Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T16:02:19.247Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

45 - The Psychiatry of Imagination

from Part VI - Altered States of the Imagination

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2020

Anna Abraham
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Get access

Summary

Psychiatric disorders provide unique insights into imagination, because they represent extremes of evolved adaptations and trade-offs. I review imagination in the two main sets of disorders of human social and imaginative cognition: the autism spectrum and the psychotic affective spectrum. To do so, I first operationally define imagination in terms of its components, then discuss how seven major aspects of imagination – pretend play, creativity, narrative and the arts, mental time travel, salience, mental imagery and sensory systems, and neural system instantiating it – are altered in autism and psychotic-affective spectrum conditions, mainly with reference to schizophrenia. I then relate fluid intelligence to imagination, and both of them to autism and schizophrenia, genetically and phenotypically. By this narrative review, the autism spectrum involves, overall, lower imagination (and higher aspects of intelligence), while schizophrenia involves higher aspects of imagination (but lower intelligence). Autism and psychotic-affective disorders provide novel opportunities to analyze the causes and consequences of increased, and reduced, imagination, with direct implications for mental-disorder therapies, and enhancements of imagination and creativity among relatively neurotypical individuals.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Acar, S., Chen, X., and Cayirdag, N. (2018). Schizophrenia and Creativity: A Meta-Analytic Review. Schizophrenia Research, 195, 2331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Acredolo, L., Goodwyn, S., and Fulmer, A. (1995). Why Some Children Create Imaginary Companions: Clues from Infant and Toddler Play Preferences. Poster presented at the biennial meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN.Google Scholar
Alonso-Solís, A., Vives-Gilabert, Y., Grasa, E., et al. (2015). Resting-State Functional Connectivity Alterations in the Default Network of Schizophrenia Patients with Persistent Auditory Verbal Hallucinations. Schizophrenia Research, 161, 261268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arzy, S., Mohr, C., Molnar-Szakacs, I., and Blanke, O. (2011). Schizotypal Perceptual Aberrations of Time: Correlation between Score, Behavior and Brain Activity. PLoS One, 6, e16154.Google Scholar
Baas, M., Nijstad, B. A., Boot, N. C., and de Dreu, C. K. (2016). Mad Genius Revisited: Vulnerability to Psychopathology, Biobehavioral Approach-Avoidance, and Creativity. Psychological Bulletin, 142(6), 668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, J. L., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2012). The Big Picture: Storytelling Ability in Adults with Autism Spectrum Conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 15571565.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). Autism: The Empathizing-Systemizing (E-S) Theory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 6880.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., and Lombardo, M. V. (2017). Autism and Talent: The Cognitive and Neural Basis of Systemizing. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 19(4), 345.Google Scholar
Batey, M., and Furnham, A. (2006). Creativity, Intelligence, and Personality: A Critical Review of the Scattered Literature. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 132(4), 355429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birchwood, M., Meaden, A., Trower, P., Gilbert, P., and Plaistow, J. (2000). The Power and Omnipotence of Voices: Subordination and Entrapment by Voices and Significant Others. Psychological Medicine, 30, 337344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonne, O., Canetti, L., Bachar, E., De-Nour, A. K., and Shalev, A. (1999). Childhood Imaginary Companionship and Mental Health in Adolescence. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 29, 277286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bottema-Beutel, K., and White, R. (2016). By the Book: An Analysis of Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Condition Co-Constructing Fictional Narratives with Peers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(2), 361377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boucher, J. (2007). Memory and Generativity in Very High Functioning Autism: A Firsthand Account, and an Interpretation. Autism, 11(3), 255264.Google Scholar
Brébion, G., Ohlsen, R. I., Pilowsky, L. S., and David, A. S. (2008). Visual Hallucinations in Schizophrenia: Confusion between Imagination and Perception. Neuropsychologia, 22, 383.Google Scholar
Brewin, C. R., and Soni, M. (2011). Gender, Personality, and Involuntary Autobiographical Memory. Memory, 19, 559565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broyd, S. J., Demanuele, C., Debener, S., et al. (2009). Default-Mode Brain Dysfunction in Mental Disorders: A Systematic Review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 279296.Google Scholar
Brugger, P., and Mohr, C. (2008). The Paranormal Mind: How the Study of Anomalous Experiences and Beliefs May Inform Cognitive Neuroscience. Cortex, 44, 12911298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The Brain’s Default Network. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 138.Google Scholar
Campbell, B. C., and Wang, S. S. (2012). Familial Linkage between Neuropsychiatric Disorders and Intellectual Interests. PloS One, 7, e30405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J., and Williams, L. (2004). Executive Function and Theory of Mind: Stability and Prediction From Ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, S. M., Tahiroglu, D., and Taylor, M. (2008). Links between Dissociation and Role Play in a Nonclinical Sample of Preschool Children. Journal of Trauma Dissociation, 9, 149171.Google Scholar
Carson, S. H. (2011). Creativity and Psychopathology: A Shared Vulnerability Model. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 144153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi-Kain, L. W., and Gunderson, J. G. (2008). Mentalization: Ontogeny, Assessment, and Application in the Treatment of Borderline Personality Disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 11271135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, S. V., Mittal, V. A., Bernard, J. A., et al. (2018). Stronger Default Mode Network Connectivity Is Associated with Poorer Clinical Insight in Youth at Ultra High Risk for Psychotic Disorders. Schizophrenia Research, 193, 244250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coleman, J. R., Bryois, J., Gaspar, H. A., et al. (2018). Biological Annotation of Genetic Loci Associated with Intelligence in a Meta-Analysis of 87,740 Individuals. Molecular Psychiatry, 1.Google Scholar
Collerton, D., Perry, E., and McKeith, I. (2005). Why People See Things That Are Not There: A Novel Perception and Attention Deficit Model for Recurrent Complex Visual Hallucinations. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 737757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, M., Meares, K., and Standart, S. (2004). Images and Goals. Memory, 12, 525531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, R. A., and Simons, J. S. (2019). Exploring the Neurocognitive Basis of Episodic Recollection in Autism. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(1), 163181. doi:10.3758/s13423-018-1504-z.Google Scholar
Craig, J., and Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Creativity and Imagination in Autism and Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 319326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, J., Baron-Cohen, S., and Scott, F. (2000). Story-Telling Ability in Autism: A Window into the Imagination. Israel Journal of Psychiatry, 37, 6470.Google ScholarPubMed
Crespi, B. J. (2016a). Autism as a Disorder of High Intelligence. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 10, 300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crespi, B. J.(2016b). The Evolutionary Etiologies of Autism Spectrum and Psychotic Affective Spectrum Disorders. In Alvergne, A, Jenkinson, C, and Faurie, C (eds.), Evolutionary Thinking in Medicine: From Research to Policy and Practice. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing AG, 299327.Google Scholar
Crespi, B., and Badcock, C. (2008). Psychosis and Autism as Diametrical Disorders of the Social Brain. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 241261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crespi, B., Leach, E., Dinsdale, N., Mokkonen, M., and Hurd, P. (2016). Imagination in Human Social Cognition, Autism, and Psychotic-Affective Conditions. Cognition, 150, 181199.Google Scholar
Currie, G. (2000). Imagination, Delusion and Hallucinations. Mind & Language, 15, 168183.Google Scholar
Currie, G., and Jureidini, J. (2003). Art and Delusion. The Monist, 86(4), 556578.Google Scholar
Currie, G., and Ravenscroft, I. (2002). Recreative Minds: Imagination in Philosophy and Psychology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Daniel, C., and Mason, O. J. (2015). Predicting Psychotic-Like Experiences during Sensory Deprivation. BioMed Research International, 2015, 439379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, P. E., Simon, H., Meins, E., and Robins, D. L. (2018). Imaginary Companions in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(8), 2790–2799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Oliveira, H., Cuervo-Lombard, C., Salamé, P., and Danion, J. (2009). Autonoetic Awareness Associated with the Projection of the Self into the Future: An Investigation in Schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 169, 8687.Google Scholar
Dentico, D., Cheung, B. L., Chang, J., et al. (2014). Reversal of Cortical Information Flow during Visual Imagery as Compared to Visual Perception. NeuroImage, 100, 237243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeYoung, C. G., Grazioplene, R. G., and Peterson, J. B. (2012). From Madness to Genius: The Openness/Intellect Trait Domain as a Paradoxical Simplex. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(1), 6378.Google Scholar
Dichter, G. S., Lam, K. S. L., Turner-Brown, L. M., et al. (2009). Generativity Abilities Predict Communication Deficits but not Repetitive Behaviors in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 12981304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickerson, A. S., Pearson, D. A., Loveland, K. A., et al. (2014). Role of Parental Occupation in Autism Spectrum Disorder Diagnosis and Severity. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8, 9971007.Google Scholar
Dierker, L. C., Davis, K. F., and Sanders, B. (1995). The Imaginary Companion Phenomenon: An Analysis of Personality Correlates and Developmental Antecedents. Dissociation, 4, 220228.Google Scholar
Elsworth, C. (2013). We Did Not Know That Our Schizophrenic Daughter January Schofield’s Imaginary Friends Were Hallucinations. The Telegraph, January 27. www.telegraph.co.uk/news/health/children/9828583/We-did-not-know-that-our-schizophrenic-daughter-January-Schofields-imaginary-friends-were-hallucinations.html.Google Scholar
Ferretti, F., Adornetti, I., Chiera, A., et al. (2018). Time and Narrative: An Investigation of Storytelling Abilities in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 944.Google Scholar
Fink, A., Weber, B., Koschutnig, K., et al. (2014). Creativity and Schizotypy from the Neuroscience Perspective. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, 378387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, U. (2012). Why We Need Cognitive Explanations of Autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 20732092.Google Scholar
Fyfe, S., Williams, C., Mason, O. J., and Pickup, G. J. (2008). Apophenia, Theory of Mind and Schizotypy: Perceiving Meaning and Intentionality in Randomness. Cortex, 44, 13161325.Google Scholar
Gleason, T. R., Jarudi, R. N., and Cheek, J. M. (2003). Imagination, Personality, and Imaginary Companions. Social Behavior and Personality, 31, 721737.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goddard, L., Howlin, P., Dritschel, B., and Patel, T. (2007). Autobiographical Memory and Social Problem-Solving in Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 291300.Google Scholar
Grandin, T. (1995). Thinking in Pictures. New York, NY: Vintage Press Random House.Google Scholar
Grandin, T.(2009). How Does Visual Thinking Work in the Mind of a Person with Autism? A Personal Account. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364, 14371442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greicius, M. D., and Menon, V. (2004). Default-Mode Activity during a Passive Sensory Task: Uncoupled from Deactivation but Impacting Activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 14841492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guo, W., Liu, F., Chen, J., et al. (2017). Hyperactivity of the Default-Mode Network in First-Episode, Drug-Naive Schizophrenia at Rest Revealed by Family-Based Case-Control and Traditional Case-Control Designs. Medicine, 96(13).Google ScholarPubMed
Hach, S., Tippett, L. J., and Addis, D. R. (2014). Neural Changes Associated with the Generation of Specific Past and Future Events in Depression. Neuropsychologia, 65, 4155.Google Scholar
Hanson, L. K., and Atance, C. M. (2014). Brief Report: Episodic Foresight in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 674684.Google Scholar
Happé, F., and Frith, U. (2006). The Weak Coherence Account: Detail-Focused Cognitive Style in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E. V. (2005). Imaginary Companions, Creativity, and Self-Image in Middle Childhood. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 167180.Google Scholar
Ivins, A., Di Simplicio, M., Close, H., Goodwin, G. M., and Holmes, E. (2014). Mental Imagery in Bipolar Affective Disorder versus Unipolar Depression: Investigating Cognitions at Times of “Positive” Mood. Journal of Affective Disorders, 166, 234242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarrold, C. (2003). A Review of Research into Pretend Play in Autism. Autism, 7, 379390.Google Scholar
Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., and Smith, P. K. (1996). Generativity Deficits in Pretend Play in Autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 275300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jauk, E., Neubauer, A. C., Dunst, B., Fink, A., and Benedek, M. (2015). Gray Matter Correlates of Creative Potential: A Latent Variable Voxel-Based Morphometry Study. NeuroImage, 111, 312320.Google Scholar
Javitt, D. C. (2009a). When Doors of Perception Close: Bottom-Up Models of Disrupted Cognition in Schizophrenia. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5, 249275.Google Scholar
Javitt, D. C.(2009b). Sensory Processing in Schizophrenia: Neither Simple nor Intact. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35, 10591064.Google Scholar
Jensen, J., and Kapur, S. (2009). Salience and Psychosis: Moving from Theory to Practise: A Commentary on: “Do Patients with Schizophrenia Exhibit Aberrant Salience?” by Roiser et al. (2008). Psychological Medicine, 39(2), 197198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, S. L., Edge, M. D., Holmes, M. K., and Carver, C. S. (2012). The Behavioral Activation System and Mania. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 243267.Google Scholar
Jolliffe, T., and Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). A Test of Central Coherence Theory: Linguistic Processing in High-Functioning Adults with Autism or Asperger Syndrome: Is Local Coherence Impaired? Cognition, 71, 149185.Google Scholar
Jones, V., and Steel, C. (2012). Schizotypal Personality and Vulnerability to Involuntary Autobiographical Memories. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43, 871876.Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., and Haier, R. J. (2007). The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of Intelligence: Converging Neuroimaging Evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(2), 135154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kana, R. K., Libero, L. E., Hu, C. P., Deshpande, H. D., and Colburn, J. S. (2014). Functional Brain Networks and White Matter Underlying Theory-of-Mind in Autism. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 98105.Google Scholar
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic Disturbances of Affective Contact. The Nervous Child, 2, 217250. Reprinted (1968) in Acta Paedopsychiatrica, 35, 100–136.Google Scholar
Karbasforoushan, H., and Woodward, N. D. (2012). Resting-State Networks in Schizophrenia. Current Topics in Medicinal Chemistry, 12, 24042414.Google Scholar
Kasari, C., Chang, Y. C., and Patterson, S. (2013). Pretending to Play or Playing to Pretend: The Case of Autism. American Journal of Play, 6(1), 124.Google Scholar
Kennedy, D. P., and Courchesne, E. (2008). Functional Abnormalities of the Default Network during Self- and Other-Reflection in Autism. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3, 177190.Google Scholar
King, M. J., Williams, L., MacDougall, A. G., et al. (2011). Patients with Bipolar Disorder Show a Selective Deficit in the Episodic Simulation of Future Events. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 18011807.Google Scholar
Krapohl, E., Euesden, J., Zabaneh, D., et al. (2016). Phenome-Wide Analysis of Genome-Wide Polygenic Scores. Molecular Psychiatry, 21(9), 1188.Google Scholar
Kunihira, Y., Senju, A., Dairoku, H., Wakabayashi, A., and Hasegawa, T. (2006). “Autistic” Traits in Non-Autistic Japanese Populations: Relationships with Personality Traits and Cognitive Ability. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(4), 553566.Google Scholar
Kyaga, S. (2014). Creativity and Mental Illness: The Mad Genius in Question. Hampshire, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Landin-Romero, R., McKenna, P. J., Salgado-Pineda, P., et al. (2014). Failure of Deactivation in the Default Mode Network: A Trait Marker for Schizophrenia? Psychological Medicine, 45(6), 13151325.Google Scholar
MacCabe, J. H., Sariaslan, A., Almqvist, C., et al. (2018). Artistic Creativity and Risk for Schizophrenia, Bipolar Disorder and Unipolar Depression: A Swedish Population-Based Case–Control Study and Sib-Pair Analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 212(6), 370376.Google Scholar
Markram, K., and Markram, H. (2010). The Intense World Theory – A Unifying Theory of the Neurobiology of Autism. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4 : 224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, N.L., Collins, K. P., Thakkar, K. N., and Park, S. (2014). Visuospatial Imagery and Working Memory in Schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 19, 1735.Google Scholar
McGill, B., and Moulds, M. L. (2014). Characteristics of Autobiographical Memories and Prospective Imagery across a Spectrum of Hypomanic Personality Traits. Memory, 22, 11391148.Google Scholar
Meyer, M. L., Davachi, L., Ochsner, K. N., and Lieberman, M. D. (2019). Evidence that Default Network Connectivity during Rest Consolidates Social Information. Cerebral Cortex, 29(5), 19101920. doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy071.Google Scholar
Meyer, T. D., Finucane, L., and Jordan, G. (2011). Is Risk for Mania Associated with Increased Daydreaming as a Form of Mental Imagery? Journal of Affective Disorders, 135, 380383.Google Scholar
Mottron, L., Dawson, M., and Soulières, I. (2009). Enhanced Perception in Savant Syndrome: Patterns, Structure and Creativity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364, 13851391.Google Scholar
Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., and Burack, J. A. (2006). Enhanced Perceptual Functioning in Autism: An Update, and Eight Principles of Autistic Perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 2743.Google Scholar
Mullally, S. L., and Maguire, E. A. (2013). Memory, Imagination, and Predicting the Future: A Common Brain Mechanism? Neuroscientist, 20, 220234.Google Scholar
Nesse, R. M. (2000). Is Depression an Adaptation? Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(1), 1420.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (2001). Strong Imagination: Madness, Creativity and Human Nature. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Neufeld, J., Kuja-Halkola, R., Mevel, K., et al. (2017). Alterations in Resting State Connectivity Along the Autism Trait Continuum: A Twin Study. Molecular Psychiatry [Epub ahead of print]. doi:10.1038/mp.2017.160.Google Scholar
Peeters, S. C., van de Ven, V., Gronenschild, E. H., et al. (2015). Default Mode Network Connectivity as a Function of Familial and Environmental Risk for Psychotic Disorder. PLoS One, 10, e0120030.Google Scholar
Pignon, B., Geoffroy, P. A., Gharib, A., et al. (2018). Very Early Hallucinatory Experiences: A School‐Based Study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59(1), 6875.Google Scholar
Power, R. A., Steinberg, S., Bjornsdottir, G., et al. (2015). Polygenic Risk Scores for Schizophrenia and Bipolar Disorder Predict Creativity. Nature Neuroscience, 18(7), 953.Google Scholar
Raffard, S., D’Argembeau, A., Lardi, C., et al. (2010). Narrative Identity in Schizophrenia. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 328340.Google Scholar
Roth, I. (2007). Autism and the Imaginative Mind. In Roth, I (ed.), Proceedings of the British Academy: Imaginative Minds. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 277306.Google Scholar
Sahyoun, C. P., Belliveau, J. W., Soulières, I., Schwartz, S., and Mody, M. (2010). Neuroimaging of the Functional and Structural Networks Underlying Visuospatial vs. Linguistic Reasoning in High-Functioning Autism. Neuropsychologia, 48, 8695.Google Scholar
Santarnecchi, E., Emmendorfer, A., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2017). Dissecting the Parieto-Frontal Correlates of Fluid Intelligence: A Comprehensive ALE Meta-Analysis Study. Intelligence, 63, 928.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., et al. (2012). The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain. Neuron, 76, 677694.Google Scholar
Scott, F. J., and Baron-Cohen, S. (1996). Imagining Real and Unreal Things: Evidence of a Dissociation in Autism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 371382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simeonova, D. I., Chang, K. D., Strong, C., and Ketter, T. A. (2005). Creativity in Familial Bipolar Disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 39(6), 623631.Google Scholar
Slotnick, S. D., Thompson, W. L., and Kosslyn, S. M. (2012). Visual Memory and Visual Mental Imagery Recruit Common Control and Sensory Regions of the Brain. Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 1420.Google Scholar
Snyder, A. W., and Thomas, M. (1997). Autistic Artists Give Clues to Cognition. Perception, 26, 9396.Google Scholar
Soulières, I., Dawson, M., Samson, F., et al. (2009). Enhanced Visual Processing Contributes to Matrix Reasoning in Autism. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 40824107.Google Scholar
Spek, A. A., and Velderman, E. (2013). Examining the Relationship between Autism Spectrum Disorders and Technical Professions in High Functioning Adults. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7, 606612.Google Scholar
Spencer, M. D., Chura, L. R., Holt, R. J., et al. (2012). Failure to Deactivate the Default Mode Network Indicates a Possible Endophenotype of Autism. Molecular Autism, 3, 19.Google Scholar
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Hashizume, H., et al. (2011). Failing to Deactivate: The Association between Brain Activity during a Working Memory Task and Creativity. NeuroImage, 55, 681687.Google Scholar
Tanweer, T., Rathbone, C. J., and Souchay, C. (2010). Autobiographical Memory, Autonoetic Consciousness, and Identity in Asperger Syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 48(4), 900908.Google Scholar
Tavassoli, T., Miller, L. J., Schoen, S. A., Nielsen, D. M., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2014). Sensory Over-Responsivity in Adults with Autism Spectrum Conditions. Autism, 18(4), 428432.Google Scholar
Taylor, C. L. (2017). Creativity and Mood Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(6), 10401076.Google Scholar
Taylor, E. H. (1998). Advances in the Diagnosis and Treatment of Children with Serious Mental Illness. Child Welfare, 77(3), 311332.Google Scholar
Taylor, M., and Carlson, S. M. (1997). The Relation between Individual Differences in Fantasy and Theory of Mind. Child Development, 68, 436455.Google Scholar
Ten Eycke, K. D., and Müller, U. (2015). Brief Report: New Evidence for a Social-Specific Imagination Deficit in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 213220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terrett, G., Rendell, P. G., Raponi-Saunders, S., et al. (2013). Episodic Future Thinking in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 25582568.Google Scholar
van Os, J. (2009). “Salience Syndrome” Replaces “Schizophrenia” in DSM-V and ICD-11: Psychiatry’s Evidence-Based Entry into the 21st Century? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 120, 363372.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (2004). Imagination and Creativity in Childhood. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 42, 797.Google Scholar
Wagner-Martin, L. (1987). Sylvia Plath: A Biography. New York, NY: St. Martin’s Griffin.Google Scholar
Warrier, V., Toro, R., Chakrabarti, B., et al. (2018). Genome-Wide Analyses of Self-Reported Empathy: Correlations with Autism, Schizophrenia, and Anorexia Nervosa. Translational Psychiatry, 8(1), 35.Google Scholar
Wible, C. G. (2012a). Schizophrenia as a Disorder of Social Communication. Schizophrenia Research and Treatment, 2012, 920485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wible, C. G.(2012b). Hippocampal Temporal-Parietal Junction Interaction in the Production of Psychotic Symptoms: A Framework for Understanding the Schizophrenic Syndrome. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6.Google Scholar
Winfield, H., and Kamboj, S. K. (2010). Schizotypy and Mental Time Travel. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 321327.Google Scholar
Wise, R. J. S., and Braga, R. M. (2014). Default Mode Network: The Seat of Literary Creativity? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 116117.Google Scholar
Wolfberg, P. J. (2009). Play and Imagination in Children with Autism. Shawnee Mission, KS: AAPC Publishing.Google Scholar
Woodard, C. R., Chung, J., and Korn, M. (2014). A Pilot Study of the Meta-Play Method: A Novel Play Intervention for Toddlers with Autism. Journal of Autism, 1, 3.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×