Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-mwx4w Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-21T01:00:11.795Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 17 - Teaching about Cultural Differences in the Correspondence Bias

from Social Psychology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 March 2018

Kenneth D. Keith
Affiliation:
University of San Diego
Get access

Summary

Modern conceptualizations of psychopathology or psychological disorders are derived primarily from Western perspectives. However, there is widespread awareness that culture and psychology are inextricably linked. The field of cultural clinical psychology is very diverse. There is no consensus on how a course in this field should be taught, and there is no single course that can capture the full range of cultural variation in human behavior and functioning. Consequently, course syllabi will vary in their objectives, contents, and method of instruction. Nevertheless, all courses in cultural clinical psychology share a common purpose, and that is to increase the students’ cultural awareness and knowledge and to prepare them to be culturally sensitive, if not competent, in their work and their daily life.  
Type
Chapter
Information
Culture across the Curriculum
A Psychology Teacher's Handbook
, pp. 371 - 390
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aronson, E., Wilson, T., & Akert, R. M. (2017). Social psychology (8th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.Google Scholar
Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. L. (1996). Who is this “we”? Levels of collective identity and self-representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 8393. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.1.83Google Scholar
Bungay, D. (2009, November 8). Raw video – Soccer player throws fist and pulls hair. Retrieved from www.youtube.com/watch?v=bjV2D4s2kDUGoogle Scholar
Chiu, C. Y., Hong, Y. Y., & Dweck, C. S. (1997). Lay dispositionism and implicit theories of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1930. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.19Google Scholar
Chiu, L.-H. (1972). A cross-cultural comparison of cognitive styles in Chinese and American children. International Journal of Psychology, 7, 235242. doi:10.1080/00207597208246604Google Scholar
Choi, I., Dalal, R., Kim-Prieto, C., & Park, H. (2003). Culture and judgment of causal relevance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 4659. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.46Google Scholar
Choi, I., & Nisbett, R. E. (1998). Situational salience and cultural differences in the correspondence bias and in the actor-observer bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 949960. doi:10.1177/0146167298249003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). Causal attribution across cultures: Variation and universality. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 4763. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.1.47Google Scholar
Ciccarelli, S. K., & White, J. N. (2017). Psychology (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.Google Scholar
Davis, A., Jacobs, D., Kolber, J. (Writers), & Nigro, M. (Director). (2015). Peer pressure. In P. C. Wells (Producer), Brain Games. National Geographic.Google Scholar
Dean, K. K., & Gardner, W. L. (2014). How will “I” vs. “we” perform? An investigation of future outlooks and self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 947958. doi:10.1177/0146167214532137Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S., Hong, Y. Y., & Chiu, C. Y. (1993). Implicit theories and individual differences in the likelihood and meaning of dispositional inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 644656. doi:10.1177/0146167293195015Google Scholar
Editorial Board. (2015, December). Don’t blame mental illness for gun violence. The New York Times. Retrieved from www.nytimes.com/2015/12/16/opinion/dont-blame-mental-illness-for-gun-violence.html?_r=0Google Scholar
Fiske, A. P., Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Nisbett, R. E. (1998). The cultural matrix of social psychology. In Gilbert, D. & Fiske, S. (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology, (Vol. 2, 4th ed., pp. 915981). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Lee, A. Y. (1999). “I” value freedom, but “we” value relationships: Self-construal priming mirrors cultural differences in judgment. Psychological Science, 10, 321326. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00162Google Scholar
Gawronski, B. (2004). Theory-based bias correction in dispositional inference: The fundamental attribution error is dead, long live the correspondence bias. European Review of Social Psychology, 15, 183217. doi:10.1080/10463280440000026Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., & Jones, E. E. (1986). Perceiver-induced constraint: Interpretations of self-generated reality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 269280. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.269Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 2138. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.21Google Scholar
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York, NY: Wiley. doi:10.1037/10628-000Google Scholar
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Johannesen-Schmidt, M. (2008). Instructor’s resource manual to social psychology (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
Jones, E. E. (1979). The rocky road from acts to disposition. American Psychologist, 34, 107117. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.34.2.107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, E. E. (1990). Interpersonal perception. New York, NY: Freeman.Google Scholar
Jones, E. E., & Harris, V. A. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 124. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(67)90034-0Google Scholar
Kashima, Y., Siegel, M., Tanaka, K., & Kashima, E. S. (1992). Do people believe behaviors are consistent with attitudes? Towards a cultural psychology of attribution processes. British Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 111124. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.1992.tb00959.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kassin, S., Fein, S., & Markus, H. R. (2017). Social psychology (10th ed.). Boston, MA: Cengage.Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 12451267. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.6.1245Google Scholar
Knowles, E. D., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C.-Y., & Hong, Y.-Y. (2001). Culture and the process of person perception: Evidence for automaticity among East Asians in correcting for situational influences on behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 13441356. doi:10.1177/01461672012710010Google Scholar
Koenig, A. M., & Dean, K. K. (2011). Cross-cultural differences and similarities in attribution. In Keith, K. D. (Ed.), Cross-cultural psychology: Contemporary themes and perspectives (pp. 475493). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Krull, D. S., Loy, M. H., Lin, J., Wang, C. F., Chen, S., & Zhao, X. (1999). The fundamental fundamental attribution error: Correspondence bias in individualist and collectivist cultures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 12081219. doi:10.1177/0146167299258003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kühnen, U., & Hannover, B. (2000). Assimilation and contrast in social comparisons as a consequence of self-construal activation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 799811. doi:10.1002/1099-0992(200011/12)30:6<799::AID-EJSP16>3.0.CO;2-2Google Scholar
Kühnen, U., Hannover, B., Pohlmann, C., & Roeder, U. (2013). How self-construal affects dispositionalism in attributions. Social Cognition, 31, 237259. doi:10.1521/soco.2013.31.2.237CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, F., Hallahan, M., & Herzog, T. (1996). Explaining real-life events: How culture and domain shape attributions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 732741. doi:10.1177/0146167296227007Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224253. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224Google Scholar
Masuda, T., & Kitayama, S. (2004). Perceiver-induced constraint and attitude attribution in Japan and the US: A case for the cultural dependence of the correspondence bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 409416. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2003.08.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Attending holistically versus analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 922934. doi:10.1037/0022-3514,81.5.922Google Scholar
Miller, J. G. (1984). Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 961978. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.46.5.961Google Scholar
Miyamoto, Y., & Kitayama, S. (2002). Culture and correspondence bias: Is the road from act to disposition rockier in Japan? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 12391248. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.5.1239Google Scholar
Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 949971. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.949Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Analytic versus holistic cognition. Psychological Review, 108, 291310. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Choi, I., & Nisbett, R. E. (2002). Cultural similarities and differences in social inference: Evidence from behavioral predictions and lay theories of behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 109120. doi:10.1177/0146167202281010Google Scholar
Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. S. (2007). Priming “culture”: Culture as situated cognition. In Kitayama, S. & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 255279). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 311142. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.311Google Scholar
Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54, 741754. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.9.741Google Scholar
Poon, C. S. K., & Koehler, D. J. (2006). Lay personality knowledge and dispositionist thinking: A knowledge-activation framework. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 177191. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.04.001Google Scholar
Prudential. (2013, May 13). Prudential: Everybody’s doing it. Retrieved from www.youtube.com/watch?v=BgRoiTWkBHUGoogle Scholar
RcJ2. (2009, November 6). Elizabeth Lambert of New Mexico Lobos suspended. Retrieved from www.youtube.com/watch?v=PJiRQsyrBoIGoogle Scholar
Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 173220). New York, NY: Academic Press. doi:10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60357-3Google Scholar
Ross, L., Amabile, T. M., & Steinmetz, J. L. (1977). Social roles, social control, and biases on social-perception processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 485494. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.35.7.485Google Scholar
Trafimow, D., Triandis, H., & Goto, S. (1991). Some tests of the distinction between the private self and the collective self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 649655. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.5.649Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview.Google Scholar
van Boven, L., Kamada, A., & Gilovich, T. (1999). The perceiver as perceived: Everyday intuition about the correspondence bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 11881199. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1188Google Scholar
Weiten, W. (2017). Psychology: Themes and variations (10th ed.). Boston, MA: Cengage.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×