Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vfjqv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T12:58:00.824Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Insights into the uniquely human origins of understanding other minds

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 November 2021

Tobias Grossmann
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA22903, USA. tg3ny@virginia.eduhttps://psychology.as.virginia.edu/people/profile/tg3nykld2db@virginia.eduhttps://psychology.as.virginia.edu/people/profile/kld2db
Kenn Lacsamana Dela Cruz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA22903, USA. tg3ny@virginia.eduhttps://psychology.as.virginia.edu/people/profile/tg3nykld2db@virginia.eduhttps://psychology.as.virginia.edu/people/profile/kld2db

Abstract

We summarize research and theory to show that, from early in human ontogeny, much information about other minds can be gleaned from reading the eyes. This analysis suggests that eyes serve as uniquely human windows into other minds, which critically extends the target article by drawing attention to what might be considered the neurodevelopmental origins of knowledge attribution in humans.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Fairhurst, M. T., Löken, L., & Grossmann, T. (2014). Physiological and behavioral responses reveal 9-month-old infants’ sensitivity to pleasant touch. Psychological Science, 25, 11241131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossmann, T. (2017). The eyes as windows into other minds: An integrative perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 107121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossmann, T., Oberecker, R., Koch, S. P., & Friederici, A. D. (2010). The developmental origins of voice processing in the human brain. Neuron, 65, 852858.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jessen, S., & Grossmann, T. (2014). Unconscious discrimination of social cues from eye whites in infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111, 1620816213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jessen, S., & Grossmann, T. (2020). The developmental origins of subliminal face processing. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 116, 454460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kano, F., & Tomonaga, M. (2010). Face scanning in chimpanzees and humans: Continuity and discontinuity. Animal Behaviour, 79, 227235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G., Schlicht, T., & Vogeley, K. (2013). Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 393414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schroeder, J., & Epley, N. (2015). The sound of intellect: Speech reveals a thoughtful mind, increasing a job candidate's appeal. Psychological Science, 26, 877891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Hare, B., Lehmann, H., & Call, J. (2007). Reliance on head versus eyes in the gaze following of great apes and human infants: The cooperative eye hypothesis. Journal of Human Evolution, 52, 314320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Melis, A. P., Tennie, C., Wyman, E., & Herrmann, E. (2012). Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: The interdependence hypothesis. Current Anthropology, 53(6), 673692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar