Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-sjtt6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-27T12:53:42.407Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 16 - Heuristic Social Sampling

from Part V - Sampling as a Tool in Social Environments

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 June 2023

Klaus Fiedler
Affiliation:
Universität Heidelberg
Peter Juslin
Affiliation:
Uppsala Universitet, Sweden
Jerker Denrell
Affiliation:
University of Warwick
Get access

Summary

A person’s social network constitutes a rich sampling space for informing judgments about social statistics (e.g., the distribution of preferences, risks, or behaviors in the broader social environment). How is this sampling space searched and used to make inferences? This chapter gives an overview on research on the social-circle model, a computational process account of how people make inferences about relative event frequencies. The social-circle model is inspired by the notion of sequential and limited search in models of bounded rationality for multi-attribute decision making. In accord with research on the structure of social memory, the model assumes that social sampling proceeds by sequentially probing a person’s social circles – including oneself, family, friends, and acquaintances – and that search is constrained by a simple stopping rule. The social-circle model has several free parameters that enable it to capture individual differences in the order in which social circles are inspected, in noise during evidence evaluation, and in discrimination thresholds. We provide a step-by-step tutorial for deriving predictions of the social-circle model, review empirical tests of the model, illustrate how the model reflects individual differences in social sampling and differences in sampling across domains, and analyze the ecological rationality of heuristic social sampling.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, J. R., & Lebiere, C. J. (2014). The atomic components of thought. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bond, C. F., & Brockett, D. R. (1987). A social context-personality index theory of memory for acquaintances. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 11101121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bond, C. F., Jones, R. L., & Weintraub, D. L. (1985). On the unconstrained recall of acquaintances: A sampling-traversal model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(2), 327337.Google Scholar
Brewer, D. D. (1995). Patterns in the recall of persons in a department of a formal organization. Journal of Quantitative Anthropology, 5, 255284.Google Scholar
Bröder, A., & Schiffer, S. (2003). Take the best versus simultaneous feature matching: Probabilistic inferences from memory and effects of representation format. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(2), 277293.Google Scholar
Brown, G. D., Gardner, J., Oswald, A. J., & Qian, J. (2008). Does wage rank affect employees’ well-being? Industrial Relations, 47(3), 355389.Google Scholar
Clauset, A., Shalizi, C. R., & Newman, M. E. (2009). Power-law distributions in empirical data. SIAM Review, 51(4), 661703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawtry, R. J., Sutton, R. M., & Sibley, C. G. (2015). Why wealthier people think people are wealthier, and why it matters: From social sampling to attitudes to redistribution. Psychological Science, 26(9), 13891400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dougherty, M. R., Gettys, C. F., & Ogden, E. E. (1999). Minerva-DM: A memory processes model for judgments of likelihood. Psychological Review, 106(1), 180209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I., Arnaboldi, V., Conti, M., & Passarella, A. (2015). The structure of online social networks mirrors those in the offline world. Social Networks, 43, 3947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Einhorn, H. J. (1970). The use of nonlinear, noncompensatory models in decision making. Psychological Bulletin, 73(3), 221230.Google Scholar
Epstein, J. M. (2001). Learning to be thoughtless: Social norms and individual computation. Computational Economics, 18(1), 924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7(2), 117140.Google Scholar
Fiedler, K., & Juslin, P. (Eds.). (2006). Information sampling and adaptive cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fiske, A. P. (1995). Social schemata for remembering people: Relationships and person attributes in free recall of acquaintances. Journal of Quantitative Anthropology, 5(4), 305324.Google Scholar
Galesic, M., Olsson, H., & Rieskamp, J. (2012). Social sampling explains apparent biases in judgments of social environments. Psychological Science, 23(12), 15151523.Google Scholar
Galesic, M., Olsson, H., & Rieskamp, J. (2018). A sampling model of social judgment. Psychological Review, 125(3), 363390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gigerenzer, G., Hertwig, R., & Pachur, T. (Eds.). (2011). Heuristics: The foundations of adaptive behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Heathcote, A., Brown, S. D., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2015). An introduction to good practices in cognitive modeling. In Forstmann, B. U. & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (Eds.), An introduction to model-based cognitive neuroscience (pp. 2548). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Hecht, M., Pachur, T. & Schulze, C. (2022). Does social sampling differ between online and offline contacts? A computational modeling analysis. In J. Culbertson, A. Perfors, H. Rabagliati, & V. Ramenzoni (Eds.), Proceedings of the 44th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 319–325). Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Henrich, N., & Henrich, J. P. (2007). Why humans cooperate: A cultural and evolutionary explanation. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hertwig, R., Pachur, T., & Kurzenhäuser, S. (2005). Judgments of risk frequencies: Tests of possible cognitive mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(4), 621642.Google Scholar
Hewstone, M., & Lord, C. G. (1998). Intergroup behavior: The role of typicality. In Sedikides, C., Schopler, J., Insko, C. A., & Insko, C. (Eds.), Intergroup cognition and intergroup behavior (pp. 367392). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Hill, R. A., & Dunbar, R. I. (2003). Social network size in humans. Human Nature, 14(1), 5372.Google Scholar
Hills, T. T. (2019). The dark side of information proliferation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(3), 323330.Google Scholar
Hills, T. T., & Pachur, T. (2012(d)). Dynamic search and working memory in social recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(1), 218228.Google Scholar
Lichtenstein, S., Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B., Layman, M., & Combs, B. (1978). Judged frequency of lethal events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4(6), 551578.Google Scholar
McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Cook, J. M. (2001). Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27(1), 415444.Google Scholar
Newman, M. E. (2005). Power laws, Pareto distributions and Zipf’s law. Contemporary Physics, 46(5), 323351.Google Scholar
Nosofsky, R. M. (1986). Attention, similarity, and the identification–categorization relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115(1), 3957.Google Scholar
Pachur, T., Hertwig, R., & Rieskamp, J. (2013). Intuitive judgments of social statistics: How exhaustive does sampling need to be? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(6), 10591077.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pachur, T., Hertwig, R., & Steinmann, F. (2012). How do people judge risks: Availability heuristic, affect heuristic, or both? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 18(3), 314330.Google Scholar
Pachur, T., Schooler, L. J., & Stevens, J. R. (2014). We’ll meet again: Revealing distributional and temporal patterns of social contact. PLoS ONE, 9(1), e86081.Google Scholar
Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1993). The adaptive decision maker. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pitt, M. A., & Myung, I. J. (2002). When a good fit can be bad. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(10), 421425.Google Scholar
Rips, L. J. (1975). Inductive judgments about natural categories. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14(6), 665681.Google Scholar
Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The “false consensus effect”: An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13(3), 279301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schafer, M., & Schiller, D. (2018). Navigating social space. Neuron, 100(2), 476489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulze, C., Hertwig, R., & Pachur, T. (2021). Who you know is what you know: Modeling boundedly rational social sampling. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(2), 221241.Google Scholar
Schulze, C., Pachur, T., & Hertwig, R. (2017). How does instance-based inference about event frequencies develop? An analysis with a computational process model. In Gunzelmann, G., Howes, A., Tenbrink, T., & Davelaar, E. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 39th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1053–1058).Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1956). Rational choice and the structure of the Psychological Review, 63(2), 129138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E. R., & Zarate, M. A. (1992). Exemplar-based model of social judgment. Psychological Review, 99(1), 321.Google Scholar
Todd, P. M., Gigerenzer, G., & the ABC Research Group. (2012). Ecological rationality: Intelligence in the world. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Alexander, M. P., & Stuss, D. (1998). Clustering and switching on verbal fluency: The effects of focal frontal-and temporal-lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 36(6), 499504.Google Scholar
Tunçgenç, B., El Zein, M., & Sulik, J., et al. (2021). Social influence matters: We follow pandemic guidelines most when our close circle does. British Journal of Psychology, 112, 763780.Google Scholar
Van den Bos, W., & Hertwig, R. (2017). Adolescents display distinctive tolerance to ambiguity and to uncertainty during risky decision making. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 111.Google Scholar
Wald, A. (1950). Statistical decision functions. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wood, A. M., Brown, G. D., & Maltby, J. (2012). Social norm influences on evaluations of the risks associated with alcohol consumption: Applying the rank-based decision by sampling model to health judgments. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 47(1), 5762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×