Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wzw2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T12:37:21.561Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Food portion sizes: trends and drivers in an obesogenic environment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 January 2024

Maria Papagiannaki*
Affiliation:
Nutrition Innovation Centre for Food and Health (NICHE), School of Biomedical Sciences, Ulster University, Coleraine, BT52 1SA, UK Middlesex University, Department of Natural Sciences, The Burroughs, London, NW4 4BT, UK
Maeve A. Kerr
Affiliation:
Nutrition Innovation Centre for Food and Health (NICHE), School of Biomedical Sciences, Ulster University, Coleraine, BT52 1SA, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Maria Papagiannaki, email: m.papagiannaki@mdx.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The prevalence of overweight and obesity in children and adults has increased worldwide. A strong environmental factor contributing to the obesity epidemic is food portion size (PS). This review evaluates the current evidence linking food PS to obesity, examines the effects of PS on energy intake (EI), and discusses the drivers of food PS selection. The leading causes of the rise in PS include globalisation, intensive farming methods, the impact of World War II, due to shortage of staple foods, and the notion of ‘waste not, want not’. Large PS of energy-dense foods may stimulate overconsumption, leading to high EI levels. However, the studies have not shown a cause-and-effect relationship, due to confounding factors. Important mechanisms explaining the attractiveness of larger PS leading to higher EI levels are value for money, portion distortion, labels on food packaging, and tableware. Consumers depend on external rather than internal PS cues to guide consumption, irrespective of satiety levels. Further research is recommended on food consumption patterns to inform policymakers and provide information and insights about changes in diet.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Introduction

Obesity and overweight levels have risen continuously worldwide over the past two decades, with 39% of adults currently considered to be overweight and 13% obese(1). In England, 27% of men and 29% of women are obese. About two-thirds of adults are overweight or obese, with prevalence higher in men (68%) than women (60%). Notably, obesity is up to 9% more prevalent in deprived areas than in those less deprived(Reference Newton, Briggs and Murray2,3) . Adult obesity is defined as having a body mass index (BMI) of 30 kg/m2 or above. By 2025, one-fifth of adults are expected to be obese(Reference Reilly, El-Hamdouchi and Diouf4).

Biological and physiological factors are the main drivers affecting food intake. However, extrinsic cues such as food portion size (PS) and food visibility are often used to regulate food intake(Reference Logue, Doherty and McCann5). Hormones that control appetite, such as leptin and ghrelin, are crucial in determining food intake. Their fasting levels are different between normal weight and overweight individuals(Reference Myers, Leibel and Seeley6). Food PS represents an environmental factor contributing to the obesity epidemic(Reference Wansink and Kim7,Reference Livingstone and Pourshahidi8) which has changed the food environment(Reference Marchiori, Papies and Klein9), with exposure to large food PS encouraging greater consumption and subsequent excess energy intake(Reference Zlatevska, Dubelaar and Holden10,Reference Hollands, Shemilt and Marteau11) . Food PS is defined as the amount of food served available for instant consumption in a single eating event(Reference Almiron-Roig, Navas-Carretero and Emery12). It is well documented that standard PS in foods and drinks has increased in the past two decades(Reference Steenhuis, Leeuwis and Vermeer13Reference Nielsen, Barry and Popkin16). This review aims to (a) review the current evidence linking food PS to obesity, (b) review the effects of food PS on energy intake (EI) and (c) discuss the main drivers of food PS selection.

Overview of evidence on current trends in portion size

Portion size

PS guidance has been developed in the UK(Reference Wrieden, Gregor and Barton17,Reference Benson18) , the USA(Reference Young and Nestle19,Reference Smiciklas-Wright, Mitchell and Mickle20) , the Netherlands(Reference Steenhuis, Leeuwis and Vermeer13) and Denmark(Reference Matthiessen, Fagt and Biltoft-Jensen21). The PS for home-cooked meals as specified in a Danish cookbook has increased over the past century(Reference Eidner, Qvistgaard Lund and Schroll Harboe22). A French study stated that the increase in the PS trend is 25% smaller compared with the USA(Reference Rozin, Kabnick and Pete23), where a fivefold increase since the 1970s is noted(Reference Young and Nestle24), indicating a trend towards larger PS. This has continued worldwide over the last 40 years(Reference English, Lasschuijt and Keller25), introducing new larger-sized portions in popular and energy-dense foods, such as pre-packed snacks(Reference Rolls, Roe and Kral26), white-bread products, macaroni(Reference Rolls, Morris and Roe27), cakes, popcorn(Reference Wansink and Park28), soft drinks(Reference Flood, Roe and Rolls29) and alcoholic beverages(Reference Young and Nestle30).

The evolved emphasis on ‘super-sized’ portions(Reference Young and Nestle30), share packs, king-size packs and duo packs(Reference Steenhuis, Leeuwis and Vermeer13) initiates overeating(Reference Pourshahidi, Kerr and McCaffrey31). Consequently, consumers are attracted to the ‘value for money’ offered(Reference Steenhuis and Vermeer32,Reference Steenhuis and Poelman33) , consuming higher amounts than the national recommendations(Reference Bryant and Dundes34Reference Condrasky, Ledikwe and Flood36), even up to eight times larger(Reference Young and Nestle24), and increasing the bite-size mechanism (increased bite-size consumption when the PS is larger)(Reference Burger, Fisher and Johnson37,Reference Almiron-Roig, Tsiountsioura and Lewis38) . This was first documented in young children (3–5 and 8–9 years old)(Reference Fisher, Rolls and Birch39,Reference Fisher40) . Furthermore, in existing dietary guidance, the terms PS and serving size (the quantity recommended to be consumed on a single eating occasion)(Reference Pourshahidi, Kerr and McCaffrey31) have been deemed to be confusing, and consumers feel challenged when deciding on the appropriate amount of food to consume(Reference Faulkner, Pourshahidi and Wallace41). Accounting for the constant change in PS and evident international differences(Reference Poelman, Eyles and Dunford42), continuous international monitoring is vital(Reference Steenhuis and Poelman33), as adults between 18 and 65 years old are mostly affected by these changes(Reference Nielsen and Popkin14,Reference Bryant and Dundes34) .

In Britain, the leading causes of the rise in PS include globalisation, intensive farming methods, the impact of World War II, due to shortage of staple foods, and the notion of ‘waste not, want not’(Reference Benson18). UK PS guidance has been considered outdated, contributing further to this phenomenon(Reference Rippin, Hutchinson and Jewell43,Reference Spence, Livingstone and Hollywood44) and leading to a distorted perception of appropriate PS(Reference Levitsky and Youn45). This increase has been noticed in restaurants(Reference Steenhuis and Poelman33,Reference Wansink and van Ittersum46) , fast-food restaurants(Reference Cutler, Glaeser and Shapiro47), take-out shops(Reference Young and Nestle19), supermarkets(Reference Nielsen and Popkin14,Reference Young and Nestle24) and in-home recipes(Reference Eidner, Qvistgaard Lund and Schroll Harboe22,Reference Wansink48) .

The UK Government provides advice on healthy eating and the daily consumption of key food groups as part of a healthy balanced diet as part of the ongoing calorie reduction strategy(49,50) . Nevertheless, guidance on the consumption of the appropriate PS of each food group and key foods within food groups is limited(51), compounding consumers’ confusion(Reference Lewis, Ahern and Jebb52).

Current trends in food portion size from National Survey Data

The UK National Diet and Nutrition Survey (NDNS) has previously presented inconsistent trends in PS over 15 years(Reference Wrieden, Gregor and Barton17). Savoury food PS appears to have increased, while that of potatoes/chips, desserts, and some fruits and vegetables has decreased(Reference Church15). Examining PS from numerous sources, namely NDNS data, past publications and manufacturers’ information between 1987 and 2006, an increase in PS was identified in confectionery products and fast foods, while PS in other foods decreased(Reference Church15,Reference Wrieden, Gregor and Barton17) . For example, the PS of McDonald’s Big Mac changed from 204 g in 1993 to 216 g in 2006. Similarly, the PS of the McChicken sandwich was 159 g in 1993 in comparison with 170 g in 2006(Reference Church15,Reference Church53) . Since the 1986/1987 NDNS, lifestyle changes, such as a broader food culture and less preparation time, have resulted in a wider range of food being available in the UK, alternating the trends(Reference Wrieden, Barton and Cochrane54). Furthermore, perhaps this increase was driven by changes in the British Agricultural Policy, which introduced the Common Agricultural Policy in the 1950s, encouraging intensive farming methods and more cost-effective food production(Reference Fennel55). This resulted in an excessive amount of cheap, readily available food(Reference Davey56) that consumers became accustomed to(Reference Vermeer, Steenhuis and Seidell57). Additionally, in Ireland, the North-South Ireland Food Consumption Survey between 1997 and 2001 and the National Adult Nutrition Survey between 2008 and 2010 suggested significant increases in the PS of white bread, wholemeal/brown bread, milk, meat and poultry, and significant decreases in potatoes/chips and sliced ham. No significant change over time was identified in yoghurt, cheese, processed potato products, butter/spreads and ham/bacon(Reference O’Brien, Livingstone and McNulty58). In contrast, the USA showed a continuously rising trend in PS for the majority of the food groups(Reference Nielsen, Barry and Popkin16) regardless of age and sex according to the Continuing Survey of Food Intake by Individuals conducted by the US Department of Agriculture(Reference Smiciklas-Wright, Mitchell and Mickle20). The latest NDNS data indicate that over 11 years (2008/2009–2018/2019), there was a 7% increase in consuming five-a-day, but only in women aged 19–64 years old. A reduction in mean consumption of red and processed meat across all age groups was noted. For the same timeframe, in sugar and chocolate confectionery, a reduction of 8% and 10%, respectively, was noted among those aged 11–18 years old(59).

To summarise, consumption of numerous food groups has increased over time, emphasising large PS and value for money(Reference Steenhuis and Vermeer32,Reference Steenhuis and Poelman33) . The increase in PS is due to globalisation(Reference Benson18) and possibly from changes in government policies leading to the production of inexpensive food(Reference Davey56). However, consumers may be confused as to what constitutes a standard PS. Research is required to identify foods that may be over- or under-consumed and to examine potential impacts on overall dietary adequacy.

Effects of portion size on energy intake

Adults and portion size

Observational studies

Numerous studies have identified that EI increases with exposure to larger PS, showing a potential risk factor for overweight and obesity(Reference Rosenheck60). The increase in consumption rates due to larger PS is up to 25% during lunch, and up to 45% for snacking(Reference Young and Nestle24). Longitudinal and cross-sectional studies have proposed a positive association between the consumption or occurrence of eating outside the home and increased BMI or weight gain(Reference Binkley, Eales and Jekanowski61Reference Kant and Graubard64). Representation of an appropriate amount to consume is structured by PS encountered in various places, such as supermarkets, restaurants, marketing images or the home(Reference Marteau, Hollands and Shemilt65). Nevertheless, the wide diversity in the characteristics of the food sector within the data leads to disagreement. Some associations were not identified(Reference Orfanos, Naska and Trichopoulos66,Reference Marin-Guerrero, Gutierrez-Fisac and Guallar-Castillon67) , or they were identified only in women(Reference Kant and Graubard64) or only in men(Reference Bezerra and Sichieri68,Reference Naska, Orfanos and Trichopoulou69) . Furthermore, serving the same food amount at various junctures established a notable consistency in the food type and the amount consumed, especially in individuals who learn the PS they need to feel satiety (‘previous experience/expectation mechanism’). The choice of PS may be influenced by prior experiences(Reference Steenhuis and Poelman33). For instance, the PS chosen and consumed at a later time depends on prior experiences with the degree of fullness produced by a food, in both adults(Reference Steenhuis and Poelman33) and children(Reference Brunstrom70).

Recent data from the Kantar Market Research Group revealed that during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic eating patterns changed considerably globally, highlighting an increase in sugar, sweeteners, herbs, seasonings, olive oil and alcohol purchases for home consumption, whereas other purchases such as health and beauty products declined(71). This speculates that the population worldwide, including the UK, was treating themselves to food. A recent scoping review indicated that the lockdown had positive (increased consumption of fresh produce and home cooking) and negative impacts (unhealthy snacking, mental health issues, physical inactivity and weight gain) on dietary practices globally(Reference Bennett, Young and Butler72). A study commissioned by the UK Food Standards Agency (FSA) identified the same beneficial changes in eating habits in households an increase in unhealthy snacking(Reference Lasko-Skinner and Sweetland73,74) . The latter was indicated in another UK study(Reference Scott and Ensaff75) and the European Institute of Innovation and Technology(76). Furthermore, it is indicated that the existing food security issues experienced in Brexit(Reference Barons and Aspinall77,Reference Ranta and Mulrooney78) , post-Brexit(Reference Ranta and Mulrooney78) and the COVID-19 pandemic(Reference Lasko-Skinner and Sweetland73,74,Reference Barons and Aspinall77,Reference Rivington, King and Duckett79) have changed the consumption patterns, highlighting a change in food trends.

Experimental studies

Various experimental studies have shown a potential long-term association between fast-food or takeaway consumption, and high EI(Reference Rosenheck60,Reference Ello-Martin, Ledikwe and Rolls80,Reference Zlatevska, Dubelaar and Holden81) , independent of individuals’ satiety levels(Reference Diliberti, Bordi and Conklin82). This suggests that adults ignore both hunger and satiety signs in the presence of external cues, such as large PS(Reference Kral, Roe and Rolls83,Reference Wansink, Painter and North84) , especially when eating out with others(Reference Ello-Martin, Ledikwe and Rolls80,Reference Hermans, Larsen and Herman85) . It is proposed that individuals learn to eat in the absence of hunger as children and continue to adulthood with this eating behaviour(Reference Fisher, Rolls and Birch39,Reference Rolls, Roe and Meengs86) , indicating a major determinant of food consumption(Reference Nielsen and Popkin14,Reference Jeffery, Rydell and Dunn87) . Indeed, continuous exposure to more than 6 months of high-energy lunch consumption led to a significant increase in EI and weight gain(Reference French, Mitchell and Wolfson88). A recent scoping review of randomised-controlled trials (RCT) and quasi-experimental studies highlighted a significant effect in lowering food consumption when offered a single smaller package compared with a larger one(Reference Liu, Tam and Rangan89). A systematic review and meta-analysis of RCT indicated a moderate to large reduction in daily EI when comparing smaller with larger PS(Reference Robinson, McFarland-Lesser and Patel90). A meta-analysis of RCT and cross-over trials (COT) indicated that the association between PS and food intake is not linear in population groups(Reference Zlatevska, Dubelaar and Holden10). Furthermore, a Cochrane review of seventy-two RCT indicated that doubling a PS leads to an increase of approximately 35% in energy consumption, mostly noticed in men and non-overweight individuals(Reference Hollands, Shemilt and Marteau11).

Studies have indicated that individuals determine the food amount consumed according to what they are accustomed to eating. This is related to frequent exposure to large portion consumption (unit size) over time(Reference Ello-Martin, Ledikwe and Rolls80,Reference Vandenbroele, Van Kerckhove and Zlatevska91) . This is called the portion size effect (PSE) or portion size response(Reference Rolls, Roe and Meengs92) (more food is offered, more is consumed)(Reference Almiron-Roig, Navas-Carretero and Emery12) and is documented in naturalistic environments, such as offices and restaurants(Reference Diliberti, Bordi and Conklin82,Reference Geier, Rozin and Doros93) . A Cochrane review of RCT identified a consistent PSE on EI in adults consuming more food when offered in larger-size versions(Reference Hollands, Shemilt and Marteau11). Furthermore, a systematic review and meta-analysis of RCT of PSE indicated that consuming larger PS was related to higher daily EI (295 kcal; 95% CI: 202, 388 kcal)(Reference Higgins, Hudson and Hayes94). A study has attempted to identify mechanisms of the PSE on food intake, indicating the importance of the dual-process theory(Reference Kahneman95). It has been proposed that the mind has two thinking systems: system 1 (intuition) and system 2 (reasoning). A research dialogue stated that system 1 is heuristic, with a preference for a dominant option that stands out based on appeal. However, when there is no dominant option, consumers may have difficulty making a decision. As a result, system 2 is activated, selecting the alternative according to their goals by comparing the attributes and values(Reference Dhar and Gorlin96). Consumers use the latter to make decisions based on time constraints, processing capacity, desired level of accuracy, and fatigue(Reference Dhar and Gorlin96). An independent relationship of other factors, such as nutritional status(Reference Rolls, Morris and Roe27,Reference Wansink, Painter and North84) , sex(Reference Rolls, Morris and Roe27) and posterior compensation(Reference Flood, Roe and Rolls29,Reference Rolls, Roe and Meengs97) was observed. More research is required to address the reasons that individuals do not comply with the satiety cues in their eating environments(Reference Ello-Martin, Ledikwe and Rolls80).

Larger effects on PS consumption have been identified in men(Reference Rolls, Roe and Kral26,Reference Rolls, Roe and Meengs86,Reference Rolls, Roe and Kral98) compared with women. Additionally, if larger PS was combined with higher energy density (ED) (the amount of energy in each weight of food), stronger effects were observed on total daily EI(Reference Marteau, Hollands and Shemilt65). Moreover, the effects of PS can persist for several days as evidenced at 2(Reference Rolls, Roe and Kral26,Reference Rolls, Roe and Meengs86) , 4(Reference Kelly, Wallace and Robson99) and 11 d(Reference Rolls, Roe and Meengs97). The larger PS of high-energy-dense foods has the greatest impact on EI, up to 279 kcal/d, especially in pre-packed foods. COT have suggested that ED directly influences ad libitum EI and provides an independent effect on the macronutrient composition of food(Reference Rolls100). Table 1 summarises studies’ evolution over time, indicating the relationship between PS and EI, including ED in adults.

Table 1. Shows studies researching the relationship between portion sizes (PS) and energy intake (EI) and/or energy density (ED) in adults

PS, portion sizes; EI, energy intake; ED, energy density.

To summarise, studies with similar design and methodology indicate a hypothetical association between large PS and obesity(Reference Rolls, Roe and Kral26,Reference Rolls, Morris and Roe27,Reference Kral, Roe and Rolls83,Reference Wansink, Painter and North84,Reference Rolls, Roe and Meengs86,Reference Rolls, Roe and Meengs92,Reference Rolls, Roe and Meengs97Reference Kelly, Wallace and Robson99) . However, it is difficult to determine whether an independent risk factor exists, due to confounding factors. Further research is needed due to the controlled environment of the above studies, where social interactions and food-related reminders were non-existent and blinding bias was absent(Reference Benton101,Reference Gough, Haynes and Clarke102) . Exploring the exact relationship between high EI, energy-dense foods and PS in real-life settings is needed to draw stronger conclusions(Reference Kral, Roe and Rolls83). Longer-duration studies are required in real-life environments to establish whether a cause-and-effect relationship exists between PS and daily EI(Reference French, Mitchell and Wolfson88,Reference Hetherington and Blundell-Birtill103) .

Children and portion size

A recent narrative review demonstrated that parents make food-related decisions for their children based on their own consumption patterns, their own gut feelings and their understanding of their appetites(Reference Acolatse, Poursahidi and Logue104). Due to the routine nature of food provision, parental choices regarding their child’s PS may be made automatically or as part of a complex process influenced by several interconnected factors, such as the child’s weight status, other family members and the parents’ own mealtime experiences as children. Parents might also base their judgements on PS on the amount of physical activity their children engage in; larger portions are served to youngsters who are thought to be more active(Reference Acolatse, Poursahidi and Logue104). Furthermore, although research suggests that customised nutritional advising systems are superior to general, one-size-fits-all approaches in improving health indices, these findings have been observed in adult populations(Reference Hoevenaars, Berendsen and Pasman105). Below are reviewed the observational and experimental studies.

Observational studies

Studies have indicated that large PS is positively associated with obesity in young children aged 1–5 years old(Reference McConahy, Smiciklas-Wright and Birch106,Reference McConahy, Smiciklas-Wright and Mitchell107) . In the UK between 1997 and 2005, adolescents consumed an increased amount of PS and EI from snacks (drinks, crisps, savoury snacks) and breakfast cereals(Reference Kerr, Rennie and McCaffrey108). Moreover, PS is positively associated with BMI percentiles in boys from 6 to 11 years old and children from 12 to 19 years old(Reference Huang, Howarth and Lin109). For pre-schoolers, 4–6 years old, EI is regulated by natural hunger-driven eating behaviours. However, environmental cues, such as large PS, can disturb this self-regulation(Reference Mrdjenovic and Levitsky110). In infants (11 months and younger) the relationship between ED and average PS is negative, proposing that as ED decreases, food intake downregulates correspondingly. In contrast, no association has been noted in toddlers (1–2 years old)(Reference Fox, Devaney and Reidy111). Evidence from a systematic review in the USA supports the positive relationship between ED and weight gain throughout life(112). Overall, PS is consistently positively associated with both EI and children’s weight(Reference Rolls100); nevertheless, these data cannot determine causality(Reference Pourshahidi, Kerr and McCaffrey31).

Experimental studies

Most experimental studies have taken place in the USA and have established that when children aged 3–6 years old doubled the PS of their main meal it resulted in an increase of EI by 40%(Reference Fisher, Rolls and Birch39,Reference Rolls, Engell and Birch113Reference Fisher, Liu and Birch115) , where the main food given was macaroni and cheese(Reference Fisher, Rolls and Birch39,Reference Rolls, Engell and Birch113,Reference Fisher, Liu and Birch115) or a selection of foods, including macaroni and cheese(Reference Vandenbroele, Van Kerckhove and Zlatevska91). Vegetables, mainly carrots, were offered alongside the food(Reference Young and Nestle30,Reference Rolls, Engell and Birch113Reference Fisher, Liu and Birch115) . The average PS consumed by 2-year-olds appears to have remained stable over the last 20 years, while PS increased(Reference Ello-Martin, Ledikwe and Rolls80). This supports the hypothesis that 3-year-old children self-regulate their intake according to hunger and satiety rather than food cues(Reference Rolls, Engell and Birch113,Reference Birch and Fisher116) . As children grow, internal cues, such as satiation, are less effective on food intake, while external factors are more influential, such as watching TV(Reference Ello-Martin, Ledikwe and Rolls80,Reference Boulos, Vikre and Oppenheimer117) . Nevertheless, studies in a controlled environment have validated the significant positive effect of larger PS on EI in 2-year-old children(Reference Fisher40,Reference Fisher, Arreola and Birch114,Reference Fisher, Liu and Birch115) . It is not known what the results would be in a free-living environment.

Few short-term studies have examined the impact of a reduction in children’s PS with a positive effect in reducing the EI in age-appropriate PS(Reference Smith, Conroy and Wen118,Reference Savage, Fisher and Marini119) . No change in EI was noted when the PS of an entrée was decreased by 25%(Reference Leahy, Birch and Fisher120). While studies are limited, they provide evidence that children from an early age are vulnerable to PS cues. Children increase the PS by eating more, but the evidence is weak to determine if they compensate for this at following eating points(Reference Leahy, Birch and Fisher120), such as in adults(Reference Kelly, Wallace and Robson99,Reference Rolls100) . No available data exist to investigate the long-term effects of PS in children(Reference Pourshahidi, Kerr and McCaffrey31). A study in a controlled environment indicated that computerised manual PS selection can be observed in children between 5 and 11 years old and the correspondence between manual portion selection and actual intake improves with age. This highlights that the relationship between children’s cognitive development and PS may help to develop age-appropriate PS(Reference Cox, Hinton and Sauchelli121).

COT have indicated that reducing the ED of an entrée reduces children’s total EI(Reference Leahy, Birch and Fisher120,Reference Leahy, Birch and Rolls122,Reference Leahy, Birch and Rolls123) . However, manipulation of the ED of a single snack did not significantly affect children’s EI at a single eating juncture(Reference Looney and Raynor124). Additional research revealed that the EI effect could be continued when the ED of multiple meals were manipulated over 2 d(Reference Leahy, Birch and Fisher120). Also, reduction of ED has a positive effect on adiposity in the longer term, particularly when individuals, both children, and adults, are receiving positively focused messages about weight control(Reference Epstein, Paluch and Beecher125). Table 2 summarises studies’ evolution over time, indicating the relationship between PS and EI, including ED in children.

Table 2. Shows studies researching the relationship between portion sizes (PS) and energy intake (EI) and/or energy density (ED) in children

PS, portion sizes; EI, energy intake; ED, energy density.

A great deal of evidence from COT positively associates ED, adiposity and PS in children(Reference Leahy, Birch and Fisher120,Reference Leahy, Birch and Rolls122Reference Looney and Raynor124) . Studies strongly support that 5-year-old children respond to increasing PS(Reference Rolls, Engell and Birch113Reference Fisher, Liu and Birch115). While a direct causal link between obesity and PS has yet to be determined, consumption of large PS of ED foods promotes obesity-eating behaviours in children(Reference Pourshahidi, Kerr and McCaffrey31,Reference Rolls100) . More research is required on influencing PS education and downsizing strategies for parents/carers, thus helping children to consume age-appropriate PS(Reference Small, Lane and Vaughan126,Reference Hetherington127) . This could identify a possible EI reduction strategy that has still not been demonstrated(Reference Savage, Haisfield and Fisher128). Additional strategies are required to help children to recognise and respond appropriately to internal signals and resist environmental influences on PS(Reference Small, Lane and Vaughan126).

Drivers of portion size selection

Portion distortion

Consumers’ inability to estimate PS and permanent exposure to larger versions contributes to a positive association of perceiving large PS (visual norm) as appropriate amounts consumed on a single eating occasion(Reference Matthiessen, Fagt and Biltoft-Jensen21). This is called ‘portion distortion’(Reference Wansink and Park28) and refers as well to consumers who do not realise that the PS mostly exceeds the serving size(Reference Bryant and Dundes34). Figure 1 helps to illustrate this. This is mostly noted in young individuals where a possible contribution to increasing both EI and waistlines was identified(Reference Steenhuis and Poelman33).

Fig. 1. The underlying factors causing portion distortion.

Steenhuis IH and Vermeer WM (2009) Portion size: review and framework for interventions. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act, 6, 1–10 [adapted]. Copyright © 2009, Steenhuis and Vermeer; licensee BioMed Central Ltd.

Labels on food packaging

Serving size guidance is voluntarily included on food packaging across the European Union (EU)(129), and is mandatory in the USA(130); however, consumers prefer household measure guidance(Reference Faulkner, Livingstone and Pourshahidi131), such as a portion control cup(Reference Logue, Doherty and McCann5), rather than referring to food labels for managing PS(Reference McConahy, Smiciklas-Wright and Birch106). Household measures result in positive behavioural changes, particularly in staple foods such as cereals, rice and pasta(Reference Logue, Doherty and McCann5). Consumers refer mostly to quality, quantity, brand, price and sell-by date, and less to ethical and sustainable food labels(Reference Ghvanidze, Velikova and Dodd132), therefore preferring visual impressions of packages and PS(Reference Ordabayeva and Chandon133). This leads to ‘awareness and estimation bias’(Reference Steenhuis and Poelman33) as they fail to identify and understand the quantity information or representation of food(Reference Lennard, Mitchell and McGoldrick134) in the packages, particularly if it is obtainable in non-metric units(Reference Viswanathan, Rosa and Harris135). A study that looked at the differences between suggested serving sizes in energy-dense foods indicated a lack of clarity in serving size guidance’ emphasises. This clearly indicates the need for effective and meaningful guidance on pre-packed foods(Reference Spence, Livingstone and Hollywood44). Contrarily, consumers with medical issues such as food allergies have reported problems with confusing and contradicting information on the food labels(Reference Cornelisse-Vermaat, Voordouw and Yiakoumaki136,Reference Hendriks, Frewer and van der Meulen137) , readability of the label(Reference Cornelisse-Vermaat, Voordouw and Yiakoumaki136), lack of harmonisation between the different countries, and the position of the labels in the food package(Reference Voordouw, Cornelisse-Vermaat and Yiakoumaki138,Reference Sheth, Waserman and Kagan139) . A literature review conducted by FSA and Food Standards Australia New Zealand highlighted the same issues(140). Men experience more difficulties in estimating appropriate PS(Reference Lewis, Ahern and Solis-Trapala141). However, other studies show that perceived healthiness of the food(Reference Faulkner, Pourshahidi and Wallace142), the ED(Reference Almiron-Roig and Solis-Trapala143) and the BMI(Reference Lewis, Forwood and Ahern144) may also contribute to shaping appropriate PS perceptions.

Different food labelling laws between countries add to consumers’ confusion. Clear information is provided in the consistent EU approach (EU Regulation No. 1169/2011) and retained regulations in the UK for the food labels on the pre-packed products(145); however, there is no harmonised approach for front-of-pack (FoP) labelling(146). A decade ago, the UK Government announced via a press release new consistency guidelines based partially on previous research(Reference Fisher, Liu and Birch115) stating that different FoP labels could hinder consumer understanding and discourage use(Reference Draper, Adamson and Clegg147). Although nutrition labels on pre-packaged food products are cost-effective at the population level, governments need to maximise their potential, by identifying new layouts and different types of information content to ensure that nutrition information is available and comprehensible for consumers’ benefit(148,Reference Campos, Doxey and Hammond149) . The food industry is pressured to reduce the PS and calories as part of the ongoing strategy to reduce childhood obesity(50). However, this leads to further issues, as consumers want value for money(Reference Rimmer150). Furthermore, a scoping review, including experimental and observational studies, indicated that food package labelling for serving size is unclear(Reference van der Horst, Bucher and Duncanson151). Specifically, consumers believe that the labelled serving size set by the manufacturer is the government recommendation(Reference van der Horst, Bucher and Duncanson151). Occasionally, the serving sizes are considered to be unrealistically small; resulting in consumers’ overestimating not only to the number of servings in one package but also the caloric content(Reference Young and Nestle19,Reference Bryant and Dundes34,Reference Marchiori, Corneille and Klein152) . Most consumers interpret the package size as a single serving size when it contains multiple servings(Reference van der Horst, Bucher and Duncanson151). Likewise, using terms such as small, medium and large leads to confusion in interpretation and differences(Reference Young and Nestle153). Regrettably, visual perception is not reliable in indicating the package size or the PS because of biases, which are highlighted in Table 3 (Reference Ordabayeva and Chandon133). Additionally, more emphasis towards dietary guidelines as well as the nutritional composition of the products sold may be warranted(Reference Culliford, Bradbury and Medici154). For instance, consumers widely know the number of PS of fruits and vegetables they need to consume every day. However, it remains unclear if they consider the consumption frequency and quantity of ultra-processed foods, such as confectionery and biscuits, when the EatWell Guide does not quantify how many portions of each food group should be consumed, unlike other food-based dietary guidelines(Reference Culliford, Bradbury and Medici154,Reference Miller, Eckberg and Decker155) .

Table 3. Shows the summary of package and portion size (PS) estimation biases

Ordabayeva N and Chandon P (2016) In the eye of the beholder: Visual biases in package and portion size perceptions. Appetite, 103, 450–457 [adapted]. Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Unit bias

The unit bias model suggests that one serving is appropriate to consume at once, irrespective of the size(Reference Geier, Rozin and Doros93). However, it is argued that segmentation bias is more applicable to use as individuals consume less food when it is divided into smaller units(Reference Kerameas, Vartanian and Herman156). A study supports that the anchoring effect works as a reference point for PS, as consumers eat a specific amount of the food. This means that PS acts as an influential anchor for determining the amount to consume, and succeeding adjustment processes do not negate the effect of that anchor(Reference Marchiori, Papies and Klein9).

The package size or the unit creates a ‘consumption norm’, operating for most individuals(Reference Zlatevska, Dubelaar and Holden10), which arguably is not appropriate according to the nation’s food recommendations(Reference Geier, Rozin and Doros93,Reference Wansink157) . The ‘clean your plate’ notion is encouraged, especially when the food is free; yet, in public settings, it promotes the bias to finish it, as the appropriateness mechanism is activated(Reference Birch and Fisher116). The PS sets a norm and dictates the amount consumed; therefore, PS and not hunger leads to food consumption(Reference Steenhuis and Poelman33,Reference Geier, Rozin and Doros93) . This is associated with human evolution, involving psychological and physiological mechanisms protecting against low adiposity(Reference Berthoud158,Reference Zheng and Berthoud159) . Consequently, consumers could be sensitive to detecting food inadequacy in terms of smaller than normal PS(Reference Haynes, Hardman and Halford160).

Tableware

The shape or size of tableware may influence the food selection and consumption(Reference Hollands, Shemilt and Marteau11,Reference Shah, Schroeder and Winn161) of a PS served in restaurants and in-home(Reference Diliberti, Bordi and Conklin82,Reference Robinson, Sheen and Harrold162) . The visual cue mechanism directs the PS intake, such as the plate emptiness degree, possibly activating meal termination(Reference Burger, Fisher and Johnson37). The rim width of the plate could influence the PS selection(Reference McClain, van den Bos and Matheson163). Thus, after choosing a large PS, passive overconsumption occurs (mindless eating), since individuals unintentionally prefer palatable and high-energy-dense foods(Reference Rolls100,Reference Blundell and Macdiarmid164) . Similar PS appear larger when served on a small plate (Delboeuf illusion), therefore controlling individuals’ judgements differently(Reference van Ittersum and Wansink165,Reference Penaforte, Japur and Diez-Garcia166) . If using larger bowls or plates, individuals tend to serve themselves more food, such as vegetables(Reference Wansink, van Ittersum and Painter167,Reference Libotte, Siegrist and Bucher168) , so the EI is not reduced(Reference Rolls, Roe and Halverson169,Reference Yip, Wiessing and Budgett170) . However, a meta-analysis of COT presented no dependable effect of tableware on food intake(Reference Robinson, Nolan and Tudur-Smith171).

COT propose that some drivers of PS selection remain unclear. However, their crucial role in body weight regulation remains evident. Food labels are difficult for consumers to interpret, leading to confusion. Well-conducted studies are required to provide solutions to consumers concerning appropriate PS(Reference Herman, Polivy and Pliner172Reference Hetherington, Blundell-Birtill and Caton174).

Conclusion

PS has increased significantly over recent decades worldwide, potentially associated with a significant rise in the prevalence of obesity. The available evidence demonstrates that the large PS of energy-dense foods may stimulate overconsumption, leading to high EI levels. However, a cause-and-effect relationship has yet to be shown, due to confounding factors. Nonetheless, studies have mostly taken place in lab environments for a short period in the USA. Although food labels assist consumers, frequently their interpretation is difficult. Studies show that consumers rely on external rather than internal PS cues to guide consumption, irrespective of satiety. Further research on food consumption patterns as well as consumer insights would help to inform the inclusion of clear and consistent information on PS into national dietary guidelines.

Acknowledgements

This work has been conducted as part of the MSc in Food Regulatory Affairs course at the University of Ulster and University College Dublin.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interests

None.

Authorship

This review was conducted as part of the dissertation module in the MSc Food Regulatory Affairs by Maria Papagiannaki who helped formulate the research question, conducted the literature search, interpreted the findings and wrote the review. Dr Maeve Kerr formulated the research question and provided guidance on the review. Dr Maeve Kerr has a track record of publications in the area of food portion size and obesity, having published nine articles in this area of research (four review articles; five original papers).

References

WHO (2021) Obesity and overweight factsheet. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/obesity-and-overweight (accessed February 2022).Google Scholar
Newton, JN, Briggs, ADM, Murray, CJL, et al. (2015) Changes in health in England, with analysis by English regions and areas of deprivation, 1990–2013: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. Lancet 386, P2257P2274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
NHS Digital (2019) Determining the worldwide prevalence of obesity. https://files.digital.nhs.uk/9D/4195D5/HSE19-Overweight-obesity-rep.pdf (accessed April 2023).Google Scholar
Reilly, JJ, El-Hamdouchi, A, Diouf, A, et al. (2018) Determining the worldwide prevalence of obesity. Lancet 391, 17731774.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logue, C, Doherty, L, McCann, M, et al. (2020) Portion control tools do they work in practice? https://www.safefood.net/getattachment/2fb6a9b9-4afa-457e-929d-79e2e2801e71/Portion-control-tools.pdf?lang=en-IE (accessed May 2023).Google Scholar
Myers, MG, Leibel, RL, Seeley, RJ, et al. (2010). Obesity and leptin resistance: distinguishing cause from effect. Trends Endocrinol Metabol 21, 643651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wansink, B & Kim, J (2005) Bad popcorn in big buckets. Portion size can influence intake as much as taste. J Nutr Educ Behav 37, 242245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingstone, MB & Pourshahidi, LK (2014) Portion size and obesity. Adv Nutr 5, 829834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchiori, D, Papies, EK & Klein, O (2014) The portion size effect on food intake. An anchoring and adjustment process? Appetite 81, 108115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zlatevska, N, Dubelaar, C & Holden, SS (2014) Sizing up the effect of portion size on consumption: a meta-analytic review. J Mark 78, 140154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollands, GJ, Shemilt, I, Marteau, TM, et al. (2015) Portion, package or tableware size for changing selection and consumption of food, alcohol and tobacco. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 14, CD011045.Google Scholar
Almiron-Roig, E, Navas-Carretero, S, Emery, P, et al. (2018) Research into food portion size: methodological aspects and applications. Food Funct 9, 715739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steenhuis, IHM, Leeuwis, FH & Vermeer, WM (2010) Small, medium, large or supersize: trends in food portion sizes in The Netherlands. Public Health Nutr 13, 852857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, SJ & Popkin, BM (2003) Patterns and trends in food portion sizes, 1977–1998. JAMA 289, 450453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Church, S (2008) Trends in portion size in the UK – a preliminary review of published information. London: Food Standards Agency.Google Scholar
Nielsen, SJ, Barry, M & Popkin, BM (2004) Changes in beverage intake between 1977 and 2001. Am J Prev Med 27, 205210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wrieden, W, Gregor, A & Barton, K (2008) Have food portion sizes increased in the UK over the last 20 years? Proc Nutr Soc 67, E211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benson, C (2009) Increasing portion size in Britain. Soc Biol Hum Aff 74, 420.Google Scholar
Young, LR & Nestle, M (2002) The contribution of expanding portion sizes to the US obesity epidemic. Am J Public Health 92, 246249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smiciklas-Wright, H, Mitchell, DC, Mickle, SJ, et al. (2003) Foods commonly eaten in the United States, 1989–1991 and 1994–1996: are portion sizes changing? J Am Diet Assoc 103, 4147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthiessen, J, Fagt, S, Biltoft-Jensen, A, et al. (2003) Size makes a difference. Public Health Nutr 6, 6572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eidner, EM, Qvistgaard Lund, AS, Schroll Harboe, B et al. (2013) Calories and portion sizes in recipes throughout 100 years: an overlooked factor in the development of overweight and obesity? Scand J Public Health 41, 839845.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P, Kabnick, K, Pete, E, et al. (2003) The ecology of eating: smaller portion sizes in France than in the United States help explain the French paradox. Psychol Sci 14, 450454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, LR & Nestle, M (2003) Expanding portion sizes in the US marketplace. Implications for nutritional counselling. J Am Diet Assoc 103, 231234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
English, L, Lasschuijt, M & Keller, KL (2015) Mechanisms of the portion size effect. What is known and where do we go from here? Appetite 88, 3949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rolls, BJ, Roe, LS, Kral, TV, et al. (2004) Salad and satiety: energy density and portion size of a first-course salad affect energy intake at lunch. J Am Diet Assoc 104, 15701576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, BJ, Morris, EL & Roe, LS (2002) Portion size of food affects energy intake in normal-weight and overweight men and women. Am J Clin Nutr 76, 12071213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wansink, B & Park, SB (2001) At the movies: how external cues and perceived taste impact consumption volume. Food Qual Prefer 12, 6974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flood, JE, Roe, LS & Rolls, BJ (2006) The effect of increased beverage portion size on energy intake at a meal. J Am Diet Assoc 106, 19841991.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, LR & Nestle, M (2012) Reducing portion sizes to prevent obesity. A call to action. Am J Prev Med 43, 565568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pourshahidi, LK, Kerr, MA, McCaffrey, TA, et al. (2014) Influencing and modifying children’s energy intake: the role of portion size and energy density. Proc Nutr Soc 73, 397406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steenhuis, IH & Vermeer, WM (2009) Portion size: review and framework for interventions. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 6, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steenhuis, IH & Poelman, M (2017) Portion size: latest developments and interventions. Curr Obes Rep 6, 1017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, R & Dundes, L (2005) Portion distortion: a study of college students. J Cons Aff 39, 399408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, J & Byrd-Bredbenner, C (2006) Portion distortion. Typical portion sizes selected by young adults. J Am Diet Assoc 106, 14121418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Condrasky, M, Ledikwe, JH, Flood, JE, et al. (2007) Chefs’ opinions of restaurant portion sizes. Obesity 15, 20862094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burger, KS, Fisher, JO & Johnson, SL (2011) Mechanisms behind the portion size effect: visibility and bite size. Obesity 19, 546551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Almiron-Roig, E, Tsiountsioura, M, Lewis, HB, et al. (2015) Large portion sizes increase bite size and eating rate in overweight women. Physiol Behav 139, 297302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, JO, Rolls, BJ & Birch, LL (2003) Children’s bite size and intake of an entrée are greater with large portions than with age-appropriate or self-selected portions. Am J Clin Nutr 77, 11641170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, JO (2007) Effects of age on children’s intake of large and self-selected food portions. Obesity 15, 403412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faulkner, GP, Pourshahidi, LK, Wallace, JMW, et al. (2012) Serving size guidance for consumers: is it effective? Proc Nutr Soc 71, 610621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poelman, M, Eyles, H, Dunford, E, et al. (2015) Package size and manufacturer-recommended serving size of sweet beverages: a cross-sectional study across four high-income countries. Public Health Nutr 19, 10081016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rippin, HL, Hutchinson, J, Jewell, J, et al. (2019) Comparison of consumed portion sizes and on-pack serving sizes of UK energy dense foods. Appetite 134, 193203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spence, M., Livingstone, MBE, Hollywood, LE, et al. (2013) A qualitative study of psychological, social and behavioral barriers to appropriate food portion size control. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 10, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levitsky, LA & Youn, T (2004) The more food young adults are served, the more they overeat, J Nutr 134, 25462549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wansink, B & van Ittersum, K (2007) Portion size me: downsizing our consumption norms. J Am Diet Assoc 107, 11031106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cutler, DM, Glaeser, EL & Shapiro, JM (2003) Why have Americans become more obese? J Econ Persp 17, 93118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wansink, B (2010) From mindless eating to mindlessly eating better. Physiol Behav 100, 454463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Department of Health (2011) Policy Paper: Calorie reduction. https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/215561/dh_127554.pdf (accessed February 2022).Google Scholar
PHE (2018) Calorie reduction: The scope and ambition for action. London, UK: PHE.Google Scholar
Department of Health (2018) The Eatwell Guide. https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/the-eatwell-guide (accessed March 2022).Google Scholar
Lewis, HB, Ahern, AL & Jebb, SA (2012) How much should I eat? A comparison of suggested portion sizes in the UK. Public Health Nutr 15, 21002117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Church, S (2008) Trends in portion sizes in the UK. Summary of pack and serving sizes tables. http://toolbox.foodcomp.info/References/RecipeCalculation/Susan%20Church%20%20-%20%20Trends%20in%20portion%20sizes%20in%20the%20UK%20-%20Tables.pdf (accessed March 2023).Google Scholar
Wrieden, DL, Barton, KL, Cochrane, L, et al. (2004) Final technical report to the food standards agency: Calculation and collation of typical food portion sizes for adults aged 19–64 and older people aged 65 and over. London: Food Standards Agency.Google Scholar
Fennel, R (1997) The common agricultural policy: Continuity and change. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davey, RC (2004) The obesity epidemic: too much food for thought? Br J Sports Med 38, 360364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vermeer, WM, Steenhuis, IH & Seidell, JC (2010) Portion size: a qualitative study of consumers’ attitudes toward point-of-purchase interventions aimed at portion size. Health Educ Res 25, 109120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Brien, SA, Livingstone, MB, McNulty, BA, et al. (2015) Secular trends in reported portion size of food and beverages consumed by Irish adults. Br J Nutr 113, 11481157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenheck, R (2008) Fast food consumption and increased caloric intake: a systematic review of a trajectory towards weight gain and obesity risk. Obes Rev 9, 535547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Binkley, JK, Eales, J & Jekanowski, M (2000) The relation between dietary change and rising US obesity. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 24, 10321039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paeratakul, S, Ferdinand, DP, Champagne, CM, et al. (2003) Fast-food consumption among US adults and children. Dietary and nutrient intake profile. J Am Diet Assoc 103, 13321338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowman, SA & Vinyard, BT (2004) Fast food consumption of U.S. adults: impact on energy and nutrient intakes and overweight status. J Am Coll Nutr 23, 163168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kant, AK & Graubard, BI (2004) Eating out in America, 1987–2000. Trends and nutritional correlates. Am J Prev Med 38, 243249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marteau, TM, Hollands, GJ, Shemilt, I, et al. (2015) Downsizing: policy options to reduce portion sizes to help tackle obesity. BMJ 351, 15.Google ScholarPubMed
Orfanos, P, Naska, A, Trichopoulos, D, et al. (2007) Eating out of home and its correlates in 10 European countries. The European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition (EPIC) study. Public Health Nutr 10, 15151525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marin-Guerrero, AC, Gutierrez-Fisac, JL, Guallar-Castillon, P, et al. (2008) Eating behaviours and obesity in the adult population of Spain. Br J Nutr 100, 11421148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bezerra, IN & Sichieri, R (2009) Eating out of home and obesity: a Brazilian nationwide survey. Public Health Nutr 12, 20372043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naska, A, Orfanos, P, Trichopoulou, A, et al. (2011) Eating out, weight and weight gain. A cross-sectional and prospective analysis in the context of the EPIC-PANACEA study. Int J Obes 35, 416426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunstrom, JM (2011) Symposium on ‘Nutrition: getting the balance right in 2010’. Session 1: Balancing intake and output: food v. exercise. The control of meal size in human subjects: a role for expected satiety, expected satiation and premeal planning. Proc Nutr Soc 70, 155161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kantar (2020) Kantar reveals world’s 2020 pandemic shopping habits. https://cdne.kantar.com/en-cn/inspiration/fmcg/kantar-reveals-worlds-2020-pandemic-shopping-habits (accessed October 2023).Google Scholar
Bennett, G, Young, E, Butler, I, et al. (2021). The impact of lockdown during the COVID-19 outbreak on dietary habits in various population groups: a scoping review. Front Nutr 8, 626432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lasko-Skinner, R & Sweetland, J (2021) Food in a pandemic. https://www.food.gov.uk/sites/default/files/media/document/fsa-food-in-a-pandemic-march-2021.pdf (accessed November 2023).Google Scholar
Scott, L & Ensaff, H (2022) COVID-19 and the National Lockdown: how food choice and dietary habits changed for families in the United Kingdom. Front Nutr 9, 847547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
European Institute of Innovation and Technology (2020) COVID-19 impact on consumer food behaviours in Europe. https://www.eitfood.eu/media/news-pdf/COVID-19_Study_-_European_Food_Behaviours_-_Report.pdf (accessed November 2023).Google Scholar
Barons, MJ & Aspinall, W (2020) Anticipated impacts of Brexit scenarios on UK food prices and implications for policies on poverty and health: a structured expert judgement approach. BMJ 10, e032376.Google Scholar
Ranta, R & Mulrooney, H (2021) Pandemics, food (in)security, and leaving the EU: what does the Covid-19 pandemic tell us about food insecurity and Brexit. Soc Sci Hum Open 3, 15.Google Scholar
Rivington, M, King, R, Duckett, D, et al. (2021) UK food and nutrition security during and after the COVID-19 pandemic. Nutr Bull 46, 8897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ello-Martin, JA, Ledikwe, JH & Rolls, BJ (2005) The influence of food portion size and energy density on energy intake: implications for weight management. Am J Clin Nutr 82, Suppl. 1, 236S241S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zlatevska, N, Dubelaar, C & Holden, SS (2012) Increasing serving size increases the amount consumed. Catch-22. Adv Cons Res 40, 843844.Google Scholar
Diliberti, N, Bordi, PL, Conklin, MT, et al. (2004) Increased portion size leads to increased energy intake in a restaurant meal. Obes Res 12, 562568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kral, TVE, Roe, LS & Rolls, BJ (2004) Combined effects of energy density and portion size on energy intake in women. Am J Clin Nutr 79, 962968.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wansink, B, Painter, JE & North, J (2005) Bottomless bowls: why visual cues of portion size may influence intake. Obes Res 13, 93100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermans, RCJ, Larsen, JK, Herman, CP, et al. (2011) How much should I eat? Situational norms affect young women’s food intake during meal time. Br J Nutr 107, 588594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, BJ, Roe, LS & Meengs, JS (2006) Larger portion sizes lead to a sustained increase in energy intake over 2 days. J Am Diet Assoc 106, 543549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeffery, RW, Rydell, S, Dunn, CL, et al. (2007) Effects of portion size on chronic energy intake. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 4, 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, SA, Mitchell, NR, Wolfson, J, et al. (2014) Portion size effects on weight gain in a free-living setting. Obesity (Silver Spring) 22, 14001405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Q, Tam, LY & Rangan, A (2022) The effect of downsizing packages of energy-dense, nutrient-poor snacks and drinks on consumption, intentions, and perceptions – a scoping review. Nutrients 14, 939.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, E, McFarland-Lesser, I, Patel, Z, et al. (2023) Downsizing food: a systematic review and meta-analysis examining the effect of reducing served food portion sizes on daily energy intake and body weight. Br J Nutr 129, 888903.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vandenbroele, J, Van Kerckhove, A & Zlatevska, N (2019) Portion size effects vary: the size of food units is a bigger problem than the number. Appetite 140, 2740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, BJ, Roe, LS, Meengs, JS, et al. (2004) Increasing the portion size of a sandwich increases energy intake. J Am Diet Assoc 104, 367372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geier, AB, Rozin, P & Doros, G (2006) Unit bias. A new heuristic that helps explain the effect of portion size on food intake. Psychol Sci 17, 521525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, KA, Hudson, JL, Hayes, A, et al. (2022) Systematic review and meta-analysis on the effect of portion size and ingestive frequency on energy intake and body weight among adults in randomized controlled feeding trials. Adv Nutr 13, 248268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D (2003) A perspective in judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. Am Psychol 58, 697720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dhar, R & Gorlin, M (2013) A dual-system framework to understand preference construction processes in choice. J Cons Psychol 23, 528554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rolls, BJ, Roe, LS & Meengs, JS (2007) The effect of large portion sizes on energy intake is sustained for 11 days. Obesity (Silver Spring) 15, 15351543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, BJ, Roe, LS, Kral, TV, et al. (2004) Increasing the portion size of a packaged snack increases energy intake in men and women. Appetite 42, 6369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, MT, Wallace, JM, Robson, PJ, et al. (2009) Increased portion size leads to a sustained increase in energy intake over 4 d in normal-weight and overweight men and women. Br J Nutr 102, 470477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, BJ (2009) The relationship between dietary energy density and energy intake. Phys Behav 97, 609615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, D (2015) Portion size: what we know and what we need to know. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr 55, 9881004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gough, T, Haynes, A, Clarke, K, et al. (2021) Out of the lab and into the wild: the influence of portion size on food intake in laboratory vs. real-world settings. Appetite 162, 105160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hetherington, MM & Blundell-Birtill, P (2018) The portion size effect and overconsumption-towards downsizing solutions for children and adolescents. Nutr Bull 43, 6168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Acolatse, L, Poursahidi, LK, Logue, C, et al. (2023) Child food portion sizes in the home environment: how do parents decide? Proc Nutr Soc 82, 386393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoevenaars, FPM, Berendsen, CMM, Pasman, WJ, et al. (2020) Evaluation of food-intake behavior in a healthy population: personalized vs. one-size-fits-all. Nutrients 12, 2819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McConahy, KL, Smiciklas-Wright, H, Birch, LL, et al. (2002) Food portions are positively related to energy intake and body weight in early childhood. J Pediatr 140, 340347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McConahy, KL, Smiciklas-Wright, H, Mitchell, DC, et al. (2004) Portion size of common foods predicts energy intake among preschool-aged children. J Am Diet Assoc 104, 975979.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerr, M, Rennie, K, McCaffrey, T, et al. (2009) Snacking patterns among adolescents: a comparison of type, frequency and portion size between Britain in 1997 and Northern Ireland in 2005. Br J Nutr 101, 122131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, TT, Howarth, NC, Lin, BH, et al. (2004) Energy intake and meal portions: associations with BMI percentile in U.S. children. Obes Res 12, 18751885.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mrdjenovic, G & Levitsky, DA (2005) Children eat what they are served: the imprecise regulation of energy intake. Appetite 44, 273282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, MK, Devaney, B & Reidy, K (2006) Relationship between portion size and energy intake among infants and toddlers: evidence of self-regulation. J Am Diet Assoc 106, Suppl.1, S77S83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
USDA (2010) Dietary Guidelines for Americans 2010. https://www.dietaryguidelines.gov/sites/default/files/2019-05/DietaryGuidelines2010.pdf (accessed April 2022).Google Scholar
Rolls, BJ, Engell, D & Birch, LL (2000) Serving portion size influences 5-year-old but not 3-year-old children’s food intakes. J Am Diet Assoc 100, 232234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, JO, Arreola, A, Birch, LL, et al. (2007). Portion size effects on daily energy intake in low-income Hispanic and African American children and their mothers. Am J Clin Nutr 86, 17091716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, JO, Liu, Y, Birch, L, et al. (2007). Effects of portion size and energy density on young children’s intake at a meal. Am J Clin Nutr 86, 174179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birch, LL & Fisher, JO (1998) Development of eating behaviors among children and adolescents. Pediatrics 101, Suppl. 2, 539549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boulos, R, Vikre, EK, Oppenheimer, S, et al. (2012) ObesiTV: how television is influencing the obesity epidemic. Physiol Behav 107, 146153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, L, Conroy, K, Wen, H, et al. (2013) Portion size variably affects food intake of 6-year-old and 4-year-old children in Kunming, China. Appetite 69, 3138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, JS, Fisher, JO, Marini, M, et al. (2012) Serving smaller age-appropriate entrée portions to children aged 3–5 y increases fruit and vegetable intake and reduces energy density and energy intake at lunch. Am J Clin Nutr 95, 335341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leahy, KE, Birch, LL, Fisher, JO, et al. (2008) Reductions in entrée energy density increase children’s vegetable intake and reduce energy intake. Obesity (Silver Spring) 16, 15591565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, JS, Hinton, EC, Sauchelli, S, et al. (2021) When do children learn how to select a portion size? Appetite 164, 105247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leahy, KE, Birch, LL & Rolls, BJ (2008) Reducing the energy density of multiple meals decreases the energy intake of preschool-age children. Am J Clin Nutr 88, 14591468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leahy, KE, Birch, LL & Rolls, BJ (2008) Reducing the energy density of an entrée decreases children’s energy intake at lunch. J Am Diet Assoc 108, 4148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Looney, SM & Raynor, HA (2011) Impact of portion size and energy density on snack intake in preschool-aged children. J Am Diet Assoc 111, 414418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, LH, Paluch, RA, Beecher, MD, et al. (2008) Increasing healthy eating vs. reducing high energy-dense foods to treat pediatric obesity. Obesity (Silver Spring) 16, 318326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Small, L, Lane, H, Vaughan, L, et al. (2013) A systematic review of the evidence: the effects of portion size manipulation with children and portion education/training interventions on dietary intake with adults. Worldviews Evid Based Nurs 10, 6981.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hetherington, MM (2019) The portion size effect and overconsumption- towards downsizing solutions for children and adolescents – an update. Nutr Bull 44, 130137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage, JS, Haisfield, L, Fisher, JO, et al. (2012). Do children eat less at meals when allowed to serve themselves? Am J Clin Nutr 96, 3643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
European Union (2011) Regulation (EU) No 1169/2011 of the European Parliament and of the Council of 25 October 2011 on the provision of food information to consumers, amending Regulations (EC) No 1924/2006 and (EC) No 1925/2006 of the European Parliament and of the Council, and repealing Commission Directive 87/250/EEC, Council Directive 90/496/EEC, Commission Directive 1999/10/EC, Directive 2000/13/EC of the European Parliament and of the Council, Commission Directives 2002/67/EC and 2008/5/EC and Commission Regulation (EC) No 608/2004 Text with EEA relevance. https://eur-lex.europa.eu/legal-content/EN/TXT/PDF/?uri=CELEX:32011R1169&from=EN (accessed April 2023).Google Scholar
FDA (2018) Food Labeling: Serving Sizes of Foods That Can Reasonably Be Consumed at One Eating Occasion; Dual-Column Labeling; Updating, Modifying, and Establishing Certain Reference Amounts Customarily Consumed; Serving Size for Breath Mints; and Technical Amendments: Guidance for Industry Small Entity Compliance Guide. ∼https://www.fda.gov/media/111144/download#:∼:text=For%20all%20products%2C%20except%20beverages,)(5)(ii)) (accessed April 2023).Google Scholar
Faulkner, GP, Livingstone, MBE, Pourshahidi, LK, et al. (2017) An evaluation of portion size estimation aids: consumer perspectives on their effectiveness. Appetite 114, 200208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghvanidze, S, Velikova, N, Dodd, T, et al. (2017) A discrete choice experiment of the impact of consumers’ environmental values, ethical concerns, and health consciousness on food choices: a cross-cultural analysis”. Br Food J 119, 863881.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ordabayeva, N & Chandon, P (2016) In the eye of the beholder: visual biases in package and portion size perceptions. Appetite 103, 450457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lennard, D, Mitchell, VW, McGoldrick, P, et al. (2001) Why consumers under-use food quantity indicators. Inter Rev Retail Distrib Cons Res 11, 177199.Google Scholar
Viswanathan, M, Rosa, JA & Harris, JE (2005) Decision making and coping of functionally illiterate consumers and some implications for marketing management. J Mark 69, 1531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornelisse-Vermaat, JR, Voordouw, J, Yiakoumaki, V, et al. (2008) Food-allergic consumers’ labelling preferences: a cross-cultural comparison. Eur J Public Health 18, 115120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hendriks, MJ, Frewer, LJ & van der Meulen, BMJ (2011) Allergens in Law: European Legislation assessed against the preferences of food allergic consumers. Eur Food Feed Law Rev 6, 7487.Google Scholar
Voordouw, J, Cornelisse-Vermaat, JR, Yiakoumaki, V, et al. (2009) Food allergic consumers’ preferences for labelling practices: a qualitative study in a real shopping environment. Int J Consum Stud 33, 94102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheth, SS, Waserman, S, Kagan, R, et al. (2010) Role of food labels in accidental exposures in food-allergic individuals in Canada. Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol 104, 6065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
FSA & FSANZ (2022) Consumers and allergen labelling: A literature review of consumer response to allergen declarations and precautionary allergen labelling. https://www.food.gov.uk/sites/default/files/media/document/fsa-and-fsanz-consumers-and-allergen-labelling-literature-review-of-consumer-response-to-allergen-declarations-and-precautionary-allergen-labelling-revised.pdf (accessed November 2023).Google Scholar
Lewis, HB, Ahern, AL, Solis-Trapala, I, et al. (2015) Effect of reducing portion size at a compulsory meal on later energy intake, gut hormones, and appetite in overweight adults. Obesity (Silver Spring) 23, 13621370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faulkner, GP, Pourshahidi, LK, Wallace, JMW, et al. (2014) Perceived “healthiness” of foods can influence consumers’ estimations of energy density and appropriate portion size. Int J Obes 38, 106112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Almiron-Roig, E, Solis-Trapala, I, et al. (2013) Estimating food portions. Influence of unit number, meal-type, and energy density. Appetite 71, 95103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, HB, Forwood, S, Ahern, A, et al. (2015) Personal and social norms for food portion sizes in lean and obese adults. Int J Obes 39, 13191324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
European Commission (2020) Report from the Commission to the European Parliament and the Council regarding the use of additional forms of expression and presentation of the nutrition declaration; COM(2020) 207 final. https://op.europa.eu/en/publication-detail/-/publication/4f4f416e-9a95-11ea-9d2d-01aa75ed71a1/language-en/format-PDF/source-273336974 (accessed October 2022).Google Scholar
Draper, AK, Adamson, AJ, Clegg, S et al. (2013) Front-of-pack nutrition labelling: are multiple formats a problem for consumers? Eur J Public Health 23, 517521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Department of Health (2013) Final design of consistent nutritional labelling system given green light. https://www.gov.uk/government/news/final-design-of-consistent-nutritional-labelling-system-given-green-light (accessed October 2023).Google Scholar
Campos, S, Doxey, J & Hammond, D. (2011) Nutrition labels on pre-packaged foods: a systematic review. Public Health Nutr 14, 14961506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rimmer, A (2018) Industry must cut calories in savoury food products by 20%, says Public Health England. BMJ 360, 382.Google ScholarPubMed
van der Horst, K, Bucher, T, Duncanson, K, et al. (2019) Consumer understanding, perception and interpretation of serving size information on food labels: a scoping review. Nutrients 11, 21892209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchiori, D, Corneille, O & Klein, O (2012) Container size influences snack food intake independently of portion size. Appetite 58, 814871.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, LR & Nestle, M (1998) Variation in perceptions of a medium’ food portion: implications for dietary guidance. J Am Diet Assoc 98, 458459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Culliford, AE, Bradbury, J & Medici, EB (2023) Improving communication of the UK sustainable healthy dietary guidelines the Eatwell Guide: a rapid review. Sustainability 15, 6149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, KB, Eckberg, JO, Decker, JO, et al. (2021) Role of food industry in promoting healthy and sustainable diets. Nutrients 13, 2740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerameas, K, Vartanian, LR, Herman, CP, et al. (2015) The effect of portion size and unit size on food intake: Unit bias or segmentation effect? Health Psychol 34, 670676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wansink, B (2004) Environmental factors that increase the food intake and consumption volume of unknowing consumers. Annu Rev Nutr 24, 455479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berthoud, HR (2004) Mind versus metabolism in the control of food intake and energy balance. Physiol Behav 81, 781793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zheng, H & Berthoud, HR (2008) Neural systems controlling the drive to eat: mind versus metabolism. Physiology 23, 7583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haynes, A, Hardman, CA, Halford, JCG, et al. (2020). Portion size normality and additional within-meal food intake: two crossover laboratory experiments. Br J Nutr 123, 462471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shah, M, Schroeder, R, Winn, W, et al. (2011). A pilot study to investigate the effect of plate size on meal energy intake in normal weight and overweight/obese women. J Hum Nutr Diet 24, 612615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, E, Sheen, F, Harrold, J, et al. (2016) Dishware size and snack food intake in a between-subjects laboratory experiment. Public Health Nutr 19, 633637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClain, AD, van den Bos, W, Matheson, D, et al. (2013) Visual illusions and plate design: the effects of plate rim widths and rim coloring on perceived food portion size. Int J Obes 38, 16.Google ScholarPubMed
Blundell, JE & Macdiarmid, JI (1997) Fat as a risk factor for overconsumption: satiation, satiety, and patterns of eating. J Am Diet Assoc 97, Suppl.7, S63S69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Ittersum, K & Wansink, B (2012) Plate size and color suggestibility: the Delboeuf illusion’s bias on serving and eating behavior. J Cons Res 39, 215318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penaforte, FRO, Japur, CC, Diez-Garcia, RW, et al. (2014) Plate size does not affect perception of food portion size. J Hum Nutr Diet 27, Suppl. 2, 214219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wansink, B, van Ittersum, K & Painter, JE (2006) Ice cream illusions. Bowls, spoons, and self-served portion sizes. Am J Prev Med 31, 240243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Libotte, E, Siegrist, M & Bucher, T (2014) The influence of plate size on meal composition. Literature review and experiment. Appetite 82, 9196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, BJ, Roe, LS, Halverson, KH, et al. (2007). Using a smaller plate did not reduce energy intake at meals. Appetite 49, 652660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yip, W, Wiessing, KR, Budgett, S, et al. (2013). Using a smaller dining plate does not suppress food intake from a buffet lunch meal in overweight, unrestrained women. Appetite 69, 102107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, E, Nolan, S, Tudur-Smith, C, et al. (2014). Will smaller plates lead to smaller waists? A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effect that experimental manipulation of dishware size has on energy consumption. Obes Rev 15, 812821.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herman, CP, Polivy, J, Pliner, P, et al. (2015) Mechanisms underlying the portion-size effect. Physiol Behav 144, 129136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raynor, HA & Champagne, CM (2016) Position of the academy of nutrition and dietetics: interventions for the treatment of overweight and obesity in adults. J Acad Nutr Diet 116, 129147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hetherington, MM, Blundell-Birtill, P, Caton, SJ, et al. (2018) Understanding the science of portion control and the art of downsizing. Proc Nutr Soc 77, 347355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, EA, Castellanos, VH, Pelkman, CL, et al. (1998) Energy density of foods affects energy intake in normal-weight women. Am J Clin Nutr 67, 412420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raynor, HA, & Wing, RR (2007) Package unit and amount of food: do both influence intake? Obesity (Silver Spring) 15, 23112319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stroebele, N, Ogden, LG & Hill, JO (2009). Do calorie-controlled portion sizes of snacks reduce energy intake? Appetite 52, 793796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchiori, D, Waroquier, L & Klein, O. (2011). Smaller food item sizes of snack foods influence reduced portions and caloric intake in young adults. J Am Diet Assoc 111, 727731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haynes, A, Hardman, CA, Halford, JCG, et al. (2020). Reductions to main meal portion sizes reduce daily energy intake regardless of perceived normality of portion size: a 5 day cross-over laboratory experiment. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 17, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spill, MK, Birch, LL, Roe, LS, et al. (2010) Eating vegetables first: the use of portion size to increase vegetable intake in preschool children. Am J Clin Nutr 91, 12371243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smethers, AD, Roe, LS, Sanchez, CE, et al. (2019). Portion size has sustained effects over 5 days in preschool children: a randomized trial. Am J Clin Nutr 109, 13611372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balcetis, E & Dunning, D (2010) Wishful seeing: more desired objects are seen as closer. Psycho Sci 21, 147152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Koningsbruggen, GM, Stroebe, W & Aarts, H (2011) Through the eyes of dieters: biased size perception of food following tempting food primes. J Exp Soc Psychol 47, 293299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornil, Y, Ordabayeva, N, Kaiser, U, et al. (2014). The acuity of vice: attitude ambivalence improves visual sensitivity to increasing portion sizes. Cons Psychol 24, 177187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chandon, P & Ordabayeva, N (2009) Supersize in one dimension, downsize in three dimensions: effects of spatial dimensionality on size perceptions and preferences. J Mark Res 46, 725738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ordabayeva, N & Chandon, P (2013) Predicting and managing consumers’ package size impressions. J Mark 77, 123137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wansink, B & Chandon, P (2006) Can ‘low-fat’ nutrition labels lead to obesity? J Mark Res 43, 605617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chandon, P & Wansink, B. (2007) Is obesity caused by calorie underestimation? A psychophysical model of fast-food meal size estimation. J Mark Res 44, 8499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chandon, P & Wansink, B (2007) The biasing health halos of fast food restaurant health claims: lower calorie estimates and higher side-dish consumption intentions. J Cons Res 34, 301314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deng, X & Kahn, BE (2009) Is your product on the right side? The “location effect” on perceived product heaviness and package evaluation. J Mark Res 46, 725738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chernev, A & Gal, D (2010) Categorization effects in value judgments: averaging bias in evaluating combinations of vices and virtues. J Mark Res 47, 738747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aydınoglu, N & Krishna, A (2011) Guiltless gluttony: the asymmetric effect of size labels on size perceptions and consumption. Consum Res 37, 10951112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krider, RE, Raghubir, P & Krishna, A (2001) Pizzas: pi or square? Psychophysical biases in area comparisons. Mark Sci 20, 405425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chandon, P & Wansink, B (2006) How biased household inventory estimates distort shopping and storage decisions. J Mark 70, 118135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornil, Y & Chandon, P (2015) Pleasure as a substitute for size: how multisensory imagery can make people happier with smaller food portions. J Mark Res 53, 847864.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornil, Y & Chandon, P (2015) Pleasure as an ally of healthy eating? Contrasting visceral and Epicurean eating pleasure and their association with portion size preferences and wellbeing. Appetite 104, 5259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Shows studies researching the relationship between portion sizes (PS) and energy intake (EI) and/or energy density (ED) in adults

Figure 1

Table 2. Shows studies researching the relationship between portion sizes (PS) and energy intake (EI) and/or energy density (ED) in children

Figure 2

Fig. 1. The underlying factors causing portion distortion.Steenhuis IH and Vermeer WM (2009) Portion size: review and framework for interventions. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act, 6, 1–10 [adapted]. Copyright © 2009, Steenhuis and Vermeer; licensee BioMed Central Ltd.

Figure 3

Table 3. Shows the summary of package and portion size (PS) estimation biases