University Press.]Google Scholar
Lightfoot, D. W. 1994 Degree-0 learnability. In Lust, B., Hermon, G., & Kornfilt, J., eds. Syntactic theory and first language acquisition: crosslinguistic perspectives, vol. 2: Binding dependency and learnability. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 453–472.Google Scholar
Lightfoot, D. W. 1997 Catastrophic change and learning theory. Lingua 100: 171–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, D. W. 1999 The development of language: acquisition, change and evolution. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Lightfoot, D. W. ed. 2002a Syntactic effects of morphological change. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, D. W. 2002b Myths and the prehistory of grammars. Journal of Linguistics 38: 113–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, D. W. 2002c More myths. Journal of Linguistics 38: 619–626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, D. W. 2002d Introduction to Chomsky 1957. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Lightfoot, D. W. 2003 Grammaticalisation: cause or effect? In Hickey, ed. 99–123.
Lightfoot, D. W. 2006 Minimizing government: deletion as cliticization. The Linguistic Review 23.2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, D. W. & Hornstein, N., eds. 1994 Verb movement. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lilly, J. C. 1975 Lilly on dolphins: humans of the sea. Garden City, NY: Anchor Press.Google Scholar
Longobardi, G. 2002 Parametric comparison and historical relatedness. Paper presented at DIGS VII, Girona.
Longobardi, G. 2003 On parameters and parameter theory. In Stark, E. & Wandruszka, U., eds. Syntaxtheorien: Modelle, Methoden, Motive. Tübingen: Gunter Narr. 273–290.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. 1961 Evolution and modification of behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lumsden, J. 1998 Language acquisition and creolization. In DeGraff, ed. 129–157.
Marler, P. 1999 On innateness: are sparrow songs “learned” or “innate”? In Hauser, M. D. & Konishi, M., eds. The design of animal communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 293–332.Google Scholar
Matthews, P. 2003 On change in “E-language.” In Hickey, ed. 7–17.
Mayr, E. 2004 What makes biology unique? Considerations on the autonomy of a scientific discipline. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meillet, A. 1937 Introduction à l'étude comparative des langues indo-européennes. Paris: Hachette [reprinted 1964, University of Alabama Press].Google Scholar
Miller, D. G. 1975 Indo-European: VSO, SOV, SVO, or all three?Lingua 37: 31–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muysken, P. 1988 Are creoles a special type of language? In Newmeyer, F., ed. Linguistics: the Cambridge survey, vol. 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 285–301.Google Scholar
Mustanoja, T. 1960 A Middle English Syntax. Helsinki: Société Néophilologique.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. & Romaine, S. 2000 Vanishing voices: the extinction of the world's languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Newmeyer, F. 1998 Language form and language function. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Newmeyer, F. 2000 On the reconstruction of “Proto-World” word order. In Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Hurford, J., eds. The evolutionary emergence of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 372–388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newmeyer, F. 2003 Grammar is grammar and usage is usage. Language 79. 4: 682–707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newport, E. L. 1998 Reduced input in the acquisition of signed languages: Contributions to the study of creolization. In DeGraff, ed. 161–178.
Newport, E. L., D. Bavelier, & H. J. Neville 2002 Critical thinking about critical periods: Perspectives on a critical period for language acquisition. In Dupoux, E., ed. Language, brain and cognitive development. Cambridge MA: MIT Press. 481–502.Google Scholar
Nichols, J. 1992 Linguistic diversity in space and time. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niyogi, P. & Berwick, R. C. 1997 A dynamical systems model of language change. Complex Systems 11: 161–204.Google Scholar
Nunes, J. 1995 The copy theory of movement and linearization of chains in the Minimalist Program. PhD dissertation, University of Maryland.
Nunes, J. 2004 Linearization of chains and sideward movement. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Nunnally, T. 1985 The syntax of the genitive in Old, Middle, and Early Modern English. PhD dissertation, University of Georgia.
Neil, O' W. 1978 The evolution of the Germanic inflectional systems: a study in the causes of language change. Orbis 27. 2: 248–285.Google Scholar
Otani, K. & Whitman, J. 1991 V-raising and VP-ellipsis. Linguistic Inquiry 22: 345–358.Google Scholar
Paul, H. 1880 Prinzipien der Sprachgeschichte. Tübingen: Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Pearl, L. 2004 Addressing acquisition from language change: a modeling perspective. Penn Working Papers in Linguistics 11. 1.Google Scholar
Pedersen, H. 1931 The discovery of language: linguistic science in the nineteenth century. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piattelli-Palmarini, M. 1986 The rise of selective theories: a case study and some lessons from immunology. In Demopoulos, W. & Marras, A., eds. Language learning and concept acquisition: foundational issues. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. 117–130.Google Scholar
Pintzuk, S. 1999 Phrase structures in competition: variation and change in Old English word order. New York: Garland.Google Scholar
Pintzuk, S. 2002 Verb-complement order in Old English. In Lightfoot, ed. 276–299.
Pintzuk, S., Tsoulas, G., & Warner, A., eds. 2000 Diachronic syntax: models and mechanisms. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pires, A. 2002 Cue-based change: inflection and subjects in the history of Portuguese infinitives. In Lightfoot, ed. 143–159.CrossRef
Plank, F. 1984 The modals story retold. Studies in Language 8: 305–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Platzack, C. 1988 The emergence of a word order difference in Scandinavian subordinate clauses. Special Issue on Comparative Germanic Syntax: McGill Working Papers in Linguistics, 215–238.Google Scholar
Popper, K. 1959 The logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Pulgram, E. 1959 Proto-Indo-European reality and reconstruction. Language 35. 3: 421–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quirk, R. & Wrenn, C. L. 1955 An Old English grammar. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Rask, R. 1818 Undersøgelse om det gamle Nordiske eller Islandske Sprogs Oprindelse. Copenhagen: Gyldendalske Boghandlings Forlag.Google Scholar
Renfrew, C. 2000 At the edge of knowability: towards a prehistory of languages. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 10. 1: 7–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzi, L. 1978 Violations of the wh-island constraint in Italian and the Subjacency Condition. In Dubuisson, C., Lightfoot, D. W., & Morin, C.-Y., eds. Montreal Working Papers in Linguistics 11: 155–190.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. 1982 Issues in Italian syntax. Dordrecht: Foris.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, I. G. 1997 Directionality and word order change in the history of English. In van Kemenade & N. Vincent, eds. 397–426.
Roberts, I. G. 1998a Verb movement and markedness. In DeGraff, ed. 287–327.
Roberts, I. G. 1998b Review of A. Harris & L. Campbell, Historical syntax in cross-linguistic perspective. Romance Philology 51: 363–370.Google Scholar
Roberts, I. G. & A. Roussou 2002 The history of the future. In Lightfoot, ed. 23–56.
Roberts, I. G. 2003 Syntactic change: a minimalist approach to grammaticalization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robins, R. H. 1967 A short history of linguistics. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Rodrigues, C. 2002 Loss of verbal morphology and the status of referential null subjects in Brazilian Portuguese. In Lightfoot, ed. 160–178.
Rohrbacher, B. 1999 Morphology-driven syntax: a theory of V to I raising and pro-drop. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, J. R. 1967 Constraints on variables in syntax. PhD dissertation, MIT.
Ross, J. R. 1969 Auxiliaries as main verbs. In Todd, W., ed. Studies in philosophical linguistics, Series I. Evanston: Great Expectations. 77–102.Google Scholar
Sampson, G. 1980 Schools of linguistics: competition and evolution. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Sandler, W., Meir, I., Padden, C., & Aronoff, M. 2005 The emergence of grammar: systematic structure in a new language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102: 2661–2665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sankoff, G. & Laberge, S. 1973 On the acquisition of speakers by a native language. Kivung 6: 32–47.Google Scholar
Sapir, E. 1921 Language. New York: Harcourt.Google Scholar
Sapir, E. 1929 The status of linguistics as a science. Language 5: 207–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saussure, F. 1879 Mémoire sur le système primitif des voyelles dans les langages européennes. Leipzig: Teubner.Google Scholar
Schleicher, A. 1848 Über die Bedeutung der Sprache für die Naturgeschichte des Menschen. Weimar: Hermann Böhlau.Google Scholar
Schleicher, A. 1861–1862 Compendium der vergleichenden Grammatik der indogermanischen Sprachen. Weimar: Hermann Böhlau.Google Scholar
Schleicher, A. 1863 Die darwinische Theorie und Sprachwissenschaft. Weimar: Hermann Böhlau.Google Scholar
Schneider, E. W. 2003 The dynamics of new Englishes: from identity construction to dialect birth. Language 79. 2: 233–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Senghas, A., Kita, S., & Özyürek, A. 2004 Children creating core properties of language: evidence from an emerging sign language in Nicaragua. Science 305: 1779–1782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sievers, E. 1876 Grundzüge der Lautphysiologie. Leipzig: Breitkopf & Härtel.Google Scholar
Singler., J. 1988 The homogeneity of the substrate as a factor in pidgin/creole genesis. Language 64. 1: 27–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singleton, J. & Newport, E. L. 2004 When learners surpass their models: the acquisition of American Sign Language from impoverished input. Cognitive Psychology 49: 370–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, N. S. H., Robertson, I., & Williamson, K. 1987 The Ijo element in Berbice Dutch. Language in Society 16: 49–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperry, R. 1968 Plasticity of neural maturation. Developmental Biology Supplement 2: 306–27.Google Scholar
Steever, S., Walker, C., & Mufwene, S., eds. 1976 Diachronic syntax. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Stewart, I. 1998 Life's other secret. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Stockwell, R. P. & D. Minkova 1991 Subordination and word order change in the history of English. In Kastovsky, D., ed. Historical English Syntax. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 367–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stokoe, W. 1960 Sign language structure: an outline of the visual communications systems. Studies in Linguistics, Occasional Papers 8.Google Scholar
Sundquist, J. D. 2002 Object Shift and Holmsberg's Generalization in the history of Norwegian. In Lightfoot, ed. 326–347.
Supalla, S. 1990 Segmentation of Manually Coded English: problems in the mapping of English in the visual/gestural mode. PhD dissertation, University of Illinois.
Thackray, A. 1970 Atoms and powers. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomason, S. G. & Kaufman, T. 1988 Language contact, creolization, and genetic linguistics. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Thompson, D. W. 1917 On growth and form. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornton, R. 1995 Referentiality and Wh-movement in child English: Juvenile D-linkuency. Language Acquisition 4: 139–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornton, R. & Wexler, K. 1999 Principle B, VP ellipsis, and interpretation in child grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thorpe, W. H. 1957 The learning of song patterns by birds, with special reference to the song of the Chaffinch Fringilla Coelebs. Ibis 100: 535–570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thráinsson, H. 2003 Syntactic variation, historical development and Minimalism. In Hendrick, R., ed. Minimalist syntax. Oxford: Blackwell. 152–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tinbergen, N. 1957 The herring gull's world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Turing, A. 1952 The chemical basis of morphogenesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London37–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vennemann, T. 1975 An explanation of drift. In Li, ed. 269–305.
Venter, C. 2002 Commencement address: Georgetown University Graduate School of Arts & Sciences.
Vikner, S. 1994 Finite verb movement in Scandinavian embedded clauses. In Lightfoot & Hornstein, eds. 117–147.
Vikner, S. 1997 V-to-I movement and inflection for person in all tenses. In Haegeman, L., ed. The new comparative syntax. London: Longman. 189–213.Google Scholar
Warner, A. R. 1993 English auxiliaries: structure and history. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warner, A. R. 1995 Predicting the progressive passive: parametric change within a lexicalist framework. Language 71. 3: 533–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warner, A. R. 1997 The structure of parametric change, and V movement in the history of English. In van Kemenade & Vincent, eds. 380–393.
Watkins, C. 1976 Toward Proto-Indo-European syntax: problems and pseudoproblems. In Steever, Walker, & Mufwene, eds. 305–326.
Weinberg, S. 1977 The forces of nature. American Scientist 65. 2: 171–176.Google Scholar
Westergaard, M. 2005 What Norwegian child language can tell us about the history of English. In , K. McCafferty, , T. Bull, & , K. Killie, eds. Contexts – historical, social, linguistic: studies in celebration of Toril Swan. Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Wexler, K. 1994 Optional infinitives, head movement, and the economy of derivations. In Lightfoot & Hornstein, eds. 305–350.
Whitney, D. 1875 The life and growth of language: an outline of linguistic science. New York: D. Appleton & Co.Google Scholar
Wurff, W. 1999 Objects and verbs in modern Icelandic and fifteenth-century English: a word order parallel and its causes. Lingua 109: 237–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, C. 2002 Knowledge and learning in natural language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zagona, K. 1988 Proper government of antecedentless VP in English and Spanish. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 6: 95–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • David Lightfoot, Georgetown University, Washington DC
  • Book: How New Languages Emerge
  • Online publication: 02 February 2010
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511616204.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • David Lightfoot, Georgetown University, Washington DC
  • Book: How New Languages Emerge
  • Online publication: 02 February 2010
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511616204.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • David Lightfoot, Georgetown University, Washington DC
  • Book: How New Languages Emerge
  • Online publication: 02 February 2010
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511616204.010
Available formats
×