Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-pjpqr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-27T07:28:15.043Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The ephemeral stories of our lives

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2023

Nick Chater*
Affiliation:
Behavioural Science Group, Warwick Business School, University of Warwick, Coventry CV4 7AL, UK. nick.chater@wbs.ac.uk

Abstract

Johnson et al. make a persuasive case that qualitative, story-like reasoning plays a crucial role in everyday thought and decision-making. This commentary questions the cohesiveness of this type of reasoning and the representations that generate it. Perhaps narratives do not underpin, but are ephemeral products of thought, created when we need to justify our actions to ourselves and others.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bloom, P. (2005). Descartes' baby: How the science of child development explains what makes us human. Random House.Google Scholar
Chater, N. (2018). The mind is flat. Penguin.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1978). A cure for the common code. In Dennett, D. C. (Ed.), Brainstorms: Philosophical essays on mind and psychology (pp. 90108). Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Erickson, T. A., & Mattson, M. E. (1981). From words to meaning: A semantic illusion. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 540552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57(2), 243259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herman, J. (1997). Mysteries of “The Big Sleep”. The Los Angeles Times, 4 December. https://www.latimes.com/archives/la-xpm-1997-dec-04-ca-60391-story.htmlGoogle Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M. (1980). Image and mind. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Muryy, A., & Glennerster, A. (2021). Route selection in non-Euclidean virtual environments. PLoS ONE, 16(4), e0247818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penrose, L. S., & Penrose, R. (1958). Impossible objects: A special type of visual illusion. British Journal of Psychology, 49, 3133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1981). The imagery debate: Analogue media versus tacit knowledge. Psychological Review, 88(1), 1645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2002). Mental imagery: In search of a theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(2), 157182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2003). Seeing and visualizing: It's not what you think. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding: An inquiry into human knowledge structures. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55(4), 189208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsetsos, K., Moran, R., Moreland, J., Chater, N., Usher, M., & Summerfield, C. (2016). Economic irrationality is optimal during noisy decision making. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 31023107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed