Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T13:11:20.096Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mood congruence effect in explicit and implicit memory tasks: a comparison between depressed patients, schizophrenic patients and controls

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 April 2020

N Bazin*
Affiliation:
CHG, Lagny-sur-Marne
P Perruchet
Affiliation:
CNRS, Université de Bourgogne, Bourgogne
A Féline
Affiliation:
CHU Bicêtre, Le Kremlin-Bicêtre, France
*
*Correspondence and reprints: Dr N Bazin, Centre Hospitalier Général de Lagny-sur-Marne, 77405 Lagny-sur-Marne cedex, France.
Get access

Summary

This study investigates mood congruence effect in explicit and implicit memory tasks in 23 inpatients fulfilling DSM-III-R criteria for major depressive disorder. Performances were compared to those of 15 in- or outpatients fulfilling DSM-III-R criteria for schizophrenia, and 37 normal subjects serving as euthymics controls. All subjects were submitted to a standard cued recall test and to a word stem completion test devised to assess the effect of the initial presentation without the explicit retrieval of the words being necessary. The material used for these two tasks consisted of emotionally negative and positive words. The results show a mood congruence effect in the implicit memory task (and not in the explicit memory task) only in patients who had recovered from their major depressive episode (and not in depressed patients, schizophrenic patients, or controls). These results suggest that implicit and explicit emotional information processing differ from one another in certain respects.

Type
Original articles
Copyright
Copyright © Elsevier, Paris 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bazin, N. Mémoire, état émotionnel et valences affectives: Revue critique. Encéphale 1991; 17: 503510Google Scholar
Bazin, NPerruchet, PDe, Bonis MFéline, A. The dissociation of explicit and implicit memory in depressed patients Psychol Med 1994 24 239245CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boucher, JOsgood, CEThe Pollyanna hypothesis New York: Academic Press Inc, 1969CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bower, GH. Mood and memory. Am Psychol 1981; 36: 129148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslow, RKocsis, JBelkin, B. Contribution of the depressive perspective to memory function in depression. Am J Psychiatry 1981; 138: 227230Google ScholarPubMed
Burt, DBZembar, MJNiederehe, G. Depression and memory impairment: A meta-analysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychol Bull 1995; 117: 285305CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, MKirsner, K. Context and repetition effects in lexical decision and recognition memory. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 1982; 21: 5569CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clare, LMc, Kenna PJMortimer, AMBaddeley, AD. Memory in schizophrenia: what is impaired preserved?. Neuropsychologia 1993; 31: 12251241CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danion, JMWillard-Schroeder, DZimmermann, MAGrangé, DSchlienger, JLSinger, L. Explicit priming and repetition priming in depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48: 707711CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denny, EBHunt, R. Affective valence and memory in depression: dissociation of recall and fragment completion. J Abnorm Psychol 1992; 101: 575580CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dowal, JM. Recall of pleasant and unpleasant words in depressed subject. J Abnorm Psychol 1984; 93: 401407Google Scholar
Dunbar, GCLishman, WA. Depression, recognition-memory and hedonic tone: a signal detection analysis. Br J Psychiatry 1984; 144: 376382CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graff, PSchacter, DL. Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. J Exp Psychol Learn Memory Cogn 1985; 11: 501518CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, TETorrey, EFGold, JMRagland, JDBigelow, LBWeinberger, DR. Learning and memory in monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia. Psychol Med 1993; 23: 7185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heinrichs, RWAwad, AG. Neurocognitive subtypes of chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1993; 9: 4958CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacoby, LLDallas, M. On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. J Exp Psychol Gen 1981; 110: 306340CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacoby, LLWitherspoon, D. Remembering without awareness. Can J Psychol 1982; 36: 300324CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martins, D. Influence des états émotionnels dans les activités de mémorisation, de rappel, d'identification et de production de matériels verbaux. Ann Psychol 1985; 185: 577597CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perruchet, PBaveux, C. Correlational analysis of explicit and implicit memory performance. Memory Cognition 1989; 17: 7786CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Randolph, CGold, JMCarpenter, CJGoldberg, TEWeinberger, DR. Implicit memory in patients with schizophrenia and normal controls: Effects of task demands on susceptibility to priming. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1993; 15: 853866CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roediger, HLMcDermott, KB. Depression and implicit memory: a commentary. J Abnorm Psychol 1992; 101: 587591CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmand, BKop, WJKuipers, TBosveld, J. Implicit memory in psychotic patients. Schizophr Res 1992; 7: 5564CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watkins, PCMathews, AWilliamson, DAFuller, RD. Moodcongruent memory in depression: emotional priming or elaboration?. J Abnorm Psychol 1992; 101: 581586CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.