Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-wq484 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T15:18:42.467Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Prevalence and maternal determinants of early and late introduction of complementary foods: results from the Growing Up in New Zealand cohort study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 April 2022

Sara Silva Ferreira
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition, School of Public Health, University of Sao Paulo, Brazil
Dirce Maria Lobo Marchioni
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition, School of Public Health, University of Sao Paulo, Brazil
Clare Rosemary Wall
Affiliation:
Nutrition Section, Faculty of Medical Sciences, University of Auckland, New Zealand
Sarah Gerritsen
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Population Health, University of Auckland, New Zealand Centre for Longitudinal Research, School of Population Health, University of Auckland, New Zealand
Juliana Araujo Teixeira
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition, School of Public Health, University of Sao Paulo, Brazil
Cameron C. Grant
Affiliation:
Centre for Longitudinal Research, School of Population Health, University of Auckland, New Zealand Department of Paediatrics: Child and Youth Health, School of Medicine, University of Auckland, New Zealand General Paediatrics, Starship Children’s Hospital, Auckland, New Zealand
Susan M. B. Morton
Affiliation:
Centre for Longitudinal Research, School of Population Health, University of Auckland, New Zealand
Teresa Gontijo de Castro*
Affiliation:
Nutrition Section, Faculty of Medical Sciences, University of Auckland, New Zealand Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Population Health, University of Auckland, New Zealand
*
*Corresponding author: Teresa Gontijo de Castro, email t.castro@auckland.ac.nz
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

A nationally generalisable cohort (n 5770) was used to determine the prevalence of non-timely (early/late) introduction of complementary food and core food groups and associations with maternal sociodemographic and health behaviours in New Zealand (NZ). Variables describing maternal characteristics and infant food introduction were sourced, respectively, from interviews completed antenatally and during late infancy. The NZ Infant Feeding Guidelines were used to define early (≤ 4 months) and late (≥ 7 months) introduction. Associations were examined using multivariable multinomial regression, presented as adjusted relative risk ratios and 95 % confidence intervals (RRR; 95% CI). Complementary food introduction was early for 40·2 % and late for 3·2 %. The prevalence of early food group introduction were fruit/vegetables (23·8 %), breads/cereals (36·3 %), iron-rich foods (34·1 %) and of late were meat/meat alternatives (45·9 %), dairy products (46·2 %) and fruits/vegetables (9·9 %). Compared with infants with timely food introduction, risk of early food introduction was increased for infants: breastfed < 6months (2·52; 2·19–2·90), whose mothers were < 30 years old (1·69; 1·46–1·94), had a diploma/trade certificate v. tertiary education (1·39; 1·1–1·70), of Māori v. European ethnicity (1·40; 1·12–1·75) or smoked during pregnancy (1·88; 1·44–2·46). Risk of late food introduction decreased for infants breastfed < 6 months (0·47; 0.27–0·80) and increased for infants whose mothers had secondary v. tertiary education (2·04; 1·16–3·60) were of Asian v. European ethnicity (2·22; 1·35, 3·63) or did not attend childbirth preparation classes (2·23; 1·24–4·01). Non-timely food introduction, specifically early food introduction, is prevalent in NZ. Interventions to improve food introduction timeliness should be ethnic-specific and support longer breast-feeding.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

as The first 1000 days of life, from conception to 2 years of age, are critical due to rapid growth and cognitive development and because nutrition in this period will have impact not only at that time but also throughout life(Reference Black, Quigg and Hurley1,Reference Martorell2) . Breast milk or infant formula alone supplies the nutritional needs of infants until the age of 6 months. After this age, the introduction of foods should be started to attend to an infant’s nutritional and developmental needs. This transition from only milk to other foods constitutes a critical period during early life(3,Reference Spence, Campbell and Lioret4) . In New Zealand (NZ), the Ministry of Health recommends that complementary feeding, defined as the gradual offering of other foods, solids or liquids, along with breast milk or infant formula, should be initiated around 6 months of age(3). The NZ guidelines also state that complementary foods should include items from core food groups and that the first items to be introduced should be good sources of iron such as iron-fortified cereals, puréed vegetables, age-appropriate meats and vegetarian alternatives(3).

The description of the timing of food introduction and its determinants within a population is important, since previous studies and systematic reviews suggest that non-timely food introductions are related to overweight/obesity among infants/toddlers which can persist across the lifespan(Reference English, Obbagy and Wong5,Reference Sun, Foskey and Allen6) , gastrointestinal infections(3,7,Reference Przyrembel8) , delayed growth and development(Reference Przyrembel8,Reference Butte, Lopez-Alarcon and Garza9) and iron deficiency anaemia(Reference Wang, Liu and Wan10). Gastrointestinal infections are related to early food introduction(3,7,Reference Przyrembel8,Reference Schincaglia, Oliveira and Sousa11,Reference Davisse-Paturet, Adel-Patient and Forhan12) , and impaired growth and development are related to late introduction (potentially due to shortage of vitamins and minerals such as iron, iron and vitamin A)(Reference Przyrembel8,Reference Pedraza and Queiroz13) , while iron deficiency anaemia and child overweight/obesity are related to both, early and late food introduction(Reference Sun, Foskey and Allen6,Reference Wang, Liu and Wan10,Reference Hassan, El-Gendy and Badra14Reference Mannan18)

The literature describing the relationship between the occurrence of food allergy and the timing of food introduction is controversial. For many decades, delaying the introduction of some foods was considered the most effective strategy to prevent food allergy(Reference Royal and Gray19,Reference Comberiati, Costagliola and D’Elios20) , but more recent research has demonstrated that such delay does not prevent the development of food allergy, with some research indicating that late food introduction may increase the risk of allergy(Reference Poole, Barriga and Leung21Reference Hicke-Roberts, Wennergren and Hesselmar23). Recent recommendations for the prevention of food allergy suggest that allergenic foods, such as peanuts, tree nuts, seeds, eggs, fish, shellfish, cows’ milk products, wheat and soyabeans, should be introduced around 6 months of age, alongside other recommended foods(Reference Royal and Gray19,Reference Comberiati, Costagliola and D’Elios20,Reference Obbagy, English and Wong24Reference Caffarelli, Di Mauro and Mastrorilli27) .

Recently published information on infant feeding practices from 80 countries reported that only two-thirds of infants aged 6 to 8 months receive complementary foods, and that 15 % of infants are introduced to foods/drinks when aged between 2 and 3 months. Among 0–5-month-old infants, 43 % are given liquids or foods other than breast milk within the first 3 d of life(28).

Previous studies have also reported inequities in the prevalence of non-timely food introduction, with infants whose mothers were younger, with lower levels of education and income, from minority ethnic groups, who smoked during pregnancy, or who had higher pre-gestational body mass index (BMI) being more likely to be exposed to non-timely food introduction(Reference Wijndaele, Lakshman and Landsbaugh29Reference Hazir, Senarath and Agho34).

NZ currently does not have nationally generalisable or representative information on age of introduction of complementary feeding (defined as any food or drink other than breast milk or suitable infant formula milk) nor on associated maternal sociodemographic and health behaviour determinants. The only indicator collected routinely by the annual health surveys reports prevalence of introduction of solid foods before the 4th and before the 6th month of life (which is asked only to caregivers in households that have children aged 4 months to < 5 years)(35). Findings from these annual surveys showed that, since 2006/2007, there has been no statistically significant change in the prevalence of infants who were introduced to solid foods before the 4th month of life. In 2019/20, 8·0 % of infants were introduced to solid foods before 4 months of age, with differences by sex (10·1 % for boys and 5·1 % for girls), maternal ethnicity (European/other: 6·9 %; Asian: 7·0 %; Māori: 13·8 % and Pacific: 15·1 %) and neighbourhood deprivation (5·1 % in the least deprived households and 11·7 % in the most deprived ones)(35) Previous study, using data from a nationally generalisable birth cohort – Growing Up in New Zealand (GUiNZ), has also described the cohort’s age of introduction to solids, showing that 39·3 % were introduced to solids before or at the age of 4 months(Reference Castro, Gerritsen and Wall36).

In this study, using the GUiNZ cohort data, we aimed to examine (i) the prevalence of non-timely introduction (early and late) of complementary foods and of core food groups among 7–12-month-olds and (ii) the associations of maternal sociodemographic and health behaviour characteristics with infants’ non-timely food introduction.

Methods

Study population, data collection waves and ethical approval

We used data from the ongoing NZ birth cohort study, GUiNZ, which enrolled 6822 pregnant women and their 6853 children who formed the cohort. Eligibility was defined by maternal residency within a region of NZ chosen for its ethnic and socio-economic diversity of the birth cohort and mothers having an estimated delivery date between 25 April 2009 and 25 March 2010. This cohort has shown to be broadly generalisable to all NZ births from 2007 to 2010(Reference Morton, Atatoa Carr and Grant37,Reference Morton, Ramke and Kinloch38) . This investigation was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the Ministry of Health Northern Y Regional Ethics Committee (NTY/08/06/055). Written informed consent was obtained from all mothers/caregivers(Reference Morton, Atatoa Carr and Grant37,Reference Morton, Ramke and Kinloch38) .

Information was sourced from four data collection waves, antenatally (conducted mostly during the third trimester of pregnancy) and when infants were approximately 6 weeks, 9 and 31 months old. Variables describing maternal sociodemographic and health behaviours were sourced from the antenatal face-to-face computer-assisted personal interview, completed during the years of 2009/10. Infants’ perinatal information (sex, fetal count, birth and gestational age) and infants’ feeding status in the first weeks of life were obtained from perinatal data linkage and the 6-week computer-assisted telephone interview, conducted with each infant’s mother. Information on infant’s age of food introduction was obtained from the 9-month mother computer-assisted personal interview. Variables measuring the duration of any breast-feeding were derived from the 6-week computer-assisted telephone interview, 9-month computer-assisted personal interview and the 31-month computer-assisted telephone interview completed with each infant’s mother(Reference Castro, Grant and Wall39).

Of the 6476 children whose mothers took part in the 9-month interview, we excluded 168 (2·6 %) twin/triplets, 211 (3·3 %) infants who were born with low birth weight (< 2500 g), 172 (2·7 %) with premature gestation (< 37 weeks of gestational age) and 155 (2·4 %) who were aged < 7 months or > 12 months at the time of the 9-month computer-assisted personal interview. As we examined the age of food introduction among infants, those > 12 months of age were excluded from the analyses. Infants aged < 7 months were excluded to ensure that every infant had the chance to have complementary feeding initiated and then their timing of food introduction classified as timely, early and late based on the national guidelines(3). Twins/triplets were excluded so that only independent maternal observation was included. Children with birth weight less than 2500 grams (g) or gestational age less than 37 weeks were excluded because the NZ Infant Feeding Guidelines(3) on timing of food introduction were established for healthy infants and may not be appropriate for pre-term or low birth weight infants, many of whom would be following tailored clinical nutrition guidelines. Thus, our final analytical sample was constituted of 5770 infants, corresponding to 89·1 % of the infants whose mother took part in the 9-month interview (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1. Flow chart describing the study sample.

Outcomes

A 25-item semi-quantitative FFQ was used to assess the age of introduction of complementary feeding at the 9-month interview. Mothers were asked to report, retrospectively, the age (which was asked in rounded months) when their infants were introduced to the following foods: infant formula or milk other than breast milk; baby rice; baby breakfast cereal; other cereal; bread or toast; rusks; biscuits; vegetables – raw or cooked; fruits – fresh and canned; meat, chicken and meat dishes; fish and fish dishes – fresh and canned; eggs; milk puddings, rice pudding, yogurt and custards; nuts or peanut butter; shellfish; soya foods, tofu and soya desserts; sweets; chocolate; hot chips; potato chips – crisps; fruit juices – includes watered down juice; herbal drinks; tea; coffee; and soft drinks(Reference Castro, Grant and Wall39). The FFQ was designed by an experienced academic paediatric dietitian, who selected items based on the NZ Infant Feeding Guidelines(3) and foods and beverages commonly fed to NZ infants(Reference Marriot, Inskip and Borland40,Reference Wall, Brunt and Grant41) .

The outcomes assessed in this study were infants’ age of introduction (in months) for (i) complementary foods and (ii) core food groups nationally recommended to infants (fruits and vegetables; meats and alternatives; breads and cereals; dairy foods and; iron-rich foods)(3). Age when complementary feeding started was defined as the age when infants first tried any foods or drinks other than breast milk and suitable infant formula(3). The age of introduction of each core food group was defined as the age when infants first tried any food or drink belonging to each core food group. The list of foods considered under each core food group is provided in Table 1. Following the NZ Infant Feeding Guidelines(3), food introduction was classified as early if introduced ≤ 4 months of age, timely if introduced when infants were 5–6 months of age and late if introduced ≥ 7 months of age.

Table 1. Food items with the Growing Up in New Zealand cohort study FFQ included in each of the core food groups recommended for infants

Covariates

Informed by the published literature, we examined the potential influence of antenatal maternal sociodemographic and health behaviour factors on the timing of infant food introduction(Reference Wijndaele, Lakshman and Landsbaugh29Reference Hazir, Senarath and Agho34,Reference Castro, Gerritsen and Teixeira42) .

The following antenatal maternal sociodemographic factors were examined: parity, pregnancy planning, level of education completed, age, maternal ethnicity and socio-economic status. Maternal ethnicity was self-prioritised and reported at the Statistics NZ Level 1 classification. The ethnic groupings used in this study were (1) European, (2) Māori, (3) Pacific Peoples, (4) Asian, (5) Middle Eastern, Latin American and African (MELAA), and (6) Other(Reference Salmond, Crampton and Atkinson43Reference Atatoa Carr, Bandara and Berry45). The groups MELAA and Other were combined for analysis purposes. Socio-economic status was described using the NZDep2006 Index of Deprivation(35). This validated small area measure of neighbourhood deprivation combines nine socio-economic characteristics from the 2006 NZ census data collected at aggregations of approximately 100 people and assigned to individual households based on geo-coded address data(Reference Salmond, Crampton and Atkinson43). NZDep2006 scores are ranked in deciles where deciles 1–2 represent the least deprived neighbourhoods and deciles 9–10 represent the most deprived ones.

The antenatal maternal health behaviour characteristics examined were: adherence to nutrition guidelines during pregnancy, pre-pregnancy BMI, smoking patterns before/during pregnancy, physical activity (PA) before/during pregnancy and mothers’ attendance at childbirth preparation classes. During pregnancy, the assessment of the adequacy of the number of servings consumed of vegetables and fruit, breads and cereals, milk and milk products and, lean meat, meat and alternatives and eggs was based on the national recommendations for pregnant women that were in place when the antenatal interview took place(Reference Morton, Grant and Wall46). For each food group, daily intakes were classified as ‘yes’ (≥ number of servings recommended) and ‘no’ (< number of servings recommended)(44). Pre-pregnancy BMI was calculated based on self-reported weight and height and classified according to the WHO cut-offs(47). Smoking patterns pre-/during pregnancy were categorised as continued smoking, stopped smoking during pregnancy and non-smokers. PA before or during pregnancy was estimated using the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ)(Reference Marriot, Inskip and Borland40). Women who engaged in moderate PA for at least 30 minutes for at least 5 out of 7 d, or vigorous PA for at least 30 minutes on at least 2 out of 7 d were classified as participating in moderate/vigorous activity. The PA categories examined were: no moderate/vigorous PA before or during pregnancy, moderate/vigorous PA before and during pregnancy, and moderate/vigorous PA only before or only during pregnancy(Reference Craig, Marshall and Sjostrom48). Mother’s attendance of childbirth preparation classes was classified as: yes; no, but intend to attend and; and no, and no intention to attend. Antenatal classes are provided by non-governmental organisations during pregnancy, and parents are recommended to participate during each pregnancy or at least during the first pregnancy(Reference Morton, Atatoa Carr and Bandara49). The main purpose of these classes is to provide information about a variety of pregnancy and childbirth-related issues including: how the NZ maternity system works; pregnancy care; normal changes in maternal health that occur during pregnancy; healthy pregnancy; labour and childbirth; postnatal care; breast-feeding and safe sleeping(50).

We estimated the duration of any breast-feeding (in months) according to methodology described in Castro et al. (Reference Morton, Ramke and Kinloch38), which was categorised as < 6 months and ≥ 6 months of duration for analytical purposes.

Statistical analyses

Categorical variables were described as proportions and continuous variables as medians and interquartile ranges. The prevalence of timely and non-timely (early and late) age of food introduction was calculated. Infants who were never introduced to complementary feeding or who never tried items under the core food groups by the date of the 9-month interview were included in the denominators. We used χ 2 tests to compare proportions of infants introduced early and late to foods according to infant sex and breast-feeding duration.

It was examined the associations between the timing of introduction to complementary feeding [dependent variable categorised as timely (0), early (1) or late (2)] and the maternal characteristics (independent variables). Relative risk ratios (RRR) and 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) were obtained from univariable and multivariable multinomial logistic regression models. Univariable associations with P < 0·15 were used to identify the independent variables to be tested in the multivariable models, following a forward stepwise approach. Variables were retained in the final model if associations with the dependent variable had P < 0·05 or if they changed the magnitude of the RRRs by 10 % or more. Infant sex (boy/girl), age (in months-continuous variable) and birth weight (in g-continuous variable) adjusted the final model as these variables are potentially important factors influencing the timing of food introduction(Reference Aronsson, Uusitalo and Vehik30,Reference Kronborg, Foverskov and Væth31) .

All analyses were performed using Stata Statistical Software (STATA) (version 14, StataCorp. 2015. Stata Statistical Software: Release 14. StataCorp LP).

Results

Prevalence of early and late introduction of complementary feeding and of core food groups (overall, by sex and by breast-feeding duration)

Four in ten infants were introduced to complementary feeding early (age ≤ 4 months), and 3·2 were introduced to complementary feeding late (age ≥ 7 months). The prevalence of early introduction for core foods groups were: breads and cereals (36·3 % of infants), iron-rich foods (34·1 %), and fruit and vegetables (23·8 %). The prevalence of late introduction for core food groups were: meats and alternatives (45·9 % of infants), dairy foods (46·2 %), and fruits and vegetables (9·9 %) (Fig. 2). By the date of the 9-month interview, the proportion of the infants who were not introduced to complementary feeding or to the core food groups were: complementary foods (0·05 %), fruits and vegetables (0·4 %), meat and alternatives (5·36 %), breads and cereals (0·54 %), dairy products (16·05 %) and iron-rich foods (0·89 %).

Fig. 2. Prevalence of timely (5–6 months), early (< 4 months) and late (> 7 months) introduction of complementary feeding and of each recommended core food group (all cohort, n = 5770), Growing Up in New Zealand cohort.

When comparing boys and girls, there were no statistically significant differences in the prevalence of early and late introduction of complementary feeding and of the core food groups (data not shown in figure).

Figure 3 displays the prevalence of early and late food introduction, according to infants’ duration of any breast-feeding. Overall and for each core food group, the prevalence of early introduction of complementary feeding and introduction of breads and cereals, Fe-rich foods, and fruits and vegetables were significantly higher among infants breastfed for < 6 months in comparison with infants breastfed for ≥ 6 months. In contrast, the prevalence of late introduction of dairy products and meat and alternatives were significantly higher among infants breastfed for ≥ 6 months in comparison with infants breastfed for < 6 months (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3. Prevalence of early (< 4 months) and late (> 7 months) introduction of complementary feeding and of each recommended core food group according to duration of any breast-feeding (< 6 months: n 1740; ≥ 6 months: n 3847), Growing Up in New Zealand cohort.

Prevalence of early and late introduction of complementary feeding according to infant’s and mothers’ characteristics

The median (interquartile ranges) age of the children who had timely, early and late introduction to complementary feeding were, respectively, 8·80 (8·61–9·01), 8·84 (8·61–9·20) and 9·00 (8·64–9·43) months. Children who had timely, early and late introduction to complementary feeding had median (interquartile ranges) birth weights of 3540 (3250–3860), 3580 (3270–3890) and 3600 (3280–3900) g (data not shown in table).

A larger proportion of infants who had early and late introduction to complementary foods were male (early: 53·6 % and late: 53·6 %), breastfed to ≥ 6 months (early: 52·1 % and late: 83·9 %) or from planned pregnancies (early: 54·3 % and late: 51·9 %). A larger proportion of their mothers were multiparous (early: 56·5 % and late: 71·8 %), non-smokers (early: 71·7 % and late: 86·3 %), with educational level lower than bachelor’s degree (early: 70·6 % and late: 73·2 %), from neighbourhoods with deprivation deciles 7–10 in the NZDep2006 index (early: 53·8 % and late: 62·9 %) or either did not attend and did not intend to attend childbirth preparation classes (early: 59·6 % and late: 77·1 %). The majority of infants’ mothers did not adhere to national recommended intakes of vegetables and fruit (early: 76·2 % and late 74·4 %), breads and cereals (early: 69·5 % and late: 76·7 %) and lean meat, meat alternatives and eggs (early: 76·6 % and late: 71·2 %) during pregnancy. The majority of infants’ mothers adhered to national recommended intakes of milk and milk products in pregnancy (early: 58·8 % and late: 53·8 %). Among the mothers who introduced complementary feeding early to their infants, 92·9 % were ≥ 20 years old, 49·5 % were of European ethnicity and for 54·4 % pre-pregnancy BMI were > 25 kg/m2. Among the mothers who introduced complementary feeding late to their infants, 56·9 % were ≥ 30 years of age, 34·8 % were of European ethnicity, and for 44 % pre-pregnancy BMI were > 25 kg/m2 (Table 2).

Table 2. Prevalence of early, timely and late introduction of complementary feeding according to infant and maternal characteristics

(number and percentages)

NCEA, National Certificate of Education Achievement; MELAA, Middle Eastern, Latin American and African; NZDep2006, neighbourhood deprivation index 2006; kg/m 2 , kilograms/squared metre.

Infants aged 7 to 12 months in the 9-month interview.

* Missing for variables listed (n): sex (n 26); breast-feeding (n 44); parity (n 40); pregnancy planned (n 62); childbirth preparation class (n 81); age group (n 35); ethnicity (n 45); education (n 50); deprivation index (n 37); BMI pre-pregnancy (n 698); physical activity (n 510); smoking pattern (n 525); adherence to fruits and vegetables consumption (n 510); adherence to breads and cereals consumption n 510; adherence to milk and derivatives consumption (n 510); and adherence to meats and alternatives consumption (n 512).

** Area-level socio-economic deprivation was measured by using the NZ Index of Deprivation, derived from the 2006 national census according to the methodology described in Salmond et al.(Reference Hazir, Senarath and Agho34). NZDep2006 deciles 1–2 represent the least deprived neighbourhoods and deciles 9–10 the most deprived neighbourhoods.

As per Growing up in New Zealand study anonymity requirement, ‘< 10’ represents greater than zero and less than ten children in the cell.

Maternal sociodemographic and health behaviour characteristics associated with early and late food introduction

In univariable analyses, except for mother’s adherence to recommended intakes of milk and milk derivatives during pregnancy, all mothers’ sociodemographic and health behaviour characteristics were associated with timing of food introduction (P < 0·15) and were therefore tested in the multivariable models (online Supplementary Table S1).

The results of the multivariable analysis showing the variables retained in the final model are presented in Table 3. When compared with infants with timely introduction of complementary foods, the risk of being introduced to foods early was increased for those infants who were never breastfed or were breastfed for < 6 months (v. breastfed for ≥ 6 months), whose mothers were younger than 30 years of age (v. 30 years), who had a diploma/trade certificate (v. a higher tertiary educational qualification), of Māori ethnicity (v. European ethnicity) or, who continued smoking during pregnancy (v. non-smokers). When compared with infants with timely food introduction, the risk of late introduction of food was decreased for infants who were breastfed for < 6 months (v. breastfed for ≥ 6 months) and was increased for infants whose mothers had secondary school-level education (v. a higher tertiary educational qualification), of Asian ethnicity (v. European ethnicity), or who did not attend nor had the intention to attend childbirth preparation classes (v. mothers who attended childbirth preparation classes).

Table 3. Adjusted RRR and 95 % CI for the associations between timing of food introduction and variables describing breast-feeding duration and maternal sociodemographic and health behaviours

(all cohort, n = 4595)

Ref., reference category; RRR, relative risk ratio; NCEA, National Certificate of Education Achievement; MELAA, Middle Eastern, Latin American and African.

Missing for variables listed in the table (n): sex (n 26); age in years (n 26); birth weight (n 26); breast-feeding (n 44); parity (n 40); planned pregnancy (n 62); age group (n 35); childbirth preparation class (n 81); education (n 50); ethnicity (n 45); deprivation index (n 37); BMI pre-pregnancy (n 698); physical activity (n 510); smoking pattern (n 525); adherence to fruits and vegetables consumption (n 510); adherence to breads and cereals consumption n 510; adherence to milk and derivatives consumption (n 510); and adherence to meats and alternatives consumption (n 512).

* Infants singletons aged 7–12 months at the 9-month interview.

** The group of mothers whose infants had timely introduction to complementary foods was the reference group in these analyses.

Multivariable multinominal logistic regression model included all variables presented in the table adjusted by maternal adherence to bread and cereals guideline, pre-pregnancy BMI, NZDep 2006***, pregnancy planned, child’s birth weight, sex and age (in months).

*** Area-level socio-economic deprivation was measured by using the NZ Index of Deprivation, derived from the 2006 national census according to the methodology described in Salmond et al.(Reference Hazir, Senarath and Agho34). NZDep2006 deciles 1–2 represent the least deprived neighbourhoods and deciles 9–10 the most deprived neighbourhoods.

Discussion

Summary of findings

This nationally generalisable birth cohort showed that 40·2 % and 3·2 % of infants were introduced to complementary feeding early and late, respectively. Breads and cereals, iron-rich foods, and fruit and vegetables were introduced early to, respectively, 36·3 %, 34·1 % and 23·8 % of the infants. Meat and alternatives and dairy products were introduced late to 45·9 % and fruits and vegetables to 9·9 % of the infants. There were no significant sex differences in the prevalence of non-timely introduction of complementary feeding and of the core food groups. Early introduction of complementary feeding was more prevalent among infants breastfed for < 6 months (v. ≥ 6 months), and late introduction of complementary feeding was more prevalent among infants breastfed for ≥ 6 months (v. < 6 months). In the multivariable model, non-timely food introduction was independently associated with breast-feeding duration and with maternal attendance/intention to attend childbirth preparation classes, education level, self-prioritised ethnicity, age and smoking patterns during pregnancy.

Comparisons of findings with previous studies

Challenges to comparing the prevalence of early, timely and late introduction of complementary feeding in our study with previous investigations include the diversity of indicators of timing of food introduction used (e.g. introduction of complementary feeding, introduction to solids and introduction to core food groups). In addition, the cut-offs used by different studies to classify food introduction as timely vary widely. The high prevalence of early introduction of complementary feeding within the GUiNZ cohort born in 2009/2010 (40·2 %) was concerning and significantly higher than prevalence reported for population-based studies performed in the Netherlands in 2009/2010 (21·4 %)(Reference Wang, van Grieken and van der Velde51), in the USA in 2009–2014 (16·3 %)(Reference Barrera, Hamner and Perrine52), and in Poland and Austria in 2017–2019 (3 %)(Reference Zielinska, Rust and Masztalerz-Kozubek53). The prevalence of early complementary feeding within the GUiNZ cohort was also higher than the prevalence in longitudinal Australian studies reporting data on early introduction of any foods from core food groups (12 %)(Reference Kronborg, Foverskov and Væth31), of soft, semi-solid and solid foods (33·3 %)(Reference Magarey, Kavian and Scott54), and of solid foods (20·0 %)(Reference Amezdroz, Carpenter and O’Callaghan55). However, the prevalence of late introduction of complementary feeding within the GUiNZ cohort (3·2 %) was similar or lower to prevalence reported for Australian cohorts [3 %(43); 10 %(44)] and for the infants in the Dutch(Reference Wang, van Grieken and van der Velde51), American(Reference Barrera, Hamner and Perrine52), Polish(Reference Zielinska, Rust and Masztalerz-Kozubek53) and Austrian(Reference Zielinska, Rust and Masztalerz-Kozubek53) population (2 %, 12·9 %, 37·1 % and 20·4 %, respectively).

Particularly concerning aspects regarding the introduction of core food groups in our study were the late introduction of iron-rich foods (7·3 %) and meat and alternatives (45·9 %), as foods in these food groups are recommended to be the first ones introduced due to their high iron content(3). Another concerning finding from this study is that one in ten infants had late introduction to fruits and vegetables. A cross-sectional study conducted in London nursery schools found that the earlier the introduction of fruits and vegetables, the higher the intake of these items among 2–6-year-olds(Reference Cooke, Wardle and Gibson56). However, a prospective study using data from four European cohorts found that associations between age of introduction to fruits and vegetables and intake of fruits and vegetables at the pre-school years were weaker and less consistent across the cohorts(Reference de Lauzon-Guillain, Jones and Oliveira57).

We did not find significant associations between infants’ sex and early food introduction, similar to some studies(Reference Rebhan, Kohlhuber and Schwegler58,Reference Scott, Binns and Graham59) but contrary to others(Reference Castro, Kearney and Layte60Reference Erkkola, Pigg and Virta-Autio63) which reported that boys were more likely to be introduced early to foods. Findings from the NZ health survey in 2019/20 for the indicator of introduction of solid foods before the age of 4 months also showed that boys were twice as likely as girls to be introduced early to solids(35). A systematic review reported that mothers’ reasons for introducing foods earlier to boys are multiple and include, among others, the mother’s perception of their infant’s needs(Reference Wijndaele, Lakshman and Landsbaugh29).

The higher prevalence of early food introduction among infants who were never breastfed or who were breastfed for shorter periods has also been reported in previous studies from the Netherlands, USA, Austria and Poland(Reference Wang, van Grieken and van der Velde51Reference Zielinska, Rust and Masztalerz-Kozubek53). It is well established that breast milk intake is displaced by infant formula milk, however, until recently, it was not clear if introduction of solid foods has the same effect in the duration of breast-feeding(Reference Lessa, Garcia and Emmett64). Lessa et al. (Reference Lessa, Garcia and Emmett64), using data from three large representative UK cohorts completed in the last 25 years, identified that, independent of background characteristics, earlier introduction of solids was associated with a shorter duration of breast-feeding with a dose–response relationship, especially in cohorts where earlier introduction of solids was the norm. A NZ trial (BLISS) which assessed the impact of a baby-led approach to complementary feeding (involving the recommendation of delaying the introduction of complementary feeding to the age of 6 months) found that BLISS infants were exclusively breastfed for longer than the control group(Reference Taylor, Williams and Fangupo65). The findings of Lessa et al(Reference Lessa, Garcia and Emmett64) and Taylor et al. (Reference Taylor, Williams and Fangupo65) support the recommendation that strategies which promote timely introduction of foods, per se, may impact on longer duration of breast-feeding in NZ. This indicator was previously reported as falling well below national recommendations (where only 37 % of NZ infants were breastfed for ≥ 12 months and 12·5 % for ≥ 24 months(Reference Castro, Grant and Wall39)).

Similar to our findings, previous studies across Europe and Oceania(Reference Aronsson, Uusitalo and Vehik30,Reference Wang, van Grieken and van der Velde51,Reference Zielinska, Rust and Masztalerz-Kozubek53,Reference Tromp, Briedé and Kiefte-de Jong66,Reference Schrempft, van Jaarsveld and Fisher67) reported that younger and less educated mothers were more likely to introduce foods early to their infants when compared with older and more educated mothers. Our study also showed that in relation to infants of European mothers, infants of Māori mothers were more likely to have early or late introduction to complementary feeding, while infants of Asian, Pacific and MELAA mothers were at increased risk of having late introduction to foods. Ethnic disparities in early-life nutrition have also been previously described within the GUiNZ cohort(Reference Castro, Grant and Wall39,Reference Castro, Gerritsen and Teixeira42,Reference Teixeira, Castro and Wall68) , indicating that, to ensure adequate nutrition in the first 1000 d of life in NZ, it is vital to address cultural differences in feeding practices. The association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and early introduction of infants to complementary foods reported in our study and in previous studies(Reference Aronsson, Uusitalo and Vehik30,Reference Castro, Kearney and Layte60,Reference Helle, Hillesund and Øverby69) may originate from behavioural, psychosocial and biological factors, such as observations that smoking mothers are less likely be health conscious and more likely to experience stress and have less social support and conscientiousness(Reference Maxson, Edwards and Ingram70).

Despite the independent association between late food introduction and attendance at childbirth preparation classes in NZ verified in our study, there is potential that this association may have been confounded or related to the unmeasured characteristics of parents who attend these classes, for example, health education seeking, and likelihood of following public health advice, among others. Additionally, the current content of childbirth classes in NZ appears to be heterogeneous, since some classes do not include content on infant feeding and timing of introduction to foods(71).

Strengths and limitations of the study

To the best of our knowledge, this represents the first NZ nationally generalisable study on the prevalence of non-timely food introduction and their main maternal determinants. The main limitation of this study refers to the level of accuracy of the reported prevalence of timely, early and late introduction given the following reasons. GUiNZ study did not collect information on infants’ age of introduction of water and cows’ milk. Each infant’s mother was only asked if their infant was introduced to cows’ milk by the date of the 9-month interview but not the age when the infants were introduced to it. Thus, the prevalence of early introduction of complementary feeding within the cohort may be potentially underestimated. However, the proportion of babies introduced to cows’ milk by the time of the 9-month interview was low within the cohort (2 % – data not presented). Another aspect is the fact that only one aggregated list of dairy foods (milk pudding, rice pudding, yogurt and custard) was included under the food group ‘dairy products’. Therefore, the lack of information on age of introduction of other recommended dairy products, such as cheese, may have potentially resulted in an overestimation of the prevalence of late introduction of dairy foods within the cohort. When calculating the prevalence of early, timely and late introduction, we opted to include those infants who were not introduced to complementary feeding or to the core food groups examined, in the denominators rather than rolling them to the group of late introduction. This is because we cannot assume that, for example, infants who were not introduced to dairy products or meats by the 9-month interview were introduced to these foods later in life. There may be situations where, for many reasons (health, religion and beliefs), parents may opt to not offer these items to their children. Finally, as the age of food introduction was retrospectively reported by the mothers only in months (and not in months and days), there is potential for inaccuracies in the estimation of the prevalence of early, timely and late food introduction within the GUiNZ cohort.

Importance of findings to infant public health nutrition

The findings reported in this nationally generalisable study can be used to guide NZ food and nutrition policies that aim to improve and support optimal timing of food introduction in NZ. Ensuring appropriate nutrition in the first year of life represents one of the internationally recommended platforms to maximise the impact for the prevention and reduction of the double burden of malnutrition during childhood and throughout the life course(72,73) .

As previously described, non-timely food introduction is associated with relevant health and nutritional issues during infancy and also subsequently during childhood and later in life(3,Reference English, Obbagy and Wong5Reference Mannan18) . These include iron deficiency anaemia, food allergies and childhood obesity(Reference Sun, Foskey and Allen6,Reference Wang, Liu and Wan10,Reference Hassan, El-Gendy and Badra14Reference Hicke-Roberts, Wennergren and Hesselmar23) , all child health conditions of public health relevance in NZ(28). Iron deficiency is of concern, with 14 % of children aged 6–23 months affected in a population-based study conducted in Auckland (NZ) from 1999 to 2002(Reference Grant, Wall and Brunt74). Findings from the largest epidemiological study among children, the International Study on Asthma and Allergies in Childhood (ISAAC)(Reference Asher, Stewart and Clayton75), found that NZ was rated in the ‘top five countries’ for asthma prevalence with levels similar to Australia, the UK, Ireland, the USA and Canada. It is assumed therefore that food allergy rates in NZ may be similar to rates found in those countries(Reference Asher, Stewart and Clayton75). Based on findings from an Australian population-based study of 12-month-olds in 2011, it is estimated that food allergies may affect more than 10 % of NZ infants(Reference Osborne, Koplin and Martin76). Childhood obesity represents one of the main national challenges in NZ, a country which was recently positioned second in the ranking for the prevalence of child/adolescent overweigh/obesity among the countries of the Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development/European Union (39·5 % of 5–19-year-old children)(28).

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that interventions to increase the prevalence of timely food introduction in NZ should be culturally sensitive and encompass the promotion and support of breast-feeding duration according to national recommendations(3), alongside improvements in policy and maternal health education. Future investigations will examine the impact of non-timely complementary feeding introduction on child/youth health and wellbeing outcomes in this cohort.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the participating families of the Growing Up in New Zealand cohort study who have given their time and shared the information that allowed them to conduct this research. The authors also thank the interviewers and researchers who designed, collected, cleaned and prepared the data for use.

The Growing Up in New Zealand study has been designed and conducted by the Growing Up in New Zealand team, led by the University of Auckland. The authors would like to acknowledge the contributions of the original study investigators: Susan M.B. Morton, Polly E. Atatoa Carr, Cameron C. Grant, Arier C. Lee, Dinusha K. Bandara, Jatender Mohal, Jennifer M. Kinloch, Johanna M. Schmidt, Mary R. Hedges, Vivienne C. Ivory, Te Kani R. Kingi, Renee Liang, Lana M. Perese, Elizabeth Peterson, Jan E. Pryor, Elaine Reese, Elizabeth M. Robinson, Karen E. Waldie and Clare R. Wall.

The Growing Up in New Zealand study has been funded by: the New Zealand Ministries of Social Development, Health, Education and Justice; the former Ministry of Science Innovation and the former Department of Labour (now both part of the Ministry of Business, Innovation and Employment); the former Ministry of Pacific Island Affairs (now the Ministry for Pacific Peoples); the former Ministry of Women’s Affairs (now the Ministry for Women); the Department of Corrections; the Families Commission and the former Social Policy Evaluation and Research Unit); Te Puni Kokiri; New Zealand Police; Sport New Zealand; Housing New Zealand Corporation; and the former Mental Health Commission (now part of the Office of the Health and Disability Commissioner); and The University of Auckland and Auckland UniServices Limited. Other support for the study has been provided by the Health Research Council of New Zealand, Statistics New Zealand, the Office of the Children’s Commissioner and the Office of Ethnic Affairs (now the Office of Ethnic Communities).

S.S.F. had a masters scholarship funded by the ‘Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nivel Superior – Brasil (CAPES) – Finance Code 001 (process number: 88882.330846/2019-01)’. T.G.C.’s salary was funded by the Discipline of Nutrition and Dietetics (Faculty of Medical Sciences, University of Auckland, New Zealand) and by Auckland Uniservices (project number 3723427).

S. S. F, T. G. C., C. W. and D. M. designed the objectives of the study. S. S. F. cleaned, merged and prepared the datasets. S. S. F. conducted the analysis, consulting with D. M. and T. G. C. S. S. F., T. G. C., C. W., S. G., J. A. T., C. C. G., S. M. B. and D. M. contributed to the data interpretation. S. S. F., T. G. C. and D. M. drafted the manuscript. C. W., S. G., J. A. T., C. C. G., S. M. B. and D. M. revised the manuscript draft critically. All listed authors reviewed and approved the final version of the manuscript.

There are no conflicts of interest.

Supplementary material

For supplementary materials referred to in this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S000711452200112X

References

Black, MM, Quigg, AM, Hurley, KM, et al. (2011) Iron deficiency and iron-deficiency anemia in the first two years of life: strategies to prevent loss of developmental potential. Nutr Rev 69, 6470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martorell, R (2017) Improved nutrition in the first 1000 days and adult human capital and health. Am J Hum Biol 29, e22952.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ministry of Health (2008) Food and Nutrition Guidelines for Healthy Infants and Toddlers (Aged 0–2): a Background Paper, 4th ed. Wellington: Minister of Health.Google Scholar
Spence, AC, Campbell, KJ, Lioret, S, et al. (2018) Early childhood vegetable, fruit, and discretionary food intakes do not meet dietary guidelines, but do show socioeconomic differences and tracking over time. J Acad Nutr Diet 118, 16341643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
English, LK, Obbagy, JE, Wong, YP, et al. (2019) Timing of introduction of complementary foods and beverages and growth, size, and body composition: a systematic review. Am J Clin Nutr 109, 935S955S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, C, Foskey, RJ, Allen, KJ, et al. (2016) The impact of timing of introduction of solids on infant body mass index. J Pediatr 179, 104110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
PAHO/WHO (2003) Guiding Principles for Complementary Feeding of the Breastfed Child. Division of Health Promotion and Protection. Food and Nutrition Program. Washington; Geneva: Pan American Health Organization/World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Przyrembel, H (2012) Timing of introduction of complementary food: short- and long-term health consequences. Ann Nutr Metab 60, 820.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butte, NF, Lopez-Alarcon, MG & Garza, C (2001) Nutrient Adequacy of Exclusive Breastfeeding for the Term Infant during the First Six Months of life. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Wang, F, Liu, H, Wan, Y, et al. (2017) Age of complementary foods introduction and risk of anemia in children aged 4–6 years: a prospective birth cohort in China. Sci Rep 23, 44726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schincaglia, RM, Oliveira, AC, Sousa, LM, et al. (2015) Práticas Alimentares e Fatores Associados à Introdução Precoce da Alimentação Complementar entre Crianças Menores de seis meses na Região Noroeste de Goiânia (Feeding practices and factors associated with early introduction of complementary feeding of children aged under six months in the northwest region of Goiânia, Brazil). Epidemiol Serv Saúde 24, 465474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davisse-Paturet, C, Adel-Patient, K, Forhan, A, et al. (2020) Breastfeeding initiation or duration and longitudinal patterns of infections up to 2 years and skin rash and respiratory symptoms up to 8 years in the EDEN mother-child cohort. Matern Chil Nutr 16, 3.Google ScholarPubMed
Pedraza, DF & Queiroz, D (2011) Micronutrientes no Crescimento e Desenvolvimento Infantil (Micronutrients in child growth and development). J Hum Growth Dev 21, 156171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hassan, FM, El-Gendy, FM, Badra, HS, et al. (2016) Evaluation of iron-deficiency anemia in infancy. Menoufia Med J, 29, 269274.Google Scholar
Zhao, A, Gao, H, Li, B, et al. (2016) Inappropriate feeding behavior: one of the important causes of malnutrition in 6- to 36- month-old children in Myanmar. Am J Trop Med Hyg 95, 702708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moss, BG & Yeaton, WH (2014) Early childhood healthy and obese weight status: potentially protective benefits of breastfeeding and delaying solid foods. Matern Child Health J 18, 12241232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huh, SY, Rifas-Shiman, SL, Taveras, EM, et al. (2011) Timing of solid food introduction and risk of obesity in preschool-aged children. Pediatrics 127, 544551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mannan, H (2018) Early infant feeding of formula or solid foods and risk of childhood overweight or obesity in a socioeconomically disadvantaged region of Australia: a longitudinal cohort analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health 15, 1685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Royal, C & Gray, C (2020) Allergy prevention: an overview of current evidence. Yale J Biol Med 93, 689698.Google ScholarPubMed
Comberiati, P, Costagliola, G, D’Elios, S et al. (2019) Prevention of food allergy: the significance of early introduction. Medicine 55, 323.Google ScholarPubMed
Poole, JA, Barriga, K, Leung, DYM, et al. (2006) Timing of initial exposure to cereal grains and the risk of wheat allergy. Pediatrics 117, 21752182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nwaru, BI, Erkkola, M, Ahonen, S, et al. (2010) Age at the introduction of solid food during the first year and allergic sensitization at age 5 years. Pediatrics 125, 5059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hicke-Roberts, A, Wennergren, G & Hesselmar, B (2020) Late introduction of solid into infants’ diet may increase the risk of food allergy development. BMC Pediatr 20, 274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Obbagy, JE, English, LK, Wong, YP, et al. (2019) Complementary feeding and food allergy, atopic dermatites/eczema, asthma, and allergic rhinitis: a systematic review. Am J Clin Nutr 109, 890S934S.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferraro, V, Zanconato, S & Carraro, S (2019) Timing of food introduction and the risk of food allergy. Nutrients 11, 1131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tran, MM, Lefebvre, DL, Dai, D, et al. (2017) Timing of food introduction and development of food sensitization in a prospective birth cohort. Pediatr Allergy Immunol 28, 471477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caffarelli, C, Di Mauro, D, Mastrorilli, C, et al. (2018) Solid food introduction and the development of food allergies. Nutrients 10, 1790.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
UNICEF (2019) The State of the World’s Children. Children, Food and Nutrition: Growing Well in a Changing World. New York: UNICEF.Google Scholar
Wijndaele, K, Lakshman, R, Landsbaugh, JR, et al. (2009) Determinants of early weaning and use of unmodified cow’s milk in infants: a systematic review. J Am Diet Assoc 109, 20172028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aronsson, CA, Uusitalo, U, Vehik, K, et al. (2015) Age at first introduction to complementary foods is associated with sociodemographic factors in children with increased genetic risk of developing type 1 diabetes. Matern Child Nutr 11, 803814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kronborg, H, Foverskov, E & Væth, M (2014) Predictors for early introduction of solid food among Danish mothers and infants: an observational study. BMC Pediatr 14, 243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manohar, N, Hayen, A, Bhole, S, et al. (2020) Predictors of early introduction of core and discretionary foods in Australian infants – results from HSHK birth cohort study. Nutrients 12, 258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alzaheb, RA (2016) Factors associated with the early introduction of complementary feeding in Saudi Arabia. Int J Environ Res Public Health 13, 702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hazir, T, Senarath, U, Agho, K, et al. (2012) Determinants of inappropriate timing of food introduction solid, semi-solid or soft food to infants in Pakistan: secondary data analysis of Demographic and Health Survey 2006–2007. Matern Child Nutr 1, 7888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ministry of Health (2020) Tier 1 Statistics 2019/2020: New Zealand Health Survey. Wellington: Ministry of Health.Google Scholar
Castro, TG, Gerritsen, S, Wall, C, et al. (2018) Infant Feeding in New Zealand: Adherence to Food and Nutrition Guidelines among the Growing up in New Zealand Cohort. Wellington: Ministry of Social Development.Google Scholar
Morton, SM, Atatoa Carr, PE, Grant, CC, et al. (2013) Cohort profile: growing Up in New Zealand. Int J Epidemiol 42, 6575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morton, SMB, Ramke, J, Kinloch, J, et al. (2015) Growing up in New Zealand cohort alignment with all New Zealand births. Aust N Z J Public Health 39, 8287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castro, TG, Grant, C, Wall, C, et al. (2017). Breastfeeding indicators among a nationally representative multi-ethnic sample of New Zealand children. NZMJ 130, 3444.Google ScholarPubMed
Marriot, LD, Inskip, HM, Borland, SE, et al. (2009) What do babies eat? Evaluation of a food frequency questionnaire to assess the diets of infants aged 12 months. Public Health Nutr 12, 967972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wall, CR, Brunt, DR & Grant, CC (2009) Ethnic variance in iron status: is it related to dietary intake? Public Health Nutr 12, 14131421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castro, TG, Gerritsen, S, Teixeira, JA, et al. (2022) An index measuring adherence to New Zealand Infant Feeding Guidelines has converg ent validity with maternal socio-demographic and health behaviours and with children’s body size. Br J Nutr 27(7), 10731085.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salmond, C, Crampton, P & Atkinson, J (2007) NZDep2006 Index of Deprivation. Wellington: University of Otago.Google Scholar
Statistics New Zealand (2005) Statistic Standard for Ethnicity Classification. Wellington: Stats New Zealand.Google Scholar
Atatoa Carr, P, Bandara, D, Berry, S, et al. (2017) Ethnic identification complexity across generations: evidence from Growing Up in New Zealand. New Zealand Popul Rev 43, 3562.Google Scholar
Morton, SMB, Grant, CC, Wall, CR, et al. (2014) Adherence to nutritional guidelines in pregnancy: evidence from the Growing Up in New Zealand birth cohort study. Public Health Nutr 17, 19191929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization. (1995) Physical Status: the Use and Interpretation of Anthropometry. WHO Technical Report Series no. 854. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
Craig, CL, Marshall, AL, Sjostrom, M, et al. (2003) International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Med Sci Sports Exerc 35, 13811395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morton, SMB, Atatoa Carr, PE, Bandara, DK, et al. (2010) Growing up in New Zealand: a Longitudinal Study of New Zealand Children and their Families. Report 1: Before we are Born. Auckland: Growing up in New Zealand.Google Scholar
Auckland District Health Board (2021) Auckland DHB Antenatal Classes- Pregnancy and Parenting Education. https://www.healthpoint.co.nz/community-health-services/community-health/auckland-dhb-antenatal-classes-pregnancy/ (accessed August 2021).Google Scholar
Wang, L, van Grieken, A, van der Velde, LA, et al. (2019) Factors associated with early introduction of complementary feeding and consumption of non-recommended foods among Dutch infants: the BeeBOFT study. BMC Public Health 19, 388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrera, CM, Hamner, HC, Perrine, CG, et al. (2018) Timing of introduction of complementary foods to US infants, National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2009–2014. J Acad Nutr Diet 118, 464470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zielinska, MA, Rust, P, Masztalerz-Kozubek, D, et al. (2019) Factors influencing the age of complementary feeding- a cross sectional study from two European countries. Int J Environ Res Public Health 16, 3799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magarey, A, Kavian, F, Scott, JA, et al. (2016) Feeding mode of Australian infants in the first 12 months of life: an assessment against national breastfeeding indicators. J Hum Lact 32, 95104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amezdroz, E, Carpenter, L, O’Callaghan, E, et al. (2015) Transition from milks to the introduction of solid foods across the first 2 years of life: findings from an Australian birth cohort study. J Hum Nutr Diet 8, 375383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooke, LJ, Wardle, J, Gibson, EL, et al. (2004) Demographic, familial and trait predictors of fruit and vegetable consumption by pre-school children. Public Health Nutr 7, 295302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Lauzon-Guillain, B, Jones, L, Oliveira, A, et al. (2013) The influence of early feeding practices on fruit and vegetable intake among preschool children in 4 European birth cohorts. Am J Clin Nutr 98, 804812.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rebhan, B, Kohlhuber, M, Schwegler, U, et al. (2009) Infant feeding practices and associated factors through the first 9 months of life in Bavaria, Germany. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 49, 467473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, JA, Binns, CW, Graham, KI, et al. (2009) Predictors of early introduction of solid foods in infants: results of a cohort study. BMC Pediatr 9, 60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castro, PD, Kearney, J & Layte, R (2015) A study of early complementary feeding determinants in the Republic of Ireland based on a cross-sectional analysis of the Growing Up in Ireland infant cohort. Public Health Nutr 18, 292302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lande, B, Andersen, LF, Bærug, A, et al. (2003) Infant feeding practices and associated factors in the first six months of life: the Norwegian Infant Nutrition Survey. Acta Paediatrica 92, 152161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grote, V, Schiess, SA, Closa-Monasterolo, R, et al. (2011) The introduction of solid food and growth in the first 2 y of life in the formula-fed children: analysis of data from a European cohort study. Am J Clin Nutr 94, 1785S1793S.Google ScholarPubMed
Erkkola, M, Pigg, H-M, Virta-Autio, P, et al. (2005) Infant feeding patterns in the Finnish type 1 diabetes prediction and prevention nutrition study cohort. Eur J Clin Nutr 59, 107113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lessa, A, Garcia, AL, Emmett, P, et al. (2020) Does early introduction of solid feeding lead to early cessation of breastfeeding? Matern Child Nutr 16, e12944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, RW, Williams, SM, Fangupo, LJ, et al. (2017) Effect of a baby-led approach to complementary feeding on infant growth and overweight: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr 171, 838846.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tromp, II, Briedé, S, Kiefte-de Jong, JC, et al. (2013) Factors associated with the timing of introduction of complementary feeding: the Generation R Study. Eur J Clin Nutr 67, 625630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schrempft, S, van Jaarsveld, CHM, Fisher, A, et al. (2013) Family and infant characteristics associated with timing of core and non-core food introduction in early childhood. Eur J Clin Nutr 67, 652657.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teixeira, J, Castro, T, Wall, C, et al. (2018) Determinants of folic acid supplement use outside national recommendations for pregnant women: results from the Growing Up in New Zealand cohort study. Public Health Nutr 21, 21832192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helle, C, Hillesund, ER & Øverby, NC (2018) Timing of complementary feeding and associations with maternal and infant characteristics: a Norwegian cross-sectional study. PLOS ONE 13, e0199455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maxson, PJ, Edwards, SE, Ingram, A, et al. (2012) Psychosocial differences between smokers and non-smokers during pregnancy. Addict Behav 37,153–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Auckland.midwife.org.nz. (2021) Auckland DHB Antenatal Classes- Pregnancy and Parenting Education. Auckland, New Zealand. https://auckland.midwife.org.nz/for-birthing-women/antenatal-classes/ (accessed October 2021).Google Scholar
WHO (2014) 2017 Global Nutrition Targets 2025: Policy Brief Series (WHO/NMH/NHD/14.2). Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
WHO (2017) The Double Burden of Malnutrition. Policy Brief. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Grant, CC, Wall, CR, Brunt, D, et al. (2007) Population prevalence and risk factors for iron deficiency in Auckland, New Zealand. J Paediatr Child Health 43, 532538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asher, MI, Stewart, AW, Clayton, T, et al. (2008) Has the prevalence and severity of symptoms of asthma changed among children in New Zealand? ISASC phase three. NZ Med J 121, 5263.Google Scholar
Osborne, NJ, Koplin, JJ, Martin, PE, et al. (2011) Prevalence of challenge-proven IgE-mediated food allergy using population-based sampling and predetermined challenge criteria in infants. J Allergy Clin Immunol 127, 668676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Flow chart describing the study sample.

Figure 1

Table 1. Food items with the Growing Up in New Zealand cohort study FFQ included in each of the core food groups recommended for infants

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Prevalence of timely (5–6 months), early (< 4 months) and late (> 7 months) introduction of complementary feeding and of each recommended core food group (all cohort, n = 5770), Growing Up in New Zealand cohort.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Prevalence of early (< 4 months) and late (> 7 months) introduction of complementary feeding and of each recommended core food group according to duration of any breast-feeding (< 6 months: n 1740; ≥ 6 months: n 3847), Growing Up in New Zealand cohort.

Figure 4

Table 2. Prevalence of early, timely and late introduction of complementary feeding according to infant and maternal characteristics(number and percentages)

Figure 5

Table 3. Adjusted RRR and 95 % CI for the associations between timing of food introduction and variables describing breast-feeding duration and maternal sociodemographic and health behaviours(all cohort, n = 4595)

Supplementary material: File

Ferreira et al. supplementary material

Table S1

Download Ferreira et al. supplementary material(File)
File 32.4 KB