Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-9pm4c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T21:59:28.527Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Real-life self-control conflicts in anorexia nervosa: An ecological momentary assessment investigation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 June 2022

Sophia Fürtjes
Affiliation:
Translational Developmental Neuroscience Section, Division of Psychological and Social Medicine and Developmental Neuroscience, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany Department of Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Maria Seidel
Affiliation:
Translational Developmental Neuroscience Section, Division of Psychological and Social Medicine and Developmental Neuroscience, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Stefan Diestel
Affiliation:
Schumpeter School of Business and Economics, Faculty of Economy, University of Wuppertal, Wuppertal, Germany
Max Wolff
Affiliation:
MIND Foundation, Berlin, Germany Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Charité Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Campus Charité Mitte, Berlin, Germany
Joseph A. King
Affiliation:
Translational Developmental Neuroscience Section, Division of Psychological and Social Medicine and Developmental Neuroscience, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Inger Hellerhoff
Affiliation:
Translational Developmental Neuroscience Section, Division of Psychological and Social Medicine and Developmental Neuroscience, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany Eating Disorder Research and Treatment Center, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Fabio Bernadoni
Affiliation:
Translational Developmental Neuroscience Section, Division of Psychological and Social Medicine and Developmental Neuroscience, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Katrin Gramatke
Affiliation:
Eating Disorder Research and Treatment Center, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Thomas Goschke
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Veit Roessner
Affiliation:
Eating Disorder Research and Treatment Center, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Stefan Ehrlich*
Affiliation:
Translational Developmental Neuroscience Section, Division of Psychological and Social Medicine and Developmental Neuroscience, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany Eating Disorder Research and Treatment Center, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
*
* Author for correspondence: Stefan Ehrlich, E-mail: transden.lab@uniklinikum-dresden.de

Abstract

Background

Individuals with anorexia nervosa (AN) are often thought to show heightened self-control and increased ability to inhibit desires. In addition to inhibitory self-control, antecedent-focused strategies (e.g., cognitive reconstrual—the re-evaluation of tempting situations) might contribute to disorder maintenance and enable disorder-typical, maladaptive behaviors.

Methods

Over a period of 14 days, 40 acutely underweight young female patients with anorexia nervosa (AN) and 40 healthy control (HC) participants reported their affect and behavior in self-control situations via ecological momentary assessment during inpatient treatment (AN) and everyday life (HC). Data were analyzed via hierarchical analyses (linear and logistic modeling).

Results

Conflict strength had a significantly lower impact on self-control success in AN compared to HC. While AN and HC did not generally differ in the number or strength of self-control conflicts or in the percentage of self-control success, AN reported self-controlled behavior to be less dependent on conflict strength.

Conclusions

While patients with AN were not generally more successful at self-control, they appeared to resolve self-control conflicts more effectively. These findings suggest that the magnitude of self-control conflicts has comparatively little impact on individuals with AN, possibly due to the use of antecedent-focused strategies. If confirmed, cognitive-behavioral therapy might focus on and help patients to exploit these alternative self-control strategies in the battle against their illness.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the European Psychiatric Association

Introduction

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is characterized by extreme restriction of food intake and rigid behaviors that serve to control many aspects of daily life related to eating and weight gain [Reference Schmidt and Treasure1, Reference Treasure and Schmidt2]. Individuals with AN are often thought to exercise excessive self-control to override food-related needs and desires in their relentless pursuit of thinness [Reference Casper3, Reference Fairburn, Shafran and Cooper4]. However, little is known about how patients with AN might actually accomplish the high levels of control conceivably required to reach and maintain severely low bodyweight or how they experience self-control situations in everyday life.

Currently, influential theories propose that high self-control is achieved via effortful inhibition of prepotent behavioral impulses and resisting desires which interfere with personal goals [Reference Kotabe and Hofmann5]. Indeed, previous research has shown that inhibitory control is associated with less consumption of fatty foods and snacks [Reference Hall6Reference Houben8], higher resistance to food desires, and successful weight loss [Reference Hofmann, Adriaanse, Vohs and Baumeister9]. Furthermore, a reduced activation in brain areas involved in inhibitory control (e.g., the right inferior frontal gyrus) has been shown to predict daily self-control failures, including food consumption [Reference Krönke, Wolff, Mohr, Kräplin, Smolka and Bühringer10, Reference Lopez, Hofmann, Wagner, Kelley and Heatherton11]. A plausible explanation for seemingly successful self-controlled behavior in AN might therefore be an unusually high capacity for inhibitory self-control, which enables resistance to temptations in the pursuit of disorder-typical goals (e.g., body/shape). This line of reasoning is supported by neuroimaging research suggesting that individuals with a history of AN show increased cognitive control (i.e., processes that organize behavior in a self-controlled goal-directed manner [Reference Cohen and Egner12]) during reward processing [Reference Ehrlich, Geisler, Ritschel, King, Boehm and Breier13], and that inhibitory control appears to require less effort in patients with AN [Reference Kullmann, Giel, Hu, Bischoff, Teufel and Thiel14Reference King, Geisler, Bernardoni, Ritschel, Boehm and Seidel16]. Following this rationale, AN patients should have higher rates of success in overcoming conflicting self-control situations (i.e., resisting temptations such as watching TV instead of doing homework) in everyday life compared with healthy controls (HCs).

However, successful self-control might not be achieved solely via reactive resistance and inhibition of desires. Furthermore, these strategies may actually often be ineffective [Reference Milyavskaya and Inzlicht17]. Behavioral strategies that proactively help to avoid tempting situations (e.g., environmental structuring or situation selection) or cognitive strategies such as, for example, goal priming (i.e., tempting cues are conditioned to activate self-control goals) and reconstrual (i.e., tempting cues are re-evaluated to be less desirable, similar to reappraisal processes) most likely also play a role in self-control by decreasing the necessity of effortful inhibition [Reference Gillebaart and de Ridder18Reference Wolff, Enge, Kräplin, Krönke, Bühringer and Smolka22]. These so-called antecedent self-control strategies (especially reconstrual/reappraisal [Reference Fujita20]), which have previously been mainly discussed in the context of emotion regulation [Reference Gross23], have recently gained interest [Reference Williamson and Wilkowski21]. Preliminary support suggesting that antecedent-focused self-control strategies are relevant to AN can be drawn from a study demonstrating that individuals with restrictive eating patterns experience less conflict when choosing the healthier of two food options (which might reflect cognitive processes such as reconstrual or goal priming [Reference Georgii, Schulte-Mecklenbeck, Richard, van Dyck and Blechert24]). If this can account for heightened self-control in AN patients, we would expect fewer and/or weaker self-control conflicts in everyday life relative to HC.

The present study investigated self-control processes in the everyday lives of patients with AN via ecological momentary assessment (EMA) [Reference Shiffman, Stone and Hufford25], involving data acquisition via smartphone several times a day over a period of several days. By assessing the frequency of conflicting self-control situations, the strength of self-control conflicts and underlying desires, and the rate of successful self-control situations in patients with AN compared to HC, we aimed to elucidate actual self-control behavior in AN, outside of laboratory settings.

Methods

Participants

The initial sample consisted of 42 females with acute AN according to DSM-5 and 57 HCs (age range: 12.9–27.3 years). HCs were selectively recruited to minimize age differences between the two groups. To further optimize group comparisons and control for possible developmental effects, we implemented a pairwise matching algorithm [Reference Munkres26]. The final sample consisted of 40 participants per group, which despite this procedure showed a small but significant age difference (Table 1). Diagnosis of AN was established using the expert form of the semi-structured interview for eating disorders (SIAB-EX) [Reference Fichter and Quadflieg27] and required a body mass index (BMI) below the 10th age percentile (if younger than 15.5 years) or below 17.5 kg/m² (if older than 15.5 years). AN patients were recruited within 96 h of admission to an inpatient treatment program at a university child and adolescent psychiatric department, which included individual, group, and family therapy. HCs had to be of normal weight, eumenorrheic, and without any history of psychiatric illness (assessed via the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children and Adolescents [Reference Sheehan, Shytle, Milo, Janavs and Lecrubier28]. All HCs were also assessed with the SIAB-EX and excluded if they showed any abnormal eating behavior. All participants were also interviewed with our own semi-structured interview to assess exclusion criteria (e.g., substance abuse and neurological conditions). The recruitment procedure was highly similar to our previous studies [Reference Fürtjes, Seidel, King, Biemann, Roessner and Ehrlich29, Reference Seidel, Petermann, Ritschel, Boehm, King and Geisler30] (see also the Supplementary Material [SM]).

Table 1. Sample characteristics and descriptive statistics.

Notes: t indicates values for independent group comparison. n = 40 per group. Age is given in years. Compliance is given in percentage of filled-out EMA questionnaires. All further variables are given as measured by the EMA questionnaire. Desire, binary variable [yes/no]; desire strength, continuous variable [1–7]; conflict, binary variable [yes/no]; conflict strength, continuous variable [1–7]; conflicting desires, desires for which conflict was affirmed; nonconflicting desires, desires for which no conflict was reported; resistance, binary variable [yes/no]; success, binary variable [yes/no]; valence of affect/calmness/energetic arousal, continuous variables [1–7].

Abbreviations: AN, anorexia nervosa; BMI, body mass index; BMI-SDS, body mass index standard deviation score; BDI-II, Beck Depression Inventory-II; BDI-SDS, body mass index standard deviation score; EDI-2, Eating Disorder Inventory-2; EMA, ecological momentary assessment, HC, healthy control.

a Significant at α ≤ 0.05.

b Significant at α ≤ 0.01.

Materials and procedure

EMA assessment

Self-control was assessed via a short questionnaire based on the works of Hofmann et al. [Reference Hofmann, Baumeister, Förster and Vohs31] and adapted from Wolff et al. [Reference Wolff, Krönke, Venz, Kräplin, Bühringer and Smolka32]. An overview of the structure of the questionnaire and the assessed variables is visualized in Figure 1. At each prompt, participants were first asked whether or not they had experienced a situation in which they felt a desire to enact a certain behavior (since the last alarm) and also had the opportunity to do so (binary variable; yes/no). If this was the case, they were asked to select the desire category by choosing 1 out of 17 domains (e.g., eating, exercise, and watching TV; see the SM) and to indicate the desire strength on a scale from one (very weak) to seven (very strong). The next question assessed conflict (binary variable; yes/no) by asking whether or not participants had thought it would be better not to enact the desired behavior (i.e., if there was a reason to engage in self-control instead of simply enacting the desired behavior). Whenever participants reported a conflict, conflict strength was assessed on a scale from one (very weak) to seven (very strong) and participants were asked whether they had tried to resist the desire (resistance, binary variable; yes/no). Lastly, success (binary variable; yes/no) versus failure was assessed via the question whether participants had succeeded in not enacting the desired behavior.

Figure 1. Procedure of the Ecological Momentary Assessment Questionnaire assessing self-control in real life and its outcomes.

Research has shown that self-control failure is often followed by negative affect in patients with AN, whereas controlled behavior may lead to positive affect. Alternative views exists as well, indicating that the relationship between failed self-control and affect in AN may be very nuanced [Reference Engel, Wonderlich, Crosby, Mitchell, Crow and Peterson33]. Therefore, we also assessed affective states via a modified version of the Multidimensional Mood Questionnaire (MDMQ) [Reference Seidel, Petermann, Ritschel, Boehm, King and Geisler30, Reference Steyer, Schwenkmezger, Notz and Eid34, Reference Fürtjes, Seidel, King, Boehm, Jaite and Roessner35]. Six items were adapted to assess calmness, energetic arousal, and valence of affect via visual analogue scales ranging from one to seven with opposite words as anchors (e.g., agitated calm).

Procedure

Before EMA, participants were interviewed, weighed, and measured. BMI and an age-adapted body mass index standard deviation score (BMI-SDS) [Reference Kromeyer-Hauschild, Wabitsch, Kunze, Geller, Geiß and Hesse36, Reference Hemmelmann, Brose, Vens, Hebebrand and Ziegler37] were calculated. Participants also completed questionnaires to assess eating disorder (ED) symptoms (Eating Disorder Inventory-2 [EDI-2]) [Reference Thiel, Jacobi, Horstmann, Paul, Nutzinger and Schüßler38] and the depressive symptoms (Beck Depression Inventory-II [BDI-II]) [Reference Hautzinger, Keller and Kühne39]. Participants were given a detailed tutorial on how to handle the study smartphone and fill out the EMA questionnaires. Afterward, they received a study smartphone with the preinstalled app for data collection (xs.movisens) [40]. EMA assessment started the day following the tutorial. HCs completed the assessment for 7 days of their everyday life, and AN for 7 days of their daily routine during inpatient treatment. Participants were prompted eight times a day by an alarm to fill out the questionnaire. The alarms were semi-randomized during a 14-h period (individually adapted to fit different daily routines), allowing for data assessment over the course of the full day. After EMA assessment was completed, participants received monetary compensation in accordance with compliance (i.e., number of completed questionnaires). The study procedure was approved by the local ethics committee, and all participants (and the legal guardians of underage participants) gave written informed consent.

Data analyses

The nested EMA data structure required hierarchical modeling with situations (Level 1) nested within subjects (Level 2). To analyze self-control, we performed a hierarchical generalized linear model (HGLM) via a population-averaged Bernoulli model for binominal outcome variables using the software HLM version 7 [Reference Raudenbush, Bryk and Congdon41]. The population-averaged model was favored over the unit-specific model since we examined average differences in self-control success in two subpopulations (AN vs. HC) [Reference Young, Preisser, Qaqish and Wolfson42].

Success (i.e., not enacting the desired behavior, coded 1 for success and 0 for failure) was predicted by group, desire strength, conflict strength, and resistance. Because we tested cross-level moderation effects on random slopes, the Level-1 predictors’ desire strength and conflict strength were centered around the person’s mean to avoid biased estimations [Reference Enders and Tofighi43]. To further investigate whether the within-person relationship between these variables and self-control success differed between groups, the model also included cross-level interactions. Because previous research found that restrained eaters tend to experience less conflict regarding food decisions [Reference Georgii, Schulte-Mecklenbeck, Richard, van Dyck and Blechert24] and successful self-control might be promoted via cognitive strategies to reduce conflict [Reference Fujita20], the conflict strength  $ \times $ group interaction was our primary focus of interest. Therefore, a reduced model including only one cross-level interaction for conflict strength  $ \times $ group is reported here; see the SM for results of a model including all cross-level interactions (Supplementary Table S8). Model comparisons via χ²-tests for differences in deviance confirmed that including additional cross-level interactions did not significantly improve model fit for any of the models (Supplementary Table S9).

Because we found that self-control success was less dependent on conflict strength in AN than HC, we asked whether the association between conflict and affective variables might be attenuated in AN. We therefore estimated further explorative linear hierarchical models in which valence of affect, calmness, and energetic arousal were (separately) predicted by the same variables described above, that is, group, desire strength and conflict strength (centered on the person’s mean), resistance, and success. Again, we report models including a cross-level interaction for conflict strength  $ \times $  group in the main manuscript; for models with all cross-level interactions, see Supplementary Table S8.

Further control analyses were conducted adjusting for compliance rate with the EMA protocol, age, and BDI-II, excluding all situations with a desire in category “eating” and excluding AN participants of the binge-purge subtype. Sensitivity analyses within the AN group taking into account BMI-SDS and EDI-2 were also added (SM).

Results

Sample and descriptive statistics

Sample characteristics and descriptive statistics including group differences between AN and HC are reported in Table 1. As expected, AN had lower mean BMI and BMI-SDS, and higher ED symptom severity and depressive symptoms compared to HC. AN participants also showed higher compliance with the EMA protocol than HC.

As displayed in the second part of Table 1, there was no group difference in the number of reported desires. On average, AN experienced higher desire strength compared with HC. Although AN participants reported a higher total number of self-control conflicts, there was no group difference in the percentage of how often a conflict was affirmed when a desire was reported (conflicting desires) or in the mean reported conflict strength. In total, AN participants showed more resistance and experienced more situations with self-control success. There was no group difference in the percentage of self-control success overall as well as regarding conflicting desires. However, AN had a higher success rate in the case of nonconflicting desires (i.e., not enacting a desired behavior even though it did not stand in conflict with a superordinate goal).

Furthermore, as expected, AN reported more negative valence of affect and less calmness than HC (Table 1).

Self-control in real life

The results of the HGLM-based multilevel logistic regression showed that the probability of self-control success was generally higher when desires were weaker and conflict strength was stronger when resistance was exercised (Table 2). Overall, there was no significant group difference in the predicted probability of self-control success between AN and HC. The significant moderating effect of group on the relationship between conflict strength and success revealed that while higher conflict strength was generally associated with a higher probability of success, this association was weaker in AN, indicating that conflict strength is less relevant for success in AN than in HC (Figure 2A). Results remained robust when controlling for EMA compliance, age, and depressive symptoms (Supplementary Tables S1–S3). They also remained robust when excluding all situations with desires of the category “eating,” indicating that the found associations were not specific to ED-related self-control (Supplementary Table S4), and AN participants of the binge-purge subtype (Supplementary Table S5). Sensitivity analysis within the AN group showed no moderating effects of ED symptoms or BMI-SDS (Supplementary Tables S6 and S7).

Table 2. Effect of group and self-control variables on self-control success (HLGM) and affect (HLM).

Notes: Nonstandardized betas of the hierarchical analyses. n = 40 per group. Group was coded 1 (patient with Anorexia nervosa) and −1 (healthy control participant). All variables are given as measured by the EMA questionnaire. The model with self-control success as outcome is a hierarchical generalized linear model via a population-averaged Bernoulli Model for binary outcomes. Models with affect as outcome, which were part of an exploratory analysis, are hierarchical linear models. Desire strength and conflict strength were centered around the subject’s mean.

Abbreviations: EMA, ecological momentary assessment; OR, odds ratio.

a Significant at α ≤ 0.05.

b Significant at α ≤ 0.01.

Figure 2. (A) Graphical representation of the positive relationship between conflict strength and self-control success, moderated by group (results from the hierarchical generalized linear model predicting success). (B) Graphical representation of the positive relationship between conflict strength and energetic arousal, moderated by group (results from the hierarchical linear model model predicting energetic arousal). n = 40 per group. AN, patients with Anorexia nervosa; HC, healthy control participants. Results of t-tests for significance of the slopes are shown for each group.

Self-control and affect (exploratory analyses)

Results of the exploratory multilevel logistic regression models showed that there was no significant association between self-control success and affect. Group had a significant effect on valence of affect and calmness, with AN experiencing less positive affect and less calmness than HC (Table 2). The cross-level interaction for conflict strength  $ \times $  group had a significant effect on energetic arousal: in HC, higher conflict strength was associated with more energetic arousal—in AN, this relationship was not significant (Figure 2B and Table 2). Again, results remained robust when controlling for compliance, age, and depressive symptoms, when excluding all situations with a desire of the category “eating” (Supplementary Tables S1–S4), and AN participants of the binge-purge subtype (see Supplementary Table S5). Sensitivity analysis within the group of AN patients revealed that neither ED symptom severity nor BMI-SDS moderated the associations between conflict strength and energetic arousal (Supplementary Tables S6 and S7).

Discussion

By analyzing momentary data collected over a period of 7 days in patients with acute AN and HC, we investigated how (elevated) self-control might be presented in and experienced by acutely underweight patients with AN in everyday life. We did not find significant group differences in either the frequency or strength of self-control conflicts. Furthermore, contrary to what one might expect, AN patients were not more successful at self-control in general, that is, the assumption that patients with AN are better at inhibiting unwanted impulses and resisting tempting desires was not supported by our findings. Instead, our findings suggest that a more nuanced look at self-control in daily life is necessary: while conflict strength played an important role for the probability of self-control success in HC (increased likelihood of success for stronger conflicts), this association was significantly less pronounced in AN. Pointing to the importance of antecedent self-control strategies [Reference Fujita20, Reference Williamson and Wilkowski21], patients with AN seem to act in a seemingly self-controlled manner even in situations of very low (or no) conflict strength.

As noted in the introduction, research has shown that successful self-control might be achieved not solely via inhibitory strategies, but also through antecedent-focused mechanisms, for example, by changing the conflict itself via goal priming or cognitive reconstrual [Reference Fujita20, Reference Williamson and Wilkowski21, Reference Fishbach, Friedman and Kruglanski44]. Studies have shown that individuals who are successful at self-control show reduced conflict during choices between temptations versus goal-congruent options. They are quicker at resolving conflict, and possibly resolve conflict without strenuous effort by employing proactive self-control strategies [Reference Georgii, Schulte-Mecklenbeck, Richard, van Dyck and Blechert24, Reference Stillman, Medvedev and Ferguson45]. Previous research suggests that a less effortful resolution of self-control conflicts might be based on individual cognitive construals (i.e., subjective representations of events [Reference Fujita and Han46] that include a more abstract, higher-level perspective and subjective goals [Reference Fujita and Han46, Reference Fujita47]). These construals are thought to affect behavior, for example, by reducing the strength of temptations [Reference Williamson and Wilkowski21]. A speculative interpretation of our findings might be that AN patients achieve self-controlled behavior (e.g., not eating the tasty muffin) via reconstruals (e.g., representing negative attributes such as high caloric density when evaluating the situation, instead of positive attributes such as taste), which reduce the impact of conflict strength on success. Considering that our findings were not constricted to food temptations but included desires from many behavioral domains, this might be a domain-general self-control strategy in AN. A further indication that AN patients might use more antecedent-focused self-control strategies (such as reconstrual) than HC is reflected by our finding that higher conflict strength was associated with higher arousal only in HC, not AN. This might be due to the possibility that AN require less effort to resolve conflict (or report conflict strength that has already been modulated via, e.g., reconstrual), therefore not experiencing the arousal that could come along with more effortful conflict resolution in HC. The fact that AN also refrained from a desired behavior even though there was little or no conflict could possibly be explained by potent reconstruals which generally promote resistance to desires (e.g., restraint itself becomes a superordinate goal).

While our study was able to shed some light on self-controlled behavior of AN patients outside the laboratory, it should be noted that the interpretations outlined above are speculative and different explanations are possible. Previous research has shown that patients with acute AN often report anhedonia, avoidance of affect, and experiential avoidance [Reference Oldershaw, Lavender, Sallis, Stahl and Schmidt48Reference Murray, Brown, Kaye and Wierenga51]. They also often show increased levels of alexithymia, that is, difficulties in identifying and describing emotional states [Reference Haynos and Fruzzetti52, Reference Lavender, Wonderlich, Engel, Gordon, Kaye and Mitchell53], and decreased levels of interoception, that is, awareness of bodily signals [Reference Pollatos, Kurz, Albrecht, Schreder, Kleemann and Schöpf54]. Increased parasympathetic activity in the acute state of the disorder might also reduce the subjective experience of arousal [Reference Petretta, Bonaduce, Scalfi, de Filippo, Marciano and Migaux55, Reference Jenkins, Eikelis, Phillipou, Castle, Wilding and Lambert56]. It could therefore be argued that we found conflict strength to be less relevant for self-control success in AN patients because, similarly to emotional states, self-control conflicts are experienced less intensely. However, sensitivity analyses revealed that BMI-SDS, which can be taken as a pathophysiological marker of the disorder, showed no associations with affect, desire strength, or strength conflict strength. We also found that AN reported stronger desires and more negative affect, but similar energetic arousal as HC (Table 1), which speaks against the aforementioned hypothesis. Another alternative interpretation could be that patients with AN experience self-control in itself as highly rewarding and therefore show self-controlled behavior independent from conflict strength. In line with the self-signaling theory, which proposes that people derive information about their identity from their behavior [Reference Bodner, Prelec, Loewenstei, Read and Baumeister57], AN patients may use self-control as a means to stabilize feelings of self-worth and may view self-discipline as an important aspect of their identity [Reference Froreich, Vartanian, Grisham and Touyz58Reference Rezek and Leary60]. It is possible that these associations between self-control and self-worth or identity could explain our findings.

The findings we presented should be considered against the backdrop of some limitations. During the course of the study, the AN participants took part in comprehensive multimodal psychiatric and psychotherapeutic inpatient treatment program. Therefore, results might not reflect the daily life of individuals with AN outside of treatment. It should also be considered that, due to the restrictions of an inpatient setting, occurrence of desires as well as opportunities to act on them were likely reduced for AN participants. It is therefore possible that self-control conflicts could not have arisen in the same way for AN as for HC. However, we specifically assessed situations in which participants had a desire and also the opportunity to act on it. Of note, the groups did not differ significantly in the total number of reported desires or the percentage of conflict in situations with a desire (see Table 1). We cannot rule out the possibility that AN might experience more self-control conflicts outside of treatment, and therefore different results might be obtained if the study were to be repeated in an outpatient setting. A further aspect that should be noted regarding the different settings between the groups is the higher compliance in AN patients. It should also be kept in mind that our findings are based on momentary associations analyzed using regression-based statistical approaches. Therefore, no conclusions can be drawn regarding the causality between the associations between the self-control variables and affect. As stated in the method and results sections, the analyses with affect as an outcome variable were not part of our primary research question, but were added based on the findings of the analysis predicting self-control success. Future research should follow up on these exploratory findings in a hypothesis-driven investigation of associations between self-control behavior and affect. As previously mentioned, the relationship between successful versus failed self-control and affect in AN is complex [Reference Engel, Wonderlich, Crosby, Mitchell, Crow and Peterson33], and the current investigation cannot do this topic justice. In addition, the present study did not investigate possible differences between AN participants with a restrictive versus binge-purge subtype, but merely confirmed the results in a purely restrictive subsample. Last but not least, our speculative interpretations regarding the possibility that AN patients make use of antecedent self-control strategies cannot be ascertained by our study design, which does not capture such processes. The reported conflict strength could possibly even reflect a value already modulated by such antecedent self-control strategies. Research using, for example, mouse-tracking paradigms [Reference Stillman, Medvedev and Ferguson45, Reference Sullivan, Hutcherson, Harris and Rangel61] to assess the dynamics of antecedent self-control processes is needed to investigate this question.

The research presented here indicates that patients with AN are not simply “better” at self-control than HC, but that they might resolve self-control conflicts more efficiently through strategies which lessen the impact of conflict strength on success or change the conflict itself. We speculated that this might be facilitated by antecedent-focused cognitive strategies such as goal priming or reconstrual, a possibility that should be addressed by future research. If patients with AN indeed show elevated self-control in daily life by resisting desires (even those with low conflict strength) through antecedent-focused cognitive strategies, cognitive-behavioral therapy might help patients to modify construals with the goal to re-evaluate food temptations as nonthreatening and food as enjoyable.

Supplementary Materials

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2022.29.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, S.E., upon reasonable request.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: S.F., M.S., S.E., and T.G.; Data curation: F.B.; Formal analysis: S.F. and S.D.; Funding acquisition: T.G. and S.E.; Methodology: S.F., M.S., and S.D.; Project administration: S.E. and V.R.; Writing—original draft: S.F.; Writing—review and editing: M.S., S.D., J.K., I.H. M.W., K.G., S.E., and T.G.

Financial Support

This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (Grant numbers EH 367/5-1 and SFB 940/2).

Conflicts of Interest

V.R. has received payment for consulting and writing activities from Lilly, Novartis, and Shire Pharmaceuticals; lecture honoraria from Lilly, Novartis, Shire Pharmaceuticals, and Medice Pharma; and support for research from Shire and Novartis. He has carried out (and is currently carrying out) clinical trials in cooperation with the Novartis, Shire, and Otsuka companies. Funding sources were not involved in the design of the study, the collection and analysis of data, or the decision to publish. The remaining authors declare no competing interests.

References

Schmidt, U, Treasure, J. Anorexia nervosa: valued and visible. A cognitive-interpersonal maintenance model and its implications for research and practice. Br J Clin Psychol. 2006;45:343–66. doi:10.1348/014466505X53902CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treasure, J, Schmidt, U. The cognitive-interpersonal maintenance model of anorexia nervosa revisited: a summary of the evidence for cognitive, socio-emotional and interpersonal predisposing and perpetuating factors. J Eat Disord. 2013;1:110. doi:10.1186/2050-2974-1-13CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casper, RC. Behavioral activation and lack of concern, core symptoms of anorexia nervosa? Int J Eat Disord. 1998;24:381–93. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199812)24:4<381::AID-EAT5>3.0.CO;2-Q3.0.CO;2-Q>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairburn, CG, Shafran, R, Cooper, Z. A cognitive behavioural theory of anorexia nervosa. Behav Res Ther. 1999;37:113. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00102-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotabe, HP, Hofmann, W. On integrating the components of self-control. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10:618–38. doi:10.1177/1745691615593382CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, PA. Executive control resources and frequency of fatty food consumption: findings from an age-stratified community sample. Health Psychol. 2012;31:235–41. doi:10.1037/a0025407CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, M, Lee, M, Higgs, S. Food-specific response inhibition, dietary restraint and snack intake in lean and overweight/obese adults: a moderated-mediation model. Int J Obes. 2016;40:877–82. doi:10.1038/ijo.2015.235CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Houben, K. Overcoming the urge to splurge: influencing eating behavior by manipulating inhibitory control. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2011;42:384–8. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.02.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, W, Adriaanse, M, Vohs, KD, Baumeister, RF. Dieting and the self-control of eating in everyday environments: an experience sampling study. Br J Health Psychol. 2014;19:523–39. doi:10.1111/bjhp.12053CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krönke, KM, Wolff, M, Mohr, H, Kräplin, A, Smolka, MN, Bühringer, G, et al. Monitor yourself! Deficient error-related brain activity predicts real-life self-control failures. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2018;18:622–37. doi:10.3758/s13415-018-0593-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopez, RB, Hofmann, W, Wagner, DD, Kelley, WM, Heatherton, TF. Neural predictors of giving in to temptation in daily life. Psychol Sci. 2014;25:1337–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, JD. Cognitive control core constructs and current considerations. In: Egner, T, editor. The Wiley handbook cognition control. Chichester, UK: John Wiley and Sons; 2017.Google Scholar
Ehrlich, S, Geisler, D, Ritschel, F, King, JA, Boehm, I, Breier, M, et al. Elevated cognitive control over reward processing in recovered female patients with anorexia nervosa. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2015;40:307–15. doi:10.1503/jpn.140249CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kullmann, S, Giel, KE, Hu, X, Bischoff, SC, Teufel, M, Thiel, A, et al. Impaired inhibitory control in anorexia nervosa elicited by physical activity stimuli. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2014;9:917–23. doi:10.1093/scan/nst070CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberndorfer, TA, Kaye, WH, Simmons, AN, Strigo, IA, Matthews, SC. Demand-specific alteration of medial prefrontal cortex response during an inhibition task in recovered anorexic women. Int J Eat Disord. 2011;44:18. doi:10.1002/eat.20750CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, JA, Geisler, D, Bernardoni, F, Ritschel, F, Boehm, I, Seidel, M, et al. Altered neural efficiency of decision making during temporal reward discounting in anorexia nervosa. Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2016;15:972–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milyavskaya, M, Inzlicht, M. What’s so great about self-control? Examining the importance of effortful self-control and temptation in predicting real-life depletion and goal attainment. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2017;8:603–11. doi:10.1177/1948550616679237CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillebaart, M, de Ridder, DTD. Effortless self-control: a novel perspective on response conflict strategies in trait self-control. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2015;9:8899. doi:10.1111/spc3.12160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galla, BM, Duckworth, AL. More than resisting temptation: beneficial habits mediate the relationship between self-control and positive life outcomes. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2015;109:508–25. doi:10.1016/j.clinbiochem.2015.06.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fujita, K. On conceptualizing self-control as more than the effortful inhibition of impulses. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2011;15:352–66. doi:10.1177/1088868311411165CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williamson, LZ, Wilkowski, BM. Nipping temptation in the bud: examining strategic self-control in daily life. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. 2020;46:961–75. doi:10.1177/0146167219883606CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolff, M, Enge, S, Kräplin, A, Krönke, K-M, Bühringer, G, Smolka, MN, et al. Chronic stress, executive functioning, and real-life self-control: an experience sampling study. J Pers. 2020;46:12587. doi:10.1111/jopy.12587Google Scholar
Gross, JJ. Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74:224–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Georgii, C, Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M, Richard, A, van Dyck, Z, Blechert, J. The dynamics of self-control: within-participant modeling of binary food choices and underlying decision processes as a function of restrained eating. Psychol Res. 2020;84:1777–88. doi:10.1007/s00426-019-01185-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiffman, S, Stone, AA, Hufford, MR. Ecological momentary assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2008;4:132. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munkres, J. Algorithms for the assignment and transportation problems. J Soc Ind Appl Math. 1957;5:32–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fichter, M, Quadflieg, N. The structured interview for anorexic and bulimic disorders for DSM-IV and ICD-10 (SIAB-EX): reliability and validity. Eur Psychiatry. 2001;16:3848. doi:10.1016/S0924-9338(00)00534-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, D, Shytle, D, Milo, K, Janavs, J, Lecrubier, Y. Reliability and validity of the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children and Adolescents (MINI-KID). J Clin Psychiatry. 2010;71:313–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fürtjes, S, Seidel, M, King, JA, Biemann, R, Roessner, V, Ehrlich, S. Rumination in anorexia nervosa: cognitive-affective and neuroendocrinological aspects. Behav Res Ther. 2018;111:92–8. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2018.10.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidel, M, Petermann, J, Ritschel, F, Boehm, I, King, JA, Geisler, D, et al. A naturalistic examination of negative affect and disorder-related rumination in anorexia nervosa. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;25:1207–16. doi:10.1007/s00787-016-0844-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, W, Baumeister, RF, Förster, G, Vohs, KD. Everyday temptations: an experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2012;102:1318–35. doi:10.1037/a0026545CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolff, M, Krönke, KM, Venz, J, Kräplin, A, Bühringer, G, Smolka, MN, et al. Action versus state orientation moderates the impact of executive functioning on real-life self-control. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2016;145:1635–53. doi:10.1037/xge0000229CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engel, SG, Wonderlich, SA, Crosby, RD, Mitchell, JE, Crow, S, Peterson, CB, et al. The role of affect in the maintenance of anorexia nervosa: evidence from a naturalistic assessment of momentary behaviors and emotion. J Abnorm Psychol. 2013;122:709–19. doi:10.1037/a0034010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steyer, R, Schwenkmezger, P, Notz, P, Eid, M. Der mehrdimensionale Befindlichkeitsfragebogen (MDBF). Handanweisung [The multidimensional affect rating scale (MDBF). Manual]. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1997.Google Scholar
Fürtjes, S, Seidel, M, King, JA, Boehm, I, Jaite, C, Roessner, V, et al. A naturalistic investigation of cognitive-affective dysfunction in anorexia nervosa: the role of inefficiency. Int J Eat Disord. 2020;53:239–47. doi:10.1002/eat.23189CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kromeyer-Hauschild, K, Wabitsch, M, Kunze, D, Geller, F, Geiß, HC, Hesse, V, et al. Perzentile für den body-mass-index für das Kindes- und Jugend- alter unter Heranziehung verschiedener deutscher Stichproben. Monatsschrift Kinderheilkd. 2001;149:807–18. doi:10.1007/s001120170107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hemmelmann, C, Brose, S, Vens, M, Hebebrand, J, Ziegler, A. Perzentilen des body-mass-index Auch für 18- bis 80-Jährige? Daten der Nationalen Verzehrsstudie II. Dtsch Medizinische Wochenschrift. 2010;135:848–52. doi:10.1055/s-0030-1253666CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thiel, A, Jacobi, C, Horstmann, S, Paul, T, Nutzinger, DO, Schüßler, G. Eine deutschsprachige version des eating disorder inventory EDI-2. Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol. 1997;47:365–76.Google Scholar
Hautzinger, M, Keller, F, Kühne, C. Beck depressions inventar: revision. Bern: Huber; 2009.Google Scholar
movisens GmbH. MovisensXS. Karslruhe: movisens GmbH; 2014.Google Scholar
Raudenbush, SW, Bryk, AS, Congdon, RT. HLM 7 for Windows. Lincolnwood: Scientific Software International Inc.; 2017.Google Scholar
Young, ML, Preisser, JS, Qaqish, BF, Wolfson, M. Comparison of subject-specific and population averaged models for count data from cluster-unit intervention trials. Stat Methods Med Res. 2007;16:167–84. doi:10.1177/0962280206071931CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enders, CK, Tofighi, D. Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: a new look at an old issue. Psychol Methods. 2007;12:121–38. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fishbach, A, Friedman, RS, Kruglanski, AW. Leading us not unto temptation: momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activation. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;84:296309. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stillman, PE, Medvedev, D, Ferguson, MJ. Resisting temptation: tracking how self-control conflicts are successfully resolved in real time. Psychol Sci. 2017;28:1240–58. doi:10.1177/0956797617705386CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fujita, K, Han, HA. Moving beyond deliberative control of impulses. Psychol Sci. 2009;20:799804. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02372.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fujita, K. Seeing the forest beyond the trees: a construal-level approach to self-control. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2008;2:1475–96. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00118.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oldershaw, A, Lavender, T, Sallis, H, Stahl, D, Schmidt, U. Emotion generation and regulation in anorexia nervosa: a systematic review and meta-analysis of self-report data. Clin Psychol Rev. 2015;39:8395. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2015.04.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lampard, AM, Byrne, SM, McLean, N, Fursland, A. Avoidance of affect in the eating disorders. Eat Behav. 2011;12:90–3. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2010.11.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dolan, S, Khindri, R, Franke, DL, Thomas, JJ, Reilly, EE, Eddy, KT. Anhedonia in eating disorders: a meta‐analysis and systematic review. Int J Eat Disord. 2022; 55:161–75. doi:10.1002/eat.23645CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, SM, Brown, CS, Kaye, WH, Wierenga, CE. Anhedonia in eating disorders. Current topics in behavioral neurosciences, Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer; 2022. doi:10.1007/7854_2021_287Google Scholar
Haynos, AF, Fruzzetti, AE. Anorexia nervosa as a disorder of emotion regulation: theory, evidence, and treatment implications. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2011;18:183202. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2850.2011.01250.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lavender, JM, Wonderlich, SA, Engel, SG, Gordon, KH, Kaye, WH, Mitchell, JE. Dimensions of emotion dysregulation in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: a conceptual review of the empirical literature. Clin Psychol Rev. 2015;40:111–22. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2015.05.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollatos, O, Kurz, A-L, Albrecht, J, Schreder, T, Kleemann, AM, Schöpf, V, et al. Reduced perception of bodily signals in anorexia nervosa. Eat Behav. 2008;9:381–8. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2008.02.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petretta, M, Bonaduce, D, Scalfi, L, de Filippo, E, Marciano, F, Migaux, M-L, et al. Heart rate variability as a measure of cardiac autonomic function in anorexia nervosa: a review of the literature. Clin Cardiol. 1997;20:219–24. doi:10.1002/erv.1081CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenkins, ZM, Eikelis, N, Phillipou, A, Castle, DJ, Wilding, HE, Lambert, EA. Autonomic nervous system function in anorexia nervosa: a systematic review. Front Neurosci. 2021;15:682208. doi:10.3389/fnins.2021.682208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bodner, R, Prelec, D. Self-signaling and self-control. In: Loewenstei, G, Read, D, Baumeister, R, editors. Time and decision: economic and psychological perspectives intertemporal choice, New York: Russell Sage Press; 2003, p. 277–98.Google Scholar
Froreich, FV, Vartanian, LR, Grisham, JR, Touyz, SW. Dimensions of control and their relation to disordered eating behaviours and obsessive-compulsive symptoms. J Eat Disord. 2016;4:19. doi:10.1186/s40337-016-0104-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Surgenor, LJ, Horn, J, Hudson, SM. Empirical scrutiny of a familiar narrative: sense of control in anorexia nervosa. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2003;11:291305. doi:10.1002/erv.499CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rezek, PJ, Leary, MR. Perceived control, drive for thinness, and food consumption: anorexic tendencies as displaced reactance. J Pers. 1991;59:129–42. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1991.tb00771.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, N, Hutcherson, C, Harris, A, Rangel, A. Dietary self-control is related to the speed with which health and taste attributes are processed. Psychol Sci. 2016;26:122–34. doi:10.1177/0956797614559543.DietaryCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample characteristics and descriptive statistics.

Figure 1

Figure 1. Procedure of the Ecological Momentary Assessment Questionnaire assessing self-control in real life and its outcomes.

Figure 2

Table 2. Effect of group and self-control variables on self-control success (HLGM) and affect (HLM).

Figure 3

Figure 2. (A) Graphical representation of the positive relationship between conflict strength and self-control success, moderated by group (results from the hierarchical generalized linear model predicting success). (B) Graphical representation of the positive relationship between conflict strength and energetic arousal, moderated by group (results from the hierarchical linear model model predicting energetic arousal). n = 40 per group. AN, patients with Anorexia nervosa; HC, healthy control participants. Results of t-tests for significance of the slopes are shown for each group.

Supplementary material: File

Fürtjes et al. supplementary material

Fürtjes et al. supplementary material

Download Fürtjes et al. supplementary material(File)
File 45.7 KB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.