Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-7drxs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T11:47:57.497Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 June 2023

Maria Garraffa
Affiliation:
University of East Anglia
Antonella Sorace
Affiliation:
University of Edinburgh
Maria Vender
Affiliation:
Università degli Studi di Verona
John W. Schwieter
Affiliation:
Wilfrid Laurier University, Ontario
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Bilingualism Matters
Language Learning Across the Lifespan
, pp. 127 - 148
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abada, S., Baum, S., & Titone, D. (2008). The effects of contextual strength on phonetic identification in younger and older listeners. Experimental Ageing Research, 34(3), 232250. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610730802070183Google Scholar
Abu Rabia, S., & Sanitsky, E. (2010). Advantages of bilinguals over monolinguals in learning a third language. Bilingual Research Journal, 33(2), 73199. https://doi.org/10.1080/15235882.2010.502797Google Scholar
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. (2008). Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: Neural evidence for language switching studies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(4), 557582. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960801920602Google Scholar
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515530. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377Google Scholar
Abutalebi, J., Guidi, L., Borsa, V., Canini, M., Della Rosa, P., Parris, B., & Weekes, B. (2015). Bilingualism provides a neural reserve for aging populations. Neuropsychologia, 69, 201210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.040Google Scholar
Abutalebi, J., Tettamanti, M., & Perani, D. (2009). The bilingual brain: Linguistic and non-linguistic skills. Brain and Language, 109(2–3), 5154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2009.04.001Google Scholar
Alladi, S., Bak, T., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A., Chaudhuri, J., & Kaul, S. (2013). Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology, 81(22), 19381944. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000436620.33155.a4Google Scholar
Alladi, S., Bak, T., Mekala, S., Rajan, A., Chaudhuri, J., Mioshi, E., Krovvidi, R., Surampudi, B., Duggirala, V., & Kaul, S. ( 2015). Impact of bilingualism on cognitive outcome after stroke. Stroke, 47(1), 258261. https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.115.010418CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. (2014). DSM-5 Manuale diagnostico e statistico dei disturbi mentali (5th ed.). Milan, Italy: Raffaello Cortina Editore.Google Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S., & Meir, N. (2016). Diagnostic accuracy of repetition tasks for the identification of specific language impairment in bilingual children: Evidence from Russian and Hebrew. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 51(6), 715731. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12242Google Scholar
Arosio, F., Branchini, C., Barbieri, L., & Guasti, M. (2014). Failure to produce direct object clitic pronouns as a clinical marker of SLI in school-aged Italian speaking children. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 28(9), 639663. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2013.877081Google Scholar
August, D., & Shanahan, T. (2006). Developing Literacy in Second-Language Learners: Lessons from the Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children and Youth. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bak, T., Long, M. R., Vega-Mendoza, M., & Sorace, A. (2016). Novelty, challenge, and practice: The impact of intensive language learning on attentional functions. PLoS ONE. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0153485Google Scholar
Baker, C. (2011). Foundations of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism (5th ed.). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Barac, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: Role of language, cultural background, and education. Child Development, 83(2), 413422.Google Scholar
Bard, E. G., Robertson, D., & Sorace, A. (1996). Magnitude estimation of linguistic acceptability. Language, 72(1), 3268. https://doi.org/10.2307/416793Google Scholar
Baum, S., & Titone, D. (2014). Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(5), 857894.Google Scholar
Berkes, M., & Bialystok, E. (2022). Bilingualism as a contributor to cognitive reserve: What it can do and what it cannot do. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease & Other Dementias, 37(0), 19. https://doi.org/10.1177/15333175221091417Google Scholar
Berko, J. (1958). The child’s learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150177.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2021). Bilingualism: Pathway to cognitive reserve. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5(5), 355364.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craig, F., & Freedman, M. (2007). Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45, 459464.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(4), 859873.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & DePape, A. (2009). Musical expertise, bilingualism, and executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(2), 565574. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012735Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Herman, J. (1999). Does bilingualism matter for early literacy? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2(1), 3544. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728999000139Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(4), 525531. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990423Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Martin, M., & Viswanathan, M. (2005). Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9(1), 103119. https://doi.org/10.1177/13670069050090010701Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Peets, K. F., & Moreno, S. (2014). Producing bilinguals through immersion education: Development of metalinguistic awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(1), 177191. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716412000288Google Scholar
Bohman, T., Bedore, L., Peña, E. D., Mendez-Perez, A., Gillam, R. (2010). What they hear and what you say: Language performance in Spanish-English bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism and Bilingual Education, 13(3), 325344. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050903342019Google Scholar
Bortolini, U., Arfé, B., Caselli, M., Degasperi, L., Deevy, P., & Leonard, L. (2006). Clinical marker for specific language impairment in Italian: The contribution of clitics and non-word repetition. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41(6), 695712. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820600570831Google Scholar
Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2001). Evidence of early language discrimination abilities in infants from bilingual environments. Infancy, 2(1), 2949. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0201_3Google Scholar
Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2003). Simultaneous bilingualism and the perception of a language-specific vowel contrast in the first year of life. Language and Speech, 46(2–3), 217243. https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309030460020801Google Scholar
Braver, T. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: A dual mechanisms framework. Trends in cognitive sciences, 16(2), 106113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010Google Scholar
Bunta, F., & Douglas, M. (2013). The effects of dual-language support on the language skills of bilingual children with hearing loss who use listening devices relative to their monolingual peers. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 44(3), 281290. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2013/12-0073)Google Scholar
Bunta, F., Douglas, M., Dickson, H., Cantu, A., Wickesberg, J., & Gifford, R. H. (2016). Dual language versus English-only support for bilingual children with hearing loss who use cochlear implants and hearing aids. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 51(4), 460472. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12223Google Scholar
Burgoyne, K., Duff, F. J., Clarke, P. J., Buckley, S., Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2012). Efficacy of a reading and language intervention for children with Down syndrome: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 53(10), 10441053. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02557.xGoogle Scholar
Burgoyne, K., Duff, F., Snowling, M., Nielsen, D., & Ulicheva, A. (2016). Bilingualism and biliteracy in Down syndrome: Insights from a case study. Language Learning, 66(4), 945971. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12179Google Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2009). Monolingual, bilingual, trilingual: Infants’ language experience influences the development of a word-learning heuristic. Developmental Science, 12(5), 815823. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00902.xGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., Burns, T., & Werker, J. (2010). The roots of bilingualism in newborns. Psychological Science, 21(3), 343348. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609360758Google Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., Morin-Lessard, E., & Lew-Williams, C. (2017). Bilingual infants control their languages as they listen. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(34), 90329037. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1703220114Google Scholar
Callan, D., Jones, J., Callan, A., & Akahane-Yamada, R. (2004). Phonetic perceptual identification by native- and second-language speakers differentially activates brain regions involved with acoustic phonetic processing and those involved with articulatory-auditory/orosensory internal models. NeuroImage, 22(3), 11821194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.006Google Scholar
Cappa, S. (2012). Imaging semantics and syntax. NeuroImage, 61(2), 427431. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.006Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11, 282298.Google Scholar
Catani, M., & Mesulam, M. (2008). The arcuate fasciculus and the disconnection theme in language and aphasia: History and current state. Cortex, 44(8), 953961. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2008.04.002Google Scholar
Catani, M., Allin, M., Husain, M., Pugliese, L., Mesulam, M., Murray, R., & Jones, D. (2007). Symmetries in human brain language pathways correlate with verbal recall. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(43), 1716317168. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0702116104Google Scholar
Chapman, R. (1995) Language development in children and adolescents with Down Syndrome. In Fletcher, B. & MacWhinney, B. (Eds.), Handbook of Child Language (pp. 641663). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Charity Hudley, A., Mallinson, C., Sudler, K., & Fama, M. (2018). The sociolinguistically trained speech-language pathologist: Using knowledge of African American English to aid and empower African American clientele. SIG 1 Language Learning and Education, 3(1), 118131.Google Scholar
Cleave, P., Kay-Raining Bird, E., Trudeau, N., & Sutton, A. (2014). Syntactic bootstrapping in children with Down syndrome: The impact of bilingualism. Journal of Communication Disorders, 49, 4254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.02.006Google Scholar
Conant, L., Liebenthal, E., Desai, A., & Binder, J. (2013). fMRI of phonemic perception and its relationship to reading development in elementary- to middle-school-age children. NeuroImage, 95, 345346.Google Scholar
Consonni, M., Cafiero, R., Marin, D., Tettamanti, M., Iadanza, A., Fabbro, F., & Perani, D. (2013). Neural convergence for language comprehension and grammatical class production in highly proficient bilinguals is independent of age of acquisition. Cortex, 49(5), 12521258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.04.009Google Scholar
Cordin, P. (2013). With our best future in mind: Lo sviluppo bilingue di bambini con L1 minoritaria. In Tempesta, I.., & Vedovelli, M. (eds.), Di Linguistica e di Sociolinguistica, Bulzoni (159–166). Roma.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Foucart, A., Arnon, I., Aparici, M., & Apesteguia, J. (2014). “Piensa” twice: on the foreign language effect in decision making. Cognition, 130(2), 236254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.010Google Scholar
Craik, F., Bialystok, E., & Freedman, M. (2010). Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease: bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology, 75(19), 17261729. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181fc2a1cGoogle Scholar
Crescentini, C., & Fabbro, F. (2014). Neuropsicologia del bilinguismo nei bambini. Trieste: Jezik-Lingua.Google Scholar
Crowley, C. J., Guest, K., & Sudler, K. (2015). Cultural competence needed to distinguish disorder from difference: Beyond Kumbaya. SIG 14 Perspectives on Communication Disorders and Sciences in Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Populations, 22(2), 6476.Google Scholar
Cummins, J. (1984). Bilingualism and Special Education: Issues in Assessment and Pedagogy. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Cummins, J. (2000). Language, Power, and Pedagogy: Bilingual Children in the Crossfire. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. https://doi.org/10.21832/9781853596773Google Scholar
Cummins, J. (2008). BICS and CALP: Empirical and theoretical status of the distinction. In Street, B. & Hornberger, N. (eds.), Encyclopedia of Language and Education (2nd ed.) (pp. 7183). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Dale, P., Dionne, G., Eley, T., & Plomin, R. (2000), Lexical and grammatical development: A behavioural genetic perspective. Journal of Child Language, 27(3), 619–42. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900004281Google Scholar
Davidson, D., & Tell, D. (2005). Monolingual and bilingual children’s use of mutual exclusivity in the naming of whole objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92(1), 2545. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2005.03.007Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2007). Parental language input patterns and children’s bilingual status. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28 (3), 411424. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716407070221Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2014). The absolute frequency of maternal input to bilingual and monolingual children: A first comparison. In Grüter, T. & Paradis, J. (eds.), Input and Experience in Bilingual Development (pp. 3758). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Dekhtyar, M., Swathi, K., & Gray, T. (2020). Is bilingualism protective for adults with aphasia? Neuropsychologia, 139, 107355. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107355Google Scholar
Del Maschio, N., & Abutalebi, J. (2019). Language Organization in the Bilingual and Multilingual Brain. In Schwieter, J. W. (ed.), The Handbook of the Neuroscience of Multilingualism (pp. 199213). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
DeLuca, V., Segaert, K., Mazaheri, A., & Krott, A. (2020). Understanding bilingual brain function and structure changes? U bet! A unified bilingual experience trajectory model. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 56(100930). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100930Google Scholar
Deriaz, M., Pelizzone, M., & Pérez Fornos, A. (2014). Simultaneous development of 2 oral languages by child cochlear implant recipients. Otology & neurotology: official publication of the American Otological Society, American Neurotology Society [and] European Academy of Otology and Neurotology, 35(9), 15411544. https://doi.org/10.1097/MAO.0000000000000497Google Scholar
Deuchar, M., & Quay, S. (2000). Bilingual Acquisition: Theoretical Implications of a Case Study. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dickson, E, Manderson, L, Obregón, M., & Garraffa, M 2021, Tracking biliteracy skills in students attending Gaelic medium education: Effects of learning experience on overall reading skills. Languages, 6(1), 55. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6010055Google Scholar
Directorate-General for communication & European Commission (2012). Europeans and Their Languages. Special Eurobarometer 386. https://op.europa.eu/en/publication-detail/-/publication/f551bd64-8615-4781-9be1-c592217dad83Google Scholar
Dobel, C., Lagemann, L., & Zwitserlood, P. (2009). Non-native phonemes in adult word learning: Evidence from the N400m. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological Sciences, 364(1536), 36973709.Google Scholar
Döpke, S. (1998). Competing language structures: The acquisition of verb placement by bilingual German-English children. Journal of Child Language, 25 (3), 555584. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000998003584Google Scholar
Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc.Google Scholar
Edgin, J. O., Kumar, A., Spanò, G., & Nadel, L. (2011). Neuropsychological effects of second language exposure in Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research: JIDR, 55(3), 351356. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01362.xGoogle Scholar
Emmorey, K., Borinstein, H., Thompson, R., & Gollan, T. (2008). Bimodal bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(1), 4361. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728907003203Google Scholar
Evans, M., & Shaw, D. (2008). Home grown for reading: Parental contributions to young children’s emergent literacy and word recognition. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 49(2), 8995. https://doi.org/10.1037/0708-5591.49.2.89Google Scholar
Fabbro, F., & Marini, A. (2010). Diagnosi e valutazione dei disturbi del linguaggio in bambini bilingui. In Vicari, S. & Caselli, M. (eds.), Neuropsicologia dello sviluppo (pp. 119132). Bologna: Il Mulino.Google Scholar
Fabbro, F., & Paradis, M. (1995). Differential Impairments in Four Multilingual Patients with Subcortical Lesions. In Paradis, M. (ed.), Aspects of Bilingual Aphasia, Pergamon Press (pp. 139176). Oxford.Google Scholar
Fabiano-Smith, L., & Barlow, J. (2010). Interaction in bilingual phonological acquisition: Evidence from phonetic inventories. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13(1), 8197. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050902783528Google Scholar
Faroqi-Shaha, Y., Frymark, T., Mullen, R., & Wang, B. (2010). Effect of treatment for bilingual individuals with aphasia: A systematic review of the evidence. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 23(4), 319341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2010.01.002Google Scholar
Favaro, G. (ed.) (2011). Dare parole al mondo. L’italiano dei bambini stranieri. Bergamo: Edizioni Junior-Spaggiari.Google Scholar
Feltmate, K., & Kay-Raining Bird, E. (2008). Language learning in four bilingual children with Down syndrome: A detailed analysis of vocabulary and morphosyntax. Canadian Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, 32, 619.Google Scholar
Flege, J., Mackay, I., & Piske, T. (2002). Assessing bilingual dominance. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 6798.Google Scholar
Garraffa, M., Beveridge, M., & Sorace, A. (2015). Linguistic and cognitive skills in Sardinian–Italian bilingual children. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 1898. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01898Google Scholar
Garraffa, M., & Grillo, N. (2008). Canonicity effects as grammatical phenomena. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(2), 177197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2007.09.001Google Scholar
Garraffa, M., Obregon, M., O’Rourke, B., & Sorace, A. (2020). Language and cognition in Gaelic-English young adult bilingual speakers: A positive effect of school immersion program on attentional and grammatical skills. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 570587.Google Scholar
Garraffa, M., Obregon, M., & Sorace, A. (2017). Linguistic and cognitive effects of bilingualism with regional minority languages: A study of Sardinian-Italian adult speakers. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1907. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01907Google Scholar
Garraffa, M., Vender, M., Sorace, A., & Guasti, M. T. (2019). Is it possible to differentiate multilingual children and children with Developmental Language Disorder? Languages, Society and Policy. https://doi.org/10.17863/CAM.37928Google Scholar
Garrett, D., Macdonald, S., & Craik, F. (2012). Intraindividual reaction time variability is malleable: Feedback- and education-related reductions in variability with age. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 101. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00101Google Scholar
Genesee, F., Nicoladis, E., & Paradis, J. (1995). Language differentiation in early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 22, 611631. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900009971Google Scholar
Giussani, C., Roux, FE., Lubrano, V., Gaini, S., & Bello, L. (2007). Review of language organisation in bilingual patients: What can we learn from direct brain mapping? Acta Neurochir (Wien) 149, 11091116. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00701-007-1266-2Google Scholar
Golestani, N., Molko, N., Dehaene, S., LeBihan, D., & Pallier, C. (2007). Brain structure predicts the learning of foreign speech sounds. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 575582.Google Scholar
Gonzalez-Barrero, A., & Nadig, A. (2019). Can bilingualism mitigate set-shifting difficulties in children with autism spectrum disorders? Child Development, 90(4), 10431060. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12979Google Scholar
Green, D. (1986). Control, activation and resource: A framework and a model for the control of speech in bilinguals. Brain and Language, 27(2), 210223. https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(86)90016-7Google Scholar
Green, D. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 6781. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728998000133Google Scholar
Green, D. (2003). The neural basis of the lexicon and the grammar in L2 acquisition. In Van Hout, R.., Hulk, A.., Kuiken, F.., & Towell, R. (eds.), The Interface Between Syntax and the Lexicon in Second Language Acquisition (pp. 197218). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Grimaldi, M. (2017). L’efficacia dell’istruzione scolastica nell’apprendimento fonetico-fonologico della L2: prospettive d’indagine e applicazioni didattiche. Studi Italiani di Linguistica Teorica e Applicata (silta), 45(1), 109126.Google Scholar
Grimaldi, M. (2019). Il cervello fonologico. Rome: Carocci.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F., & L. P. (eds.) (2013). The Psycholinguistics of Bilingualism. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (1998). Studying bilinguals: Methodological and conceptual issues. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 1, 131149.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (2008). Studying Bilinguals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Grüter, T. (2005). Comprehension and production of French object clitics by child second language learners and children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26(3), 363391. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716405050216Google Scholar
Guthrie, J. (2004). Teaching for literacy engagement. Journal of Literacy Research, 36, 130.Google Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V., Simon-Cereijido, G., & Wagner, C. (2008). Bilingual children with language impairment: A comparison with monolinguals and second language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29(1), 320. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716408080016Google Scholar
Hambly, C., & Fombonne, E. (2012). The impact of bilingual environments on language development in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 13421352. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1365-zGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, L., Hayiou-Thomas, M., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. (2016). The home literacy environment as a predictor of the early literacy development of children at family-risk of dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20(5), 401419.Google Scholar
Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2000). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of speech perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 131138. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364–6613(00)01463-7Google Scholar
Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). Opinion: The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(5), 393402. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2113Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2018). Bilingual development in children of immigrant families. Child development perspectives, 12(2), 8086. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12262Google Scholar
Howard, K., Gibson, J., & Katsos, N. (2021). Parental perceptions and decisions regarding maintaining bilingualism in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 51, 179192. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04528-xGoogle Scholar
Hurtado, A., & Vega, L. (2004). Shift happens: Spanish and English transmission between parents and their children. Journal of Social Issues, 60, 137155.Google Scholar
Illes, J., Francis, W., Desmond, J., Gabrieli, J., Glover, G., Poldrack, R., Lee, J. & Wagner, A. (1999). Convergent cortical representation of semantic processing in bilinguals. Brain and Language, 70, 347363.Google Scholar
Johnson, J., & Newport, E. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21(1), 6099. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0Google Scholar
Kastenbaum, J., Bedore, L., Peña, E., Sheng, L., Mavis, I., Sebastian-Vaytadden, R., et al. (2019). The influence of proficiency and language combination on bilingual lexical access. Bilingualism, 22, 300330. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918000366Google Scholar
Kavé, G., Eyal, N., Shorek, A., & Cohen-Mansfield, J. (2008). Multilingualism and cognitive state in the oldest old. Psychology and Aging, 23(1), 7078. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.70Google Scholar
Kay-RainingBird, E., Cleave, P., Trudeau, N., Thordardottir, E., Sutton, A., & Thorpe, A. (2005). The language abilities of bilingual children with Down syndrome. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 14, 187199. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2005/019)Google Scholar
Kenner, C., Kress, G., Hayat, A., Kam, R., & Tsai, K. (2004). Finding the keys to biliteracy: How young children interpret different writing systems. Language and Education, 18(2), 124144.Google Scholar
Klein, D., & Doctor, E. (2003). Patterns of developmental dyslexia in bilinguals. In Goulandris, N. (ed.), Dyslexia in different languages: Cross-linguistic comparisons (pp. 112136). Philadelphia, PA: Whurr.Google Scholar
Klimova, B. (2018). Learning a foreign language: A review on recent findings about its effect on the enhancement of cognitive functions among healthy older individuals. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12 (305). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00305Google Scholar
Kovács, A. (2009). Early bilingualism enhances mechanisms of false-belief reasoning. Developmental Science, 12, 4854. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00742.xGoogle Scholar
Kovelman, I., Bisconti, S., & Hoeft, F. (2016). Literacy & Dyslexia Revealed through Bilingual Brain Development. https://dyslexiaida.org/literacy-dyslexia-revealed-through-bilingual-brain-development/Google Scholar
Kremer-sadlik, T. (2005). To Be or Not to Be Bilingual: Autistic Children from Multilingual Families. In Cohen, J.., McAlister, K.., Rolstad, K. & MacSwan, J. (eds.). Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism (pp. 12251234). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Kroll, J., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(2), 149174.Google Scholar
Ku, A., Lachmann, E., & Nagler, W. (1996). Selective language aphasia from herpes simplex encephalitis. Pediatric Neurology, 15(2), 169171. https://doi.org/10.1016/0887-8994(96)00154-3Google Scholar
Kuhl, P., Stevens, E., Hayashi, A., Deguchi, T., Kiritani, S., & Iverson, P. (2006). Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental Science, 9(2), 1321. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00468.xGoogle Scholar
Kupisch, T., & Rothman, J. (2018). Terminology matters! Why difference is not incompleteness and how early child bilinguals are heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 22, 564582. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006916654355Google Scholar
Kuzmina, E., Goral, M., Norvik, M., & Weekes, B. S. (2019). What influences language impairment in bilingual aphasia? A Meta-Analytic Review. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 445. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00445Google Scholar
Lallier, M., Thierry, G., Barr, P., Carreiras, M., & Tainturier, M-J. (2018). Learning to read bilingually modulates the manifestations of dyslexia in adults. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22(4), 335349. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2018.1447942Google Scholar
Landerl, K., Ramus, F., Moll, K., Lyytinen, H., Leppänen, P., Lohvansuu, K., O’Donovan, M., Williams, J., Bartling, J., Bruder, J., et al. (2013). Predictors of developmental dyslexia in European orthographies with varying complexity. Journal of Child Psychology, Psychiatry, 54, 686694. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12029Google Scholar
Law, J., Boyle, J., Harris, F., Harkness, A., & Nye, C. (2000). Prevalence and natural history of primary speech and language delay: Findings from a systematic review of the literature. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 35(2), 165188. https://doi.org/10.1080/136828200247133Google Scholar
Leonard, L. (ed.) (2014). Children with Specific Language Impairment. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Leung, R. C., & Zakzanis, K. K. (2014). Brief report: Cognitive flexibility in autism spectrum disorders: A quantitative review. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 26282645.Google Scholar
Liu, C., Jiao, L., Timmer, K., & Wang, R. (2021). Structural brain changes with second language learning: A longitudinal voxel-based morphometry study. Brain and Language, 222, 105015.Google Scholar
Luise, M. (2006). L’italiano come seconda lingua. Elementi di didattica. UTET Libreria, Torino: Università.Google Scholar
Madrazo, A., & Bernardo, A.B.I. (2018). Measuring two types of inhibitory control in bilinguals and trilinguals: Is there a trilingual advantage? Psychological Studies, 63(1), 5260.Google Scholar
Maggu, A., Kager, R., Xu, S., & Wong, P. (2019). Complexity drives speech sound development: Evidence from artificial language training. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45(5), 628644.Google Scholar
Maneva, B., & Genesee, F. (2002). Bilingual babbling: Evidence for language differentiation in dual language acquisition. In Skarabela, B.., Fish, S., & Do, A. H.-J. (eds.), Proceedings of 26th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 383392). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Marchman, V., Martínez-Sussmann, C., & Dale, P. (2004). The language-specific nature of grammatical development: Evidence from bilingual language learners. Developmental Science, 7, 212224. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00340.xGoogle Scholar
Marinis, T., Armon-Lotem, S., & Pontikas, G. (2017). Language impairment in bilingual children: State of the art 2017. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7(34), 265276. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.7.3-4Google Scholar
Markman, E., & Wachtel, G. (1988). Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meaning of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 121–57.Google Scholar
Mårtensson, J., Eriksson, J., Bodammer, N. C., Lindgren, M., Johansson, M., Nyberg, L., & Lövdén, M. (2012). Growth of language-related brain areas after foreign language learning. NeuroImage, 63(1), 240244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.043Google Scholar
Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P., Lambertz, G., Halsted, N., Bertoncini, J., & Amiel-Tison, C. (1988). A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition, 29(2), 143178. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(88)90035-2Google Scholar
Melloni, C., Vender, M., & Delfitto, D. (2019). Nonword pluralization: evidence for an advantage of bilingualism in Albanian-Italian and Romanian-Italian bilingual children. In Slabakova, R.., Corbet, J.., Dominguez, L.., Dudley, A.., & Wallington, A. (eds.), Explorations in Second Language Acquisition and Processing (pp. 238250). Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Morgan-Short, K. (2014). Electrophysiological approaches to understanding second language acquisition: A field reaching its potential. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 34, 1536. https://doi.org/10.1017/S026719051400004XGoogle Scholar
Morgan-Short, K., Deng, Z., Brill-Schuetz, K., Faretta-Stutenberg, M., Wong, P., & Wong, F. (2015). A view of the neural representation of second language syntax through artificial language learning under implicit contexts of exposure. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37(2), 383419. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263115000030Google Scholar
Moro, A. (2016). The Boundaries of Babel: The Brain and the Enigma of Impossible Languages. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Muñoz, M., & Marquardt, T. (2003). Picture naming and identification in bilingual speakers of Spanish and English with and without aphasia. Aphasiology, 17(12), 11151132. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030344000427Google Scholar
Murineddu, M., Duca, V., & Cornoldi, C. (2006). Difficoltà di apprendimento degli studenti stranieri. Difficoltà di apprendimento, 12(1), 4970.Google Scholar
Nicoladis, E., & Secco, G. (2000). The role of a child’s productive vocabulary in the language choice of a bilingual family. First Language, 20, 328. https://doi.org/10.1177/014272370002005801Google Scholar
Nicolson, R., & Fawcett, J. (eds.) (2008). Dyslexia, Learning and the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ohashi, J. K., Mirenda, P., Marinova-Todd, S., Hambly, C., Fombonne, E., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., Roberts, W., Smith, I., Vaillancourt, T., Volden, J., Waddell, C., Zwaigenbaum, L., Georgiades, S., Duku, E., & Thompson, A. (2012). Comparing early language development in monolingual- and bilingual-exposed young children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(2), 890897. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.12.002Google Scholar
Okita, T. (2002). Invisible Work: Bilingualism, Language Choice and Childrearing in Intermarried Families. Amsterdam; Philadelphia: Benjamins Pub. Co.Google Scholar
Osterhout, L., Poliakov, A., Inoue, K., McLaughlin, J., Valentine, G., Pitkanen, I., Pitkanena, I., Frenck-Mestre, C., & Hirschensohn, J. (2008). Second-language learning and changes in the brain. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(6), 509521. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2008.01.001Google Scholar
Paap, K., & Sawi, O. (2016). The role of test-retest reliability in measuring individual and group differences in executive functioning. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 274, 8193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2016.10.002Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232258.Google Scholar
Pakulak, E., & Neville, H. J. (2010). Proficiency differences in syntactic processing of monolingual native speakers indexed by event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(12), 27282744. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21393Google Scholar
Paplikar, A., Mekala, S., Bak, T., Dahramkar, S., Alladi, S., & Kaul, S. (2017). Bilingualism and the severity of poststroke aphasia. Aphasiology, 33(1), 5872. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21393Google Scholar
Paradis, J. (2010). The interface between bilingual development and specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31(2), 227252. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716409990373Google Scholar
Paradis, J. (2011). Individual differences in child English second language acquisition: Comparing child-internal and child-external factors. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(3), 213237. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.1.3.01parGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Crago, M., Genesee, F., & Rice, M. (2003). Bilingual Children with Specific language impairment: How do they compare with their monolingual peers? Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 46(1), 115. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2003/009)Google Scholar
Paradis, J., Emmerzael, K., & Sorenson, T. (2010). Assessment of English language learners: Using parent report on first language development. Journal of Communication Disorders, 43(6), 474497. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.01.002Google Scholar
Paradis, J., & Genesee, F. (1996). Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or interdependent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 125. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263100014662Google Scholar
Paradis, M. (2001). Bilingual and polyglot aphasia. In Berndt, R. S. (ed.), Language and Aphasia (pp. 6991). Elsevier Science Publishers B.V., Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
Paulston, C. (1980). Bilingual Education: Theories and Issues. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Peace-Hughes, T., de Lima, P., Cohen, B., Jamieson, L., Tisdall, K., & Sorace, A. (2021). What do children think of their own bilingualism? Exploring bilingual children’s attitudes and perceptions. International Journal of Bilingualism, 25(5), 11831199. https://doi.org/10.1177/13670069211000853Google Scholar
Pearson, B., Fernández, S., & Oller, D. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43, 93120. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-1770.1993.tb00174.xGoogle Scholar
Penn, C., Frankel, T., Watermeyer, J., & Russell, N. (2010). Executive functions and cognitive strategies in bilingual aphasia. Aphasiology, 24(2), 288308. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030902958399Google Scholar
Perani, D., & Abutalebi, J. (2005). The neural basis of first and second language processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15(2), 202206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.007Google Scholar
Perani, D., & Abutalebi, J. (2015). Bilingualism, dementia, cognitive and neural reserve. Current Opinions in Neurology, 28(6), 618625. https://doi.org/10.1097/WCO.0000000000000267Google Scholar
Perani, D., Abutalebi, J., Paulesu, E., Brambati, S., Scifo, P., Cappa, S., & Fazio, F. (2003). The role of age of acquisition and language usage in early, high proficient bilinguals: An FMRI study during verbal fluency. Human Brain Mapping, 19(3), 170182. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.10110Google Scholar
Perani, D., Dehaene, S., Grassi, F., Cohen, L., Cappa, S. F., Dupoux, E., Fazio, F., & Mehler, J. (1996). Brain processing of native and foreign languages. Neuroreport, 7(15–17), 24392444. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199611040-00007Google Scholar
Perani, D., Paulesu, E., Galles, N. S., Dupoux, E., Dehaene, S., Bettinardi, V., Cappa, S. F., Fazio, F., & Mehler, J. (1998). The bilingual brain. Proficiency and age of acquisition of the second language. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 121(Pt 10), 18411852. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/121.10.1841Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. (2003). The universal grammar of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7(1), 324. https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532799XSSR0701_02Google Scholar
Peristeri, E., Baldimtsi, E., Andreou, M., & Tsimpli, I. (2020). The impact of bilingualism on the narrative ability and the executive functions of children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Communication Disorders, 85, 105999. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2020.105999Google Scholar
Petersen, J. M., Marinova-Todd, S. H., & Mirenda, P. (2012). Brief report: An exploratory study of lexical skills in bilingual children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(7), 14991503. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1366-yGoogle Scholar
Pliatsikas, C., Johnstone, T., & Marinis, T. (2014a). Grey matter volume in the cerebellum is related to the processing of grammatical rules in a second language: A structural voxel-based morphometry study. Cerebellum, 13(1), 5563. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-013-0515-6Google Scholar
Pliatsikas, C., Johnstone, T., & Marinis, T. (2014b). fMRI Evidence for the involvement of the procedural memory system in morphological processing of a second language. PLoS ONE, 9(5), e97298. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0097298Google Scholar
Pliatsikas, C., Meteyard, L., Veríssimo, J., DeLuca, V., Shattuck, K., & Ullman, M. (2020). The effect of bilingualism on brain development from early childhood to young adulthood. Brain Structure and Function, 225, 21312152. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-020-02115-5Google Scholar
Pot, A., Keijzer, M., De Bot, K. (2018). Intensity of multilingual language use predicts cognitive performance in some multilingual older adults. Brain Sciences, 8(5), 92. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci8050092Google Scholar
Poulin-dubois, D., & Goodz, N. (2001). Language differentiation in bilingual infants: Evidence from babbling. In Cenoz, J. & Genesee, F. (eds.), Trends in Bilingual Acquisition (pp. 95106). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.1.06pouGoogle Scholar
Quine, W. (ed.) (1960). Word and Object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ramírez, N., Ramírez, R., Clarke, M., Taulu, S., & Kuhl, P. (2016). Speech discrimination in 11-month-old bilingual and monolingual infants: A magnetoencephalography study. Developmental Science, 20(1), e12427. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12427Google Scholar
Ramus, F., & Szenkovits, G. (2008). What phonological deficit? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(1), 129141. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210701508822Google Scholar
Reetzke, R., Zou, X., Sheng, L., & Katsos, N. (2015). Communicative development in bilingually exposed Chinese children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 58(3), 813825. https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-13-0258Google Scholar
Reyes, I. (2006). Exploring connections between emergent biliteracy and bilingualism. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 6(3), 267292. https://doi.org/10.1177/1468798406069801Google Scholar
Richardson, F., Thomas, M., Filippi, R., Harth, H., & Price, C. (2010). Contrasting effects of vocabulary knowledge on temporal and parietal brain structure across lifespan. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(5), 943954. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21238Google Scholar
Rothman, J. (2009). Understanding the nature and outcomes of early bilingualism: Romance languages as heritage languages. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13(2), 155163. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006909339814Google Scholar
Sakai, K., Miura, K., Narafu, N., & Muraishi, Y. (2004). Correlated functional changes of the prefrontal cortex in twins induced by classroom education of second language. Cerebral Cortex, 14(11), 12331239. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhh084Google Scholar
Saur, D., Kreher, B. W., Schnell, S., Kümmerer, D., Kellmeyer, P., Vry, M. S., Umarova, R., Musso, M., Glauche, V., Abel, S., Huber, W., Rijntjes, M., Hennig, J., & Weiller, C. (2008). Ventral and dorsal pathways for language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(46), 1803518040. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0805234105Google Scholar
Schmid, M., & Köpke, B. (2019). The Oxford Handbook of Language Attrition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Scortichini, F., Stella, G., & Morlini, I. (2012). La diagnosi di dislessia e disortografia evolutiva nei bambini bilingui (L2): Evidenze sul ruolo del lessico. Dislessia, 3, 319339.Google Scholar
Seeman, M. (2016). Bilingualism and schizophrenia. World Journal of Psychiatry, 6(2), 192198. https://dx.doi.org/10.5498/wjp.v6.i2.192Google Scholar
Semenza, C., Franzon, F., & Zanini, F. (2019). Il cervello morfologico. Rome: Carocci.Google Scholar
Serratrice, L., Sorace, A., & Paoli, S. (2004). Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax–pragmatics interface: Subjects and objects in English–Italian bilingual and monolingual acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7(3), 183206. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728904001610Google Scholar
Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. (1996). The domain of supervisory processes and temporal organization of behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 351, 14051411. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1996.0124Google Scholar
Shany, M., & Geva, E. (2012). Cognitive, language, and literacy development in socio-culturally vulnerable school children: The case of Ethiopian Israeli children. In Leikin, M.., Schwartz, M.., & Tobin, Y. (eds.), Current Issues in Bilingualism: Cognitive and Socio-linguistic Perspectives (pp. 77118). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Siegel, L. (2016). Bilingualism and dyslexia: The case of children learning English as an additional language. In Peer, L.. & Reid, G. (eds.), Multilingualism, Literacy and Dyslexia: Breaking Down Barriers for Educators (pp. 137147). Abingdon: Routledge.Google Scholar
Simon, J., & Rudell, A. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 51(3), 300304. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0020586Google Scholar
Soltero-González, L., Sparrow, W., Butvilofsky, S., Escamilla, K., & Hopewell, S. (2016). Effects of a paired literacy program on emerging bilingual children’s biliteracy outcomes in third grade. Journal of Literacy Research, 48(1), 80.Google Scholar
Sorace, A. (2010). Magnitude estimation in language acquisition research. In Unsworth, S. & Blom, E. (eds.), Experimental Methods in Language Acquisition Research (pp. 5772). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Sorace, A. (2011). Pinning down the concept of “interface”. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(1), 133. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.1.1.01sorGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A., Serratrice, L., Filiaci, F., & Baldo, M. (2009). Discourse conditions on subject pronoun realization: Testing the linguistic intuitions of older bilingual children. Lingua, 119(3), 460477. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2008.09.008Google Scholar
Soskey, L., Holcomb, P., & Midgley, K. (2016). Language effects in second-language learners: A longitudinal electrophysiological study of Spanish classroom learning. Brain Research, 1646, 4452. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2016.05.028Google Scholar
Stein, M., Winkler, C., Kaiser, A., & Dierks, T. (2014). Structural brain changes related to bilingualism: Does immersion make a difference? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1116. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01116Google Scholar
Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurology, 11(11), 1006–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474–4422(12)70191-6Google Scholar
Tannenbaum, M., & Berkovich, M. (2005). Family relations and language maintenance: Implications for language educational policies. Language Policy, 4, 287309. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10993-005-7557-7Google Scholar
Taylor, C., Hall, S., Manivannan, S., Mundil, N., & Border, S. (2022). The neuroanatomical consequences and pathological implications of bilingualism. Journal of Anatomy, 240(2), 410427. https://doi.org/10.1111/joa.13542Google Scholar
Thomas, E., El-kashlan, H., & Zwolan, T. (2008). Children with cochlear implants who live in monolingual and bilingual homes. Otology and Neurotology, 29, 230234.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I., Peristeri, E., & Andreu, M. (2017). Object clitic production in monolingual and bilingual children with specific language impairment: A comparison between elicited production and narratives. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7(3–4), 394430.Google Scholar
Unsworth, S. (2013). Assessing the role of current and cumulative exposure in simultaneous bilingual acquisition: The case of Dutch gender. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 86110.Google Scholar
Unsworth, S. (2013a). Current issues in multilingual first language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 33, 2150. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0267190513000044Google Scholar
Valenzuela, M., & Sachdev, P. (2009). Can cognitive exercise prevent the onset of dementia? Systematic review of randomized clinical trials with longitudinal follow-up. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 1(3), 179187. https://doi.org/10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181953b57Google Scholar
Vallar, G., & Papagno, C. (1993). Preserved vocabulary acquisition in Down’s syndrome: the role of phonological short-term memory. Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 29(3), 467483. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010–9452(13)80254-7Google Scholar
Vemuri, P., Lesnick, T., Przybelski, S., Machulda, M., Knopman, D., Mielke, M., Roberts, R., Geda, Y., Rocca, W., Petersen, R., & Jack, C. Jr. (2014). Association of lifetime intellectual enrichment with cognitive decline in the older population. JAMA Neurology, 71(8), 10171024. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.963Google Scholar
Vender, M., Krivochen, D. G., Phillips, B., Saddy, D., & Delfitto, D. (2019). Implicit learning, bilingualism, and dyslexia: Insights from a study assessing AGL with a modified Simon task. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1647. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01647Google Scholar
Vender, M. (ed.) (2017). Disentangling Dyslexia: Phonological and Processing Deficit in Developmental Dyslexia. Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Vender, M., Delfitto, D., & Melloni, C. (2018). Clitic production and bilingualism: When exposure matters. Languages, 3, 117. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages3030022Google Scholar
Vender, M., Delfitto, D., & Melloni, C. (2020). How do bilingual dyslexic and typically developing children perform in nonword repetition? Evidence from a study on Italian L2 children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(4), 884896. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000828Google Scholar
Vender, M., Garraffa, M., Sorace, G., & Guasti, M. (2016). How early L2 children perform on Italian clinical markers of SLI: A study of clitic production and nonword repetition. Clinical Linguistics and Phonology, 30, 150169. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2015.1120346Google Scholar
Vender, M., & Guasti, M. (2020). L’apprendimento della letto-scrittura nei bambini con Italiano L2. In Bidese, E.., Casalicchio, J.., & Moroni, M. (eds.), Linguistics: Views from the Alps: Language Theory, Didactics and Society (pp. 241267). Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Vender, M., Hu, S., Mantione, F., Delfitto, D., & Melloni, C. (2018b). The production of clitic pronouns: A study on bilingual and monolingual dyslexic children. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2301. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02301Google Scholar
Vender, M., Hu, S., Mantione, F., Savazzi, S., Delfitto, D., & Melloni, C. (2018a). Bilingualism, dyslexia and morphological awareness: Evidence for an advantage of bilingualism in dyslexia. Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 24(2), 118. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2018.1450355Google Scholar
Vingerhoets, G., Van Borsel, J., Tesink, C., van den Noort, M., Deblaere, K., Seurinck, R., Vandemaele, P., & Achten, E. (2003). Multilingualism: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 20(4), 21812196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.029Google Scholar
Voits, T., Robson, H., Rothman, J., & Pliatsikas, C. (2022). The effects of bilingualism on hippocampal volume in ageing bilinguals. Brain Structure and Function, 227, 979994.Google Scholar
Volterra, V. (2014). Chi ha paura della lingua dei segni? Psicologia Clinica dello Sviluppo, 3, 425477.Google Scholar
Volterra, V., & Taeschner, T. (1978). The acquisition and development of language by bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 5(2), 311326. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900007492Google Scholar
Voss, M. W., Heo, S., Prakash, R. S., Erickson, K. I., Alves, H., Chaddock, L., Szabo, A., Mailey, E., Wójcicki, T., White, S., Gothe, N., McAuley, E., Sutton, B., & Kramer, A. F. (2013). The influence of aerobic fitness on cerebral white matter integrity and cognitive function in older adults: Results of a one‐year exercise intervention. Human Brain mapping, 34(11), 29722985. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22119Google Scholar
Voss, M.W., Nagamatsu, L.S., Liu-Ambrose, T., & Kramer, A.F. (2011). Exercise, brain, and cognition across the life span. Journal of Applied Physiology, 111(5), 15051513.Google Scholar
Wartenburger, I., Heekeren, H. R., Abutalebi, J., Cappa, S. F., Villringer, A., & Perani, D. (2003). Early setting of grammatical processing in the bilingual brain. Neuron, 37(1), 159170. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896–6273(02)01150-9Google Scholar
Weikum, W. M., Vouloumanos, A., Navarra, J., Soto-Faraco, S., Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Visual language discrimination in infancy. Science, 316(5828), 1159. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1137686Google Scholar
Werker, J., & Tees, R. (2005). Speech perception as a window for understanding plasticity and commitment in language systems of the brain. Developmental Psychobiology, 46, 233251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dev.20060Google Scholar
Wingfield, A., & Tun, P. (2007). Cognitive supports and cognitive constraints on comprehension of spoken language. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 18(7), 548558. http://dx.doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.18.7.3Google Scholar
Winkler, I., Kujala, T., Tiitinen, H., Sivonen, P., Alku, P., Lehtokoski, A., Czigler, I., Csépe, V., Ilmoniemi, R. J., & Näätänen, R. (1999). Brain responses reveal the learning of foreign language phonemes. Psychophysiology, 36(5), 638642. https://doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3650638Google Scholar
Yip, V., & Matthews, S. (2007). The Bilingual Child: Early Development and Language Contact (Cambridge Approaches to Language Contact). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Zelazo, P. (2006). The dimensional change card sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1(1), 297301. https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2006.46Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×