Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-75dct Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-02T02:28:55.343Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Mother–Child Conversations about Children’s Moral Wrongdoing: A Constructivist Perspective on Moral Socialization

from Part II - Social Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 May 2017

Nancy Budwig
Affiliation:
Clark University, Massachusetts
Elliot Turiel
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Philip David Zelazo
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, R., & Laursen, B. (2007). The correlates of conflict: Disagreement is not necessarily detrimental. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 445458.Google Scholar
Callanan, M., Valle, A., Luce, M., & Rigney, J. (2014). Discussions of moral issues emerging in family conversations about science. In Wainryb, C. & Recchia, H. (Eds.), Talking about right and wrong: Parent-child conversations as contexts for moral development (pp. 193216). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dunn, J., & Hughes, C. (2014). Family talk about moral issues: The toddler and preschool years. In Wainryb, C. & Recchia, H. (Eds.), Talking about right and wrong: Parent-child conversations as contexts for moral development (pp. 2143). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T., & Sadovsky, A. (2006). Empathy-related responding in children. In Killen, M. & Smetana, J. (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 517550). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fivush, R. (2007). Maternal reminiscing style and children’s developing understanding of self and emotion. Clinical Social Work, 35, 3746.Google Scholar
Grusec, J., & Davidov, M. (2006). Socialization in the family: The role of parents. In Grusec, J. & Hastings, P. (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 284308). New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
Hardy, S., & Carlo, G. (2011). Moral identity: What is it, how does it develop, and is it linked to moral action? Child Development Perspectives, 5, 212218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kochanska, G., Coy, K., & Murray, K. (2001). The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 10911111.Google Scholar
Komolova, M., & Wainryb, C. (2011). What I want and what you want: Children’s thinking about competing personal preferences. Social Development, 20, 334352.Google Scholar
Kuczynski, L., & Parkin, M. (2006). Agency and bidirectionality in socialization. In Grusec, J. & Hastings, P. (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 259283). New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
Lagattuta, K. (2005). When you shouldn’t do what you want to do: Young children’s understanding of desires, rules, and emotions. Child Development, 76, 713733.Google Scholar
Laible, D., & Murphy, T. (2014). Constructing moral, emotional, and relational understandings in the context of mother–child reminiscing. In Wainryb, C. & Recchia, H. (Eds.), Talking about right and wrong: Parent-child conversations as contexts for moral development (pp. 98121). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McLean, K., & Pasupathi, M. (2012). Processes of identity development: Where I am and how I got there. Identity, 12, 828.Google Scholar
Miller, P. (2014). Placing discursive practices front and center: A socio-cultural approach to the study of early socialization. In Wainryb, C. & Recchia, H. (Eds.), Talking about right and wrong: Parent-child conversations as contexts for moral development (pp. 416447). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nucci, L. (2001). Education in the moral domain. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nucci, L. (2014). Conversations in the home: The role of dialogue and resistance in children’s emerging understandings of moral, convention, and the personal. In Wainryb, C. & Recchia, H. (Eds.), Talking about right and wrong: Parent-child conversations as contexts for moral development (pp. 367388). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pasupathi, M., & Wainryb, C. (2010a). Developing moral agency through narrative. Human Development, 53, 5580.Google Scholar
Pasupathi, M., & Wainryb, C. (2010b). On telling the whole story: Facts and interpretations in autobiographical memory narratives from childhood through midadolescence. Developmental Psychology, 46, 735746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. New York, NY: Free Press.Google Scholar
Recchia, H., & Wainryb, C. (2014). Mother-child conversations about hurting others: Scaffolding the construction of moral agency through childhood and adolescence. In Wainryb, C. & Recchia, H. (Eds.), Talking about right and wrong: Parent-child conversations as contexts for moral development (pp. 242269). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Recchia, H., Wainryb, C., Bourne, S., & Pasupathi, M. (2014). The construction of moral agency in mother-child conversations about helping and hurting across childhood and adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 50, 3444Google Scholar
Recchia, H., Wainryb, C., & Pasupathi, M. (2013). “Two for flinching”: Children’s and adolescents’ narrative accounts of harming their friends and siblings. Child Development, 84, 14591474.Google Scholar
Shaw, L., & Wainryb, C. (2006). When victims don’t cry: Children’s understandings of victimization, compliance, and subversion. Child Development, 77, 10501062.Google Scholar
Smetana, J. (2011). Adolescents, families, and social development: How adolescents construct their worlds. West Sussex, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
Smetana, J. (2013). Moral development: The social domain theory view. In Zelazo, P. (Ed.), Oxford handbook of developmental psychology, Vol. 1, (pp. 832866). NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sterponi, L. (2014). Caught red-handed: How Italian parents engage children in moral discourse and action. In Wainryb, C. & Recchia, H. (Eds.), Parent-child conversations as contexts for moral development (pp. 122142). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. (2010). Feeling and understanding through the prism of relationships. In Calkins, S. & Bell, M. (Eds.), Child development at the intersection of emotion and cognition (pp. 7995). Washington, DC: APA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R., Meyer, S., & McGinley, M. (2006). Understanding values in relationships: The development of conscience. In Killen, M. & Smetana, J. (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp .267298). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (2010). Domain specificity in social interactions, social thought, and social development. Child Development, 81, 720726.Google Scholar
Wainryb, C., & Brehl, B. (2006). I thought she knew that would hurt my feelings: Developing psychological knowledge and moral thinking. In Kail, R. (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 34 (pp. 131173). New York, NY: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Wainryb, C., Brehl, B., & Matwin, S. (2005). Being hurt and hurting others: Children’s narrative accounts and moral judgments of their own interpersonal conflicts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 70 (Serial No. 281).Google ScholarPubMed
Wainryb, C., Komolova, M., & Brehl, B. (2014). Being left out: Children’s narrative accounts and judgments of their own experiences with peer exclusion. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 60, 461490.Google Scholar
Wainryb, C., & Pasupathi, M. (2015). Saints, and the rest of us: Broadening the perspective on moral identity development. Human Development, 58, 154163.Google Scholar
Wainryb, C., & Recchia, H. E. (2012). Emotion and the moral lives of adolescents: Vagaries and complexities in the emotional experience of doing harm. New Directions in Youth Development, 136, 1326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wainryb, C., & Recchia, H. (2014a). Parent-child conversations as contexts for moral development: Why conversations, and why conversations with parents? In Wainryb, C. & Recchia, H. (Eds.), Talking about right and wrong: Parent-child conversations as contexts for moral development (pp. 320). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wainryb, C., & Recchia, H. (2014b) (Eds.). Talking about right and wrong: Parent-child conversations as contexts for moral development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wright, J., & Bartsch, K. (2008). Portraits of early moral sensibility in two children’s everyday conversations. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 54, 5685.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×