Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T11:29:23.020Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Altered syntactic abilities in first episode patients: An inner phenomenon characterizing psychosis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2020

Giuseppe Delvecchio
Affiliation:
aUniversity of Milan, Department of Pathophysiology and Transplantation, Milan, Italy
Elisabetta Caletti
Affiliation:
bFondazione IRCCS Ca’ Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Department of Neurosciences and Mental Health, Milan, Italy
Cinzia Perlini
Affiliation:
cDepartment of Neurosciences, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, Section of Clinical Psychology, University of Verona, Italy
Francesca Marzia Siri
Affiliation:
bFondazione IRCCS Ca’ Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Department of Neurosciences and Mental Health, Milan, Italy
Angela Andreella
Affiliation:
dDepartment of Statistical Sciences, University of Padua, Italy
Livio Finos
Affiliation:
eDepartment of Developmental Psychology and Socialization, University of Padua, Italy
Marcella Bellani
Affiliation:
fUOC of Psychiatry, Azienda Ospedaliera Universitaria Integrata (AOUI) of Verona, Italy
Franco Fabbro
Affiliation:
gDepartment of Medicine, University of Udine, Udine, Italy
Antonio Lasalvia
Affiliation:
cDepartment of Neurosciences, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, Section of Clinical Psychology, University of Verona, Italy fUOC of Psychiatry, Azienda Ospedaliera Universitaria Integrata (AOUI) of Verona, Italy
Chiara Bonetto
Affiliation:
cDepartment of Neurosciences, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, Section of Clinical Psychology, University of Verona, Italy
Doriana Cristofalo
Affiliation:
cDepartment of Neurosciences, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, Section of Clinical Psychology, University of Verona, Italy
Paolo Scocco
Affiliation:
hDepartment of Mental Health, AULSS 6 Euganea, Padua, Italy
Armando D’Agostino
Affiliation:
iDepartment of Health Sciences, San Paolo University Hospital, University of Milan, Milan, Italy
Stefano Torresani
Affiliation:
jDepartment of Psychiatry, Azienda ULSS, Bolzano, Italy
Massimiliano Imbesi
Affiliation:
kDepartment of Psychiatry, AUSL Emilia, Italy
Francesca Bellini
Affiliation:
lDepartment of Psychiatry, AUSL Romagna, Italy
Angela Veronese
Affiliation:
hDepartment of Mental Health, AULSS 6 Euganea, Padua, Italy
Cinzia Bressi
Affiliation:
aUniversity of Milan, Department of Pathophysiology and Transplantation, Milan, Italy bFondazione IRCCS Ca’ Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Department of Neurosciences and Mental Health, Milan, Italy
Mirella Ruggeri
Affiliation:
cDepartment of Neurosciences, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, Section of Clinical Psychology, University of Verona, Italy fUOC of Psychiatry, Azienda Ospedaliera Universitaria Integrata (AOUI) of Verona, Italy
Paolo Brambilla*
Affiliation:
aUniversity of Milan, Department of Pathophysiology and Transplantation, Milan, Italy bFondazione IRCCS Ca’ Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Department of Neurosciences and Mental Health, Milan, Italy
*
*Corresponding author at: Fondazione IRCCS Ca’ Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, via F. Sforza 35, 20122 Milan, Italy. E-mail address: paolo.brambilla1@unimi.it (P. Brambilla).

Abstract

Background.

Research has consistently shown that language abilities represent a core dimension of psychosis; however, to date, very little is known about syntactic comprehension performance in the early stages of psychosis. This study aims to compare the linguistic abilities involved in syntactic comprehension in a large group of First Episode Psychosis (FEP) patients and healthy controls (HCs).

Methods.

A multiple choice test of comprehension of syntax was administered to 218 FEP patients (166 non-affective FEP patients [FEP-NA] and 52 affective FEP patients [FEP-A]) and 106 HCs. All participants were asked to match a sentence they listen with one out of four vignettes on a pc screen. Only one vignette represents the stimulus target, while the others are grammatical or non-grammatical (visual) distractors. Both grammatical and non-grammatical errors and performance in different syntactic constructions were considered.

Results.

FEP committed greater number of errors in the majority of TCGB language domains compared to HCs. Moreover, FEP-NA patients committed significantly more non-grammatical (z = −3.2, p = 0.007), locative (z = −4.7, p < 0.001), passive-negative (z = −3.2, p = 0.02), and relative (z = −4.6, p < 0.001) errors compared to HCs as well as more passive-affirmative errors compared to both HCs (z = −4.3, p < 0.001) and FEP-A (z = 3.1, p = 0.04). Finally, we also found that both FEP-NA and FEP-A committed more grammatical (FEP-NA: z = −9.2, p < 0.001 and FEP-A: z = −4.4, p < 0.001), total (FEP-NA: z = −8.2, p < 0.001 and FEP-A: z = 3.9, p = 0.002), and active-negative (FEP-NA: z = −5.8, p < 0.001 and FEP-A: z = −3.5, p = 0.01) errors compared to HCs.

Conclusions.

This study shows that the access to syntactic structures is already impaired in FEP patients, especially in those with FEP-NA, ultimately suggesting that language impairments represent a core and inner feature of psychosis even at early stages.

Type
Original article
Copyright
Copyright © 2019 European Psychiatric Association

1. Introduction

Psychosis has been found to be associated with linguistic deficits [1Reference Underwood, Kumari and Peters3] and poorer social and clinical outcomes [Reference Wilcox, Winokur and Tsuang4, Reference Roche, Segurado, Renwick, McClenaghan, Sexton and Frawley5]. The central role of language in the development of schizophrenia (SKZ) has been first hypothesized by Crow [Reference Crow6] who correlated the origin of psychotic symptoms with an altered hemispheric lateralization [Reference Crow7]. Furthermore, language comprehension deficits have been observed in the premorbid phase of SKZ [Reference Condray, Steinhauer and Goldstein8], further suggesting that linguistic assessment is important for predicting the development of psychosis [Reference Roche, Segurado, Renwick, McClenaghan, Sexton and Frawley5, 9Reference Fuller, Nopoulos, Arndt, O’Leary, Ho and Andreasen11]. Additionally, syntactic deficits have been found in both First Episode Psychosis (FEP) patients [Reference Thomas, Leudar, Newby and Johnston12] and in individuals at high-risk for psychosis [Reference Solomon, Olsen, Niendam, Ragland, Yoon and Minzenberg13].

Moreover, another pressing issue of clinical research is represented by the investigation of similarities and differences between affective and non-affective psychosis in the linguistic domain [Reference Lott, Guggenbühl, Schneeberger, Pulver and Stassen14]. Interestingly, although speech deficits have been observed in both affective and non-affective FEP [Reference Kravariti, Reichenberg, Morgan, Dazzan, Morgan and Zanelli15, Reference Xu, Hui, Longenecker, Lee, Chang and Chan16], syntactic comprehension abilities in these patients have not yet been clearly explored [Reference Kravariti, Reichenberg, Morgan, Dazzan, Morgan and Zanelli15]. In general, language is generally divided in comprehension processing and syntactic processing. Comprehension has been described as sufficient vocabulary and knowing the meanings of enough words, while syntax has been described as set of rules, specific for each language, used to combine words in sentences, each word requiring for the generation of well-formed sentences [Reference Bellani and Brambilla17].

Although only a few behavioral studies explored linguistic abilities in FEP patients, they have been consistently investigated in both patients with SKZ and BD. Indeed, two previous studies from our group explored the linguistic performance in chronic psychotic patients [Reference Tavano, Sponda, Fabbro, Perlini, Rambaldelli and Ferro18, Reference Perlini, Marini, Garzitto, Isola, Cerruti and Marinelli19]. In particular, Tavano et al. [Reference Tavano, Sponda, Fabbro, Perlini, Rambaldelli and Ferro18] showed that patients with SKZ were significantly less able to produce appropriate interpretations, indicating the presence of abnormal pragmatic inferential abilities.

Also, the presence of pragmatic deficits in SKZ was further confirmed by a more recent study carried out by Bambini et al. [Reference Bambini, Arcara, Bechi, Buonocorec, Cavallaro and Bosia20], which reported altered pragmatic abilities, especially in comprehending discourse and non-literal meanings, in 77% of their patients with SKZ, ultimately suggesting that these abilities might be considered a core feature of SKZ.

Similarly, Perlini et al. [Reference Perlini, Marini, Garzitto, Isola, Cerruti and Marinelli19] suggested that the syntactic receptive verbal abilities were impaired in both chronic SKZ and BD, being, however, more severe and generalized in SKZ than in BD. Also, impairments in syntactic abilities were suggested by Moro et al. [Reference Moro, Bambini, Bosia, Anselmetti, Riccaboni and Cappa21] in patients with SKZ while processing of an anomaly detection task, which allow to investigate either the syntactic or semantic knowledge. The authors also reported that the syntactic impairments were independent from cognitive abilities and psychopathological measures.

Interestingly, these studies further aligned with the evidence reported by several other independent studies, which showed that patients with SKZ had severe and generalized deficits in terms of language impairments [Reference Crow6, Reference Crow7, 22Reference Strik, Dierks, Hubl and Horn25].

Therefore, it seems that language abilities are important to be studied in psychosis, especially because language is a complex dynamic cognitive system, which brings to integration of multiple levels of linguistic and cognitive processing [Reference Spalletta, Tomaiuolo, Marino, Bonaviri, Trequattrini and Caltagirone26]. Indeed, psychotic patients showed word-finding difficulties and lexical processing impairments [Reference Iwashiro, Koike, Satomura, Suga, Nagai and Natsubori27] as well as they have the speech usually filled with irrelevant pieces of information and derailments [Reference Marini, Spoletini, Rubino, Ciuffa, Bria and Martinotti28].

In this context, this study aims to compare, for the first time, the linguistic abilities involved in syntactic comprehension in a large group of FEP patients and healthy controls (HCs), by considering both affective (FEP-A) and non-affective (FEP-NA) psychosis. In particular, we expected that a) FEP patients would show deficits in linguistic performance compared to HCs and b) FEP-NA patients would display more severe disturbances compared to both FEP-A and HCs.

2. Methods

2.1 Subjects

Patients were recruited in the frame of the ‘Genetics Endophenotypes and Treatment: Understanding early Psychosis’ (GET UP) Study (see Ruggeri et al. [Reference Ruggeri, Bonetto, Lasalvia, De Girolamo, Fioritti and Rucci29, Reference Ruggeri, Bonetto, Lasalvia, Fioritti, De Girolamo and Santonastaso30] for a detailed description of subjects’ enrolment). ICD-10 diagnoses were obtained after 9 months from the first contact with participating psychiatric services using the Item Group Checklist of the Schedule for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (IGC-SCAN) [Reference World Health Organization31]. Overall, 218 FEP patients and 106 HCs were evaluated. Among FEP, 166 were diagnosed with non-affective psychosis (FEP-NA) and 52 with affective psychosis (FEP-A). Specifically, for FEP-NA patients the diagnoses were schizophrenia (F20; N = 64), schizotypal disorder (F21; N = 4), delusional disorder (F22; N = 33), brief psychotic disorder (F23; N = 33), schizoaffective disorder (F25; N = 20), unspecified psychosis not due to a substance or known physiological condition (F29; N = 12). For FEP-A patients the diagnoses were mania with psychotic symptoms (F30.2; N = 13), bipolar affective disorder (F31; N = 6), bipolar affective disorder, current manic episode without psychotic symptoms (F31.1; N = 1), bipolar affective disorder, current manic episode with psychotic symptoms (F31.2; N = 4), bipolar affective disorder, current episode of severe depression with psychotic symptoms (F31.5; N = 1), bipolar affective disorder, current mixed episode (F31.6; N = 2), bipolar affective disorder, unspecified (F31.9; N = 2), mild depressive episode (F32; N = 1), severe depressive episode with psychotic symptoms (F32.3; N = 22). Symptoms were assessed using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) [Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler32], which is formed by one total score (PANSS-Total) and three sub-scales evaluating positive symptoms (PANSS-Positive), negative symptoms (PANSS-Negative) and general psychopathology (PANSS- Psychopathology), the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS) [Reference Hamilton33], and the Bech–Rafaelsen Mania Rating Scale (BRMRS) [Reference Bech, Rafaelsen, Kramp and Bolwig34]. Also, the Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) [Reference American Psychiatric Association35] was administered. Finally, the Brief Intelligence Test (TIB) [Reference Colombo, Sartori and Brivio36] was used to obtain a measure of the Intelligence Quotient (IQ). Patients with other mental and behavioural disorders, alcohol or substance abuse in the six months preceding the assessment, history of traumatic head injury, neurological or medical disease and mental retardation were excluded from the study. Notably, 99 out of 218 FEP patients (27 FEP-A and 72 FEP-NA) were drug-free. In contrast, 116 FEP patients (23 FEP-A and 93 FEP-NA) were taking different antipsychotic medications, either typical or atypical, and 3 FEP patients were taking only antidepressants (2 FEP-A and 1 FEP-NA). Specifically, FEP-A patients were taking haloperidol (N = 1), aripiprazole (N = 4), olanzapine (N = 8), paliperidone (N = 1), quetiapine (N = 5), risperidone (N = 6), and ziprasidone (N = 1). In contrast, FEP-NA were taking: haloperidol (N = 9), aripiprazole (N = 14), clozapine (N = 1), perphenazine (N = 2), olanzapine (N = 28), paliperidone (N = 8), quetiapine (N = 6), risperidone (N = 31) and ziprasidone (N = 1). With regards to antidepressants, three patients were taking only sertraline (N = 2, FEP-A) or paroxetine (N = 1, FEP-NA). Finally, within the groups of FEP-A patients, 11 were taking antidepressants or mood stabilizers in association with antipsychotics. HCs were recruited by word of mouth and leaflets. Subjects with a history of psychiatric symptoms, also in first degree relatives, were excluded from the study. All participants had Italian as native language. The GET UP was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Azienda Ospedaliera of Verona (http://www.ospedaliverona.it/Istituzionale/Comitati-Etici/Sperimentazione) on 6 May 2009 (Prot. N. 20406/CE, Date 14/05/2009), and by the ethics committee of each participating unit [Reference Ruggeri, Bonetto, Lasalvia, De Girolamo, Fioritti and Rucci29]. All participants signed inform consent after having understood all issues involved in the study design.

2.2 Syntactic comprehension measures

A multiple choice test of comprehension of syntax was administered to all participants. In particular, an adapted computer based version of the ‘Test di Comprensione Grammaticale per Bambini’ (TCGB) [Reference Chilosi and Cipriani37] assessing syntactic comprehension was used. This test has been used in previous research conducted by our group, showing good psychometric properties [Reference Tavano, Sponda, Fabbro, Perlini, Rambaldelli and Ferro18, Reference Perlini, Marini, Garzitto, Isola, Cerruti and Marinelli19]. Shortly, subjects were asked to match a sentence they hear with one out of four vignettes on a PC screen. Only one vignette represents the stimulus target, while the others are grammatical (which have a role of syntactic contrast with respect to the target) or non-grammatical (visual) (which do not have any specific role of syntactic contrast) distractors. The task analyses different grammatical structure such as locative (e.g." The dog is above the chair"), active negative (e.g. "The girl doesn’t run"), passive-negative (e.g. "The piano is not played"), passive-affirmative (e.g. "The car is washed by the child"), and relative (e.g. "The vase that the child is painting is on the chair"). For a more detailed description of the test please refer to Perlini et al. [Reference Perlini, Marini, Garzitto, Isola, Cerruti and Marinelli19].

2.3 Statistical analyses

All the analyses were conducted using R [Reference Team38]. For exploring the presence of differences between the groups on clinical and sociodemographic variables we performed a chi–square test (χ2), for qualitative variables (i.e. gender), and t -tests or Analysis of Variance (ANOVA), for quantitative variables. Then, we employed a hierarchical approach for investigating the differences between the groups in TCGB variables [Reference Goeman and Finos39]. First, a general Multivariate Analyses of variance (MANOVA) with all TCGB variables as dependent variables as well as group, age, TIB and educational level as covariates were carried out in order to explore whether the variable “group” was significant. The MANOVA was carried out between the two groups (whole group of FEP vs HCs) and between the three groups (FEP-NA, FEP-A and HCs). Second, since the assumption of normality, necessary for a standard linear model, was not satisfied, a gamma generalized linear model with identity link function, corrected for Bonferroni, with group, age, TIB and educational level as covariates, was performed separately after we found that the variable group was significant in the MANOVA. Then, for each significant model, a post-hoc analysis was performed and the Holm correction for multiple comparisons was applied. Finally, ANOVAs based on gamma generalized linear model with identity link function with age, TIB and educational level as covariates, were carried out in order to analyse the relationship between clinical features and TCGB scales. We used Bonferroni to correct for multiple testing. Additionally, Cohen's f was employed for measuring the effect size of the regression in order to provide measures of magnitude of the observed correlations. Cohen's f2 values can be interpreted as small (0.02), medium (0.15) and large (0.35) [Reference Cohen40]. Notably, the regressions were considered significant based on both p-value and effect sizes, as suggested by Sullivan and Feinn [Reference Sullivan and Feinn41].

3. Results

3.1 Socio demographic and clinical variables

No statistically significant differences were found in any of the socio-demographic variables between the whole group of FEP patients vs HCs as well as between FEP-NA, FEP-A and HCs, except for the educational level. Specifically, we observed that the whole group of FEP patients had lower educational level compared to HCs (t = 10, df = 227.3, p < 0.001). Similarly, we also found that educational level was different between the FEP-NA, FEP-A and HCs (F(2,321) = 46.5, p < 0.001). Specifically, post-hoc tests showed that FEP-A (z = 5.4, p < 0.001) and FEP-NA (z = 8.3, p < 0.001) had lower educational level compared to HCs. Furthermore, significant differences were observed between FEP-NA and FEP-A patients on some clinical variables. Specifically, we found that FEP-NA patients showed higher PANSS-Total (t = 4.3, df = 112.4, p < 0.001), PANSS-Positive (t = 6.1, df = 113.4, p < 0.001) and PANSS-Psychopathology (t = 3.1, df = 111.4, p = 0.03) compared to FEP-A. Also, FEP-NA patients showed lower GAF scores compared to FEP-A (t= −3.4, df = 79.0, p = 0.01). Finally, the two groups did not differ in terms of PANSS-Negative, HDRS and BRMRS scores as well as in terms of dose of antipsychotics. Socio-demographic and clinical data are shown in Tables 1 and 2.

3.2 Syntactic comprehension

3.2.1 FEP patients vs healthy controls

Table 3 and Fig. 1 showed the significant results emerged from the post-hoc analysis.

Overall, the results showed that FEP patients committed more total errors than HCs in the comprehension of syntactic constructions (z = −8.1, p < 0.001). Specifically, they produced significantly greater grammatical (z = −9.2, p < 0.001) and non grammatical (z = −3.2, p = 0.005) errors as well as locative (z = −4.4, p < 0.001), active-negative (z = −6.1, p < 0.001), passive-affirmative (z = −3.5, p = 0.002) and relative (z = −4.3, p < 0.001) errors compared to HCs. No differences were observed in passive-negative sentences (z = −2.6, p = 0.07).

Table 1 Socio-demographic and clinical variables in the whole group of first episode psychosis (FEP) patients and healthy controls (HCs).

BRMRS = Bech-Rafaelsen Manic Rating Scale; df = Degree of Freedom; PANSS = Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; HDRS = Hamilton Depressive Rating Scale; FEP = First Episode Psychosis; GAF = Global Assessment of Functioning; HCs = Healthy controls; SD = Standard Deviation; TIB = Brief Intelligence Test.

a The statistical tests applied were the chi–square test for qualitative variables and t –tests for quantitative variables.

Table 2 Socio-demographic and clinical variables in the three study groups.

BRMRS = Bech-Rafaelsen Mania Rating Scale; df = Degree of Freedom; PANSS = Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; HDRS = Hamilton Depressive Rating Scale; GAF = Global Assessment of Functioning; FEP-A = Affective First Episode Psychosis; FEP-NA = Non-Affective First Episode Psychosis; HCs = Healthy Controls; SD = Standard Deviation; TIB = Brief Intelligence Test.

a The statistical tests applied were the chi–square test for qualitative variables and t –tests or Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) for quantitative variables.

Table 3 Differences in syntactic comprehension between first episode psychosis (FEP) patients and healthy controls (HCs).

FEP = First Episode Psychosis; HCs = Healthy Controls; SD = Standard Deviation; TCGB = Test di Comprensione Grammaticale per Bambini.

a The post-hoc tests were calculated on the coefficient of a generalized linear model with gamma and identity link function corrected for multiple testing with Bonferroni correction.

Fig. 1. Significant mean differences in TCGB syntactic comprehension measures between first episode psychosis (FEP) patients and healthy controls (HCs). The post-hoc tests were calculated on the coefficient of a generalized linear model with gamma and identity link function corrected for multiple testing with Bonferroni correction.

Table 4 Differences in syntactic comprehension among the three study groups.

FEP-A = Affective First Episode psychosis; FEP-NA = Non-affective First Episode Psychosis; HCs = Healthy Controls; TCGB = Test di Comprensione Grammaticale per Bambini.

a The post-hoc tests were calculated on the coefficient of a generalized linear model with gamma and identity link function, corrected for multiple comparisons with Holm method and for multiple testing with Bonferroni correction.

3.2.2 Affective FEP patients vs non-affective FEP patients vs healthy controls

Table 4 and Fig. 2 showed the significant results emerged from the post-hoc analysis.

Comparison between the three groups showed that FEP-NA and FEP-A patients committed more grammatical (FEP-NA: z = −9.2, p < 0.001; FEP-A: z = −4.4, p < 0.001), total (FEP-NA: z = −8.2, p < 0.001; FEP-A: z = −3.9, p = 0.002) and active-negative (FEP-NA: z = −5.8, p < 0.001; FEP-A: z = −3.5, p = 0.01) errors compared to HCs. Interestingly, FEP-NA patients also committed more non-grammatical (z = −3.2, p = 0.007), locative (z = −4.7, p < 0.001), passive negative (z = −3.2, p = 0.02) and relative (z = −4.6, p < 0.001) errors compared to HCs. Finally, FEP-NA patients produced more passive-affirmative errors compared to HCs (z = −4.3, p < 0.001) and FEP-A (z = 3.1, p = 0.04).

3.3 Correlations between syntactic comprehension and clinical or sociodemographic variables in FEP patients

No statistical significant correlations were observed between TCGB measures and educational level, PANSS-Positive, PANSS-Psychopathology, HDRS scores, BRMRS scores, GAF scores and dose of antipsychotics in the whole FEP group, except for PANSS-Negative that significantly positively correlated with TCGB grammatical errors (p = 0.04; effect size = 0.06). Additionally, no statistical significant correlations were observed between TCGB measures and educational level, PANNS-Negative, PANSS-Positive, PANSS-Psychopathology, PANSS-Total, HDRS scores, BRMRS scores, GAF scores and dose of antipsychotics in FEP-NA. Similarly, we found no statistical significant correlations between all TCGB measures and these scales also in FEP-A patients, except for TCGB grammatical errors, which were significantly positively correlated with GAF scores (p = 0.049; effect size = 0.04). Finally, with regards to HCs, no statistical significant correlations were observed between TCGB measures and educational level scores.

Please refer to Supplementary Materials for all the tables showing the p-values and effect sizes of the correlations performed between all TCGB measures and clinical or sociodemographic variables in the whole groups of FEP (Table S1), FEP-NA (Table S2), FEP-A (Table S3) and HCs (Table S4).

4. Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study in the literature that explored syntactic comprehension language abilities in a large sample of FEP patients.

Overall, we found a greater number of errors in performing the syntactic comprehension test in the group of FEP patients as compared to HCs. This finding is consistent with previous research on patients with SKZ both conducted by our [Reference Tavano, Sponda, Fabbro, Perlini, Rambaldelli and Ferro18, Reference Perlini, Marini, Garzitto, Isola, Cerruti and Marinelli19] and other [Reference Thomas, Leudar, Newby and Johnston12, Reference Kravariti, Reichenberg, Morgan, Dazzan, Morgan and Zanelli15, Reference Xu, Hui, Longenecker, Lee, Chang and Chan16] research groups. Indeed, in Tavano et al. [Reference Tavano, Sponda, Fabbro, Perlini, Rambaldelli and Ferro18] patients with SKZ showed significantly poorer syntactic diversity skills, including narrative and conversational speech, than HCs. Similarly, Perlini et al. [Reference Perlini, Marini, Garzitto, Isola, Cerruti and Marinelli19] reported that patients with SKZ had selective impairments in active-negative sentences, as measured by the same TCGB test employed in our study, compared to HCs. Furthermore, the study also showed that patients with longer history of illness had a more generalized deficit in correctly identifying syntactic constructions compared to HCs [Reference Perlini, Marini, Garzitto, Isola, Cerruti and Marinelli19]. Taken together, these findings suggest that syntactic deficits are a core dimension of psychosis, as also hypothesized by Crow et al. [Reference Crow6, Reference Crow7]. Specifically, according to Crow’s hypothesis, the nuclear symptoms of psychoses represent a failure in establishing the left hemisphere dominance for language, which is one of the most lateralized dimension (most commonly to the left hemisphere) [Reference Crow7]. Indeed, it has to be noted that in the healthy brain syntactic abilities are anatomically sustained by a distributed fronto-temporal network within the left hemisphere, including the inferior frontal gyrus and the posterior superior temporal sulcus [42Reference Blank, Balewski, Mahowald and Fedorenko46]. Therefore, altered lateralization, which is defined as an inverse asymmetry or a lack of prevalence of one hemisphere on the other, is one of the most replicated behavioral and neuroimaging findings in psychosis, being also present in FEP and in subjects at genetic risk to develop psychosis [47Reference Crow, Chance, Priddle, Radua and James50]. In this context, we might hypothesize that cerebral asymmetry changes may possibly explain the deterioration of syntactic abilities that we observed in our group of FEP subjects. However, further studies are necessary to confirm these alterations over time.

Fig. 2. Significant mean differences in TCGB syntactic comprehension measures between affective first episode psychosis (FEP) patients, non-affective FEP patients and healthy controls (HCs). The post-hoc tests were calculated on the coefficient of a generalized linear model with gamma and identity link function, corrected for multiple comparisons with Holm method and for multiple testing with Bonferroni correction.

Moreover, our findings showed that both FEP-NA and FEP-A patients committed more grammatical and active negative errors compared to HCs, ultimately suggesting a shared syntactic deficit between the two diagnostic groups. These common syntactic impairments may be understood within the psychosis continuum theoretical framework [Reference Craddock, O’Donovan and Owen51]. The adoption of categorical system alone to classify psychotic disorders has been, indeed, widely questioned in light of evidence showing shared cognitive and brain deficits between non-affective and affective psychosis [52Reference Lewandowski, Cohen, Keshavan and Öngür54]. Notably, a recent study carried out by our group also reported similar deficits in linguistic and emotional prosodic [Reference Caletti, Delvecchio, Andreella, Finos, Perlini and Tavano55] as well as in comprehension of figurative language [Reference Perlini, Bellani, Finos, Lasalvia, Bonetto and Scocco56] deficits between affective and non-affective FEP patients. Therefore, in line with these evidence, the syntactic deficits observed in both FEP-NA and FEP-A patients may represent a common clinical feature consistent with a possible nosological overlap. However, it is worth noticing that FEP-NA patients showed unique syntactic impairments, with worse performance in non-grammatical, locative, passive-affirmative, passive-negative and relative sentences compared to HCs as well as in passive-affirmative constructions compared to FEP-A patients, ultimately suggesting a more extensive language dysfunction in these patients. This result is also in line with our previous study [Reference Caletti, Delvecchio, Andreella, Finos, Perlini and Tavano55] showing more prominent prosodic deficits in FEP-NA patients compared to FEP-A, as well as with previous research showing more severe neuropsychological deficits in SKZ as compared to BD [Reference Krabbendam, Arts, van Os and Aleman57, Reference Caletti, Paoli, Fiorentini, Cigliobianco, Zugno and Serati58], even when matched for clinical and demographic characteristics [Reference Krabbendam, Arts, van Os and Aleman57]. Furthermore, the more extensive language deficits found in our group of FEP-NA patients could be anatomically sustained by a more remarkable disruption of leftward functional hemispheric lateralization for language observed in SKZ compared to BD, independently of task performance, severity of symptoms, age and gender, as suggested by previous studies [59Reference Royer, Delcroix, Leroux, Alary, Razafimandimby and Brazo61].

Therefore, based on these evidence, language disturbances seem to have a central role in the presentation of psychotic disorders and it should be considered as a potential target of intervention in FEP patients, as also suggested by a previous study [Reference Roche, Segurado, Renwick, McClenaghan, Sexton and Frawley5].

4.1 Correlations between TCGB measures and clinical variables

Our results showed the presence of significant correlations between clinical symptoms and syntactic abilities only in the whole group of FEP and FEP-A patients. Specifically, we found that PANSS-Negative, for the whole group of FEP patients, and GAF scores, for only FEP-A patients, positively correlated with TCGB grammatical errors, ultimately suggesting that illness severity played a key role in influencing the performance in this test. Interestingly, these results seemed to partially align with the evidence reported by our previous study investigating prosody abilities in a partially overlapping sample, which showed that emotional prosody impairments were significantly correlated with illness severity [Reference Caletti, Delvecchio, Andreella, Finos, Perlini and Tavano55]. Moreover, our results are also in agreement with evidence reporting significant correlations between illness severity and cognitive impairments in BD [Reference Zubieta, Huguelet, Lajiness-O’Neill and Giordani62] and SKZ [Reference Heydebrand, Weiser, Rabinowitz, Hoff, DeLisi and Csernansky63]. Indeed, it has been found that in BD the performance in executive functions was negatively correlated with the number of manic or depressive episodes [Reference Zubieta, Huguelet, Lajiness-O’Neill and Giordani62]. Similarly, Heydebrand et al. [Reference Heydebrand, Weiser, Rabinowitz, Hoff, DeLisi and Csernansky63] also observed that negative symptoms were associated with deficits in memory, verbal fluency and executive functions in first episode SKZ.

Therefore, our study further suggests that clinical symptomatology might play a key role on language deficits in FEP, ultimately proposing the need of a clear clinical stratification of FEP patients for a better understanding of language deficits in these patients. However, it is important to point out that the significant correlations found in our study were characterized by small effect sizes and therefore further studies are warranted to confirm our results.

4.2 Limitations

Although this study investigated language abilities in a large sample of FEP patients, some limitations might have reduced the generalizability of the results and therefore should be taken into account. First, all participating patients were on medication, which may have a potential confounding role. Therefore, future studies on medication-free FEP patients are warranted for ruling out the possible medication effects on language abilities. Second, the specific cognitive abilities were not assessed and they should be considered in future studies, especially because executive functions and working memory have an important role in language abilities, as suggested by previous investigations [64Reference Wilson, Galantucci, Tartaglia, Rising, Patterson and Henry66]. Nevertheless, in this study we controlled for an estimated measure of the IQ in all the statistical analyses.

5. Conclusion

This study shows that the syntactic comprehension impairments are already present in both FEP-NA and FEP-A patients, confirming that reduced access to syntactic structures is a core impairment of psychoses, in particular in FEP-NA patients. Therefore, given the centrality of language domain for functional outcome, the assessment of different aspects of language and the remediation of linguistic comprehension alterations should be included in routine clinical practice in the early phases of psychosis. Finally, further neuroimaging studies should be warranted in order to explore the relationship between syntactic comprehension impairment and cerebral asymmetry and connectivity at different illness stages.

Declaration of interest

None.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by grants from the Italian Ministry of Health (H61J0800020000 to MR, RF-2016-02364582 to PB) and the Fondazione Cariverona (Promoting research to improve quality of care; Sotto-obiettivo A9 ‘Disability cognitiva e comportamentale nelle demenze e nelle psicosi’ to PB and MR).

Appendix A Supplementary data

Supplementary material related to this article can be found, in the online version, at doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2019.08.001.

Footnotes

1.

The two authors contributed equally to this work.

References

Stain, H.J., Hodne, S., Joa, I., ten Velden Hegelstad, W., Douglas, K.M., Langveld, J., et al. The relationship of verbal learning and verbal fluency with written story production: implications for social functioning in first episode psychosis. Schizophr Res, 2012; 138(2–3): 212217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reser, M.P., Allott, K.A., Killackey, E., Farhall, J., Cotton, S.M., Exploring cognitive heterogeneity in first-episode psychosis: what cluster analysis can reveal. Psychiatry Res. 2015; 229(3): 819827CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Underwood, R., Kumari, V., Peters, E., Cognitive and neural models of threat appraisal in psychosis: a theoretical integration. Psychiatry Res, 2016; 239: 131138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilcox, J., Winokur, G., Tsuang, M., Predictive value of thought disorder in new-onset psychosis. Compr Psychiatry. 2012; 53(6): 674678CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roche, E., Segurado, R., Renwick, L., McClenaghan, A., Sexton, S., Frawley, T., et al. Language disturbance and functioning in first episode psychosis. Psychiatry Res, 2016; 235: 2937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crow, T.J., Is schizophrenia the price that Homo sapiens pays for language?. Schizophr Res, 1997; 28(2–3): 127141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crow, T.J., Schizophrenia as the price that Homo sapiens pays for language: a resolution of the central paradox in the origin of the species. Brain Res Rev, 2000; 31(2–3): 118129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Condray, R., Steinhauer, S.R., Goldstein, G., Language comprehension in schizophrenics and their brothers. Biol Psychiatry. 1992; 32(9): 790802CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klosterkötter, J., Hellmich, M., Steinmeyer, E.M., Schultze-Lutter, F., Diagnosing schizophrenia in the initial prodromal phase. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001; 58(2): 158164CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannon, M., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T.E., Harrington, H., Taylor, A., Murray, R.M., et al. Evidence for early-childhood, pan-developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002; 59(5): 449456CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuller, R., Nopoulos, P., Arndt, S., O’Leary, D., Ho, B.C., Andreasen, N.C., Longitudinal assessment of premorbid cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia through examination of standardized scholastic test performance. Am J Psychiatry. 2002; 159(7): 11831189CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, P., Leudar, I., Newby, D., Johnston, M., Syntactic processing and written language output in first onset psychosis. J Commun Disord. 1993; 26(4): 209230CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solomon, M., Olsen, E., Niendam, T., Ragland, J.D., Yoon, J., Minzenberg, M., et al. From lumping to splitting and back again: atypical social and language development in individuals with clinical-high-risk for psychosis, first episode schizophrenia, and autism spectrum disorders. Schizophr Res, 2011; 131(1–3): 146151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lott, P.R., Guggenbühl, S., Schneeberger, A., Pulver, A.E., Stassen, H.H., Linguistic analysis of the speech output of schizophrenic, bipolar, and depressive patients. Psychopathology. 2002; 35(4): 220227CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kravariti, E., Reichenberg, A., Morgan, K., Dazzan, P., Morgan, C., Zanelli, J.W., et al. Selective deficits in semantic verbal fluency in patients with a first affective episode with psychotic symptoms and a positive history of mania. Bipolar Disord. 2009; 11(3): 323329CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu, J.Q., Hui, C.L.M., Longenecker, J., Lee, E.H.M., Chang, W.C., Chan, S.K.W., et al. Executive function as predictors of persistent thought disorder in first-episode schizophrenia: a one-year follow-up study. Schizophr Res. 2014; 159(2): 465470CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellani, M., Brambilla, P., Social cognition, schizophrenia and brain imaging. Epidemiol Psychiatry Soc. 2008; 17(2): 117119CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tavano, A., Sponda, S., Fabbro, F., Perlini, C., Rambaldelli, G., Ferro, A., et al. Specific linguistic and pragmatic deficits in Italian patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res, 2008; 102(1–3): 5362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perlini, C., Marini, A., Garzitto, M., Isola, M., Cerruti, S., Marinelli, V., et al. Linguistic production and syntactic comprehension in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2012; 126(5): 363376CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bambini, V., Arcara, G., Bechi, M., Buonocorec, M., Cavallaro, R., Bosia, M., The communicative impairment as a core feature of schizophrenia: frequency of pragmatic deficit, cognitive substrates, and relation with quality of life. Compr Psychiatry, 2016; 71: 106120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moro, A., Bambini, V., Bosia, M., Anselmetti, S., Riccaboni, R., Cappa, S.F., et al. Detecting syntactic and semantic anomalies in schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia, 2015; 79: 147157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeLisi, L.E., Speech disorder in schizophrenia: review of the literature and exploration of its relation to the uniquely human capacity for language. Schizophr Bull. 2001; 27(3): 481496CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
David, A.S., The cognitive neuropsychiatry of auditory verbal hallucinations: an overview. Cogn Neuropsychiatry, 2004; 9(1–2): 107123.Google ScholarPubMed
McKenna, P.J., Oh, T.M., Schizophrenic speech. Making sense of bathroots and ponds that fall in doorways, Cambridge, New York: Cambridge University Press; 2015.Google Scholar
Strik, W., Dierks, T., Hubl, D., Horn, H., Hallucinations, thought disorders, and the language domain in schizophrenia. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2008; 39(2): 9194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spalletta, G., Tomaiuolo, F., Marino, V., Bonaviri, G., Trequattrini, A., Caltagirone, C., Chronic schizophrenia as a brain misconnection syndrome: a white matter voxel-based morphometry study. Schizophr Res. 2003; 64(1): 1523CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iwashiro, N., Koike, S., Satomura, Y., Suga, M., Nagai, T., Natsubori, T., et al. Association between impaired brain activity and volume at the sub-region of Broca’s area in ultra-high risk and first-episode schizophrenia: a multi-modal neuroimaging study. Schizophr Res, 2016; 172(1–3): 915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marini, A., Spoletini, I., Rubino, I.A., Ciuffa, M., Bria, P., Martinotti, G., et al. The language of schizophrenia: an analysis of micro and macrolinguistic abilities and their neuropsychological correlates. Schizophr Res, 2008; 105(1–3): 144155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruggeri, M., Bonetto, C., Lasalvia, A., De Girolamo, G., Fioritti, A., Rucci, P. et al. A multi-element psychosocial intervention for early psychosis (GET UP PIANO TRIAL) conducted in a catchment area of 10 million inhabitants: study protocol for a pragmatic cluster randomized controlled trial. Trials, 2012: 3(1)73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruggeri, M., Bonetto, C., Lasalvia, A., Fioritti, A., De Girolamo, G., Santonastaso, P., et al. Feasibility and effectiveness of a multi-element psychosocial intervention for first-episode psychosis: results from the cluster-randomized controlled GET UP PIANO trial in a catchment area of 10 million inhabitants. Schizophr Bull. 2015; 41(5): 11921203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Health Organization, , Schedules for clinical assessment in neuropsychiatry, Geneva: WHO; 1992.Google Scholar
Kay, S.R., Fiszbein, A., Opler, L.A., The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987; 13(2): 261276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, M., A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry, 1960; 23: 5662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bech, P., Rafaelsen, O.J., Kramp, P., Bolwig, T.G., The mania rating scale: scale construction and inter-observer agreement. Neuropharmacology. 1978; 17(6): 430431CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association, , Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders 3rd edn, revised, Washington, DC: APA; 1987. (DSM–III–R)Google Scholar
Colombo, L., Sartori, G., Brivio, C., Stima del quoziente intellettivo tramite l’applicazione del TIB (test breve di Intelligenza). Giornale Italiano di Psicologia.. 2002; 29(3): 613638Google Scholar
Chilosi, A.M., Cipriani, P., Test di Comprensione Grammaticale per Bambini, Italy: Del Cerro, Tirrenia; 1995.Google Scholar
Team, R.C., R: a language and environment for statistical computing, Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013.Google Scholar
Goeman, J.J., Finos, L., The inheritance procedure: multiple testing of tree structured hypotheses. Stat Appl Genet Mol Biol, 2012; 11(1): 21CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J., Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences 2nd ed., Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers; 1988.Google Scholar
Sullivan, G.M., Feinn, R., Using effect size—or why the P value is not enough. J Grad Med Educ. 2012; 4(3): 279282CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grodzinsky, Y., The neurology of syntax: language use without Broca’s area. Behav Brain Sci. 2000; 23(1): 121CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodd, J.M., Vitello, S., Woollams, A.M., Adank, P., Localising semantic and syntactic processing in spoken and written language comprehension: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Brain Lang, 2015; 141: 89102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaccarella, E., Friederici, A.D., Merge in the human brain: a sub-region based functional investigation in the left pars opercularis. Front Psychol, 2015; 6: 1818CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xiao, Y., Friederici, A.D., Margulies, D.S., Brauer, J., Development of a selective left-hemispheric fronto-temporal network for processing syntactic complexity in language comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 2016; 3: 274282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blank, I., Balewski, Z., Mahowald, K., Fedorenko, E., Syntactic processing is distributed across the language system. Neuroimage, 2016; 127: 307323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Francis, A.N., Seidman, L.J., Jabbar, G.A., Mesholam-Gately, R., Thermenos, H.W., Juelich, R., et al. Alterations in brain structures underlying language function in young adults at high familial risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2012; 141(1): 6571CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, X., Alapati, V., Jackson, C., Xia, S., Bertisch, H.C., Branch, C.A., et al. Structural abnormalities in language circuits in genetic high-risk subjects and schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry Res. 2012; 201(3): 182189CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, H.Y., Hwang, J.Y., Jung, W.H., Shin, N.Y., Shim, G., Jang, J.H., et al. Altered asymmetry of the anterior cingulate cortex in subjects at genetic high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res, 2013; 150(2–3): 512518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crow, T.J., Chance, S.A., Priddle, T.H., Radua, J., James, A.C., Laterality interacts with sex across the schizophrenia/bipolarity continuum: an interpretation of meta-analyses of structural MRI. Psychiatry Res. 2013; 210(3): 12321244CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craddock, N., O’Donovan, M.C., Owen, M.J., Psychosis genetics: modeling the relationship between schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and mixed (or “schizoaffective”) psychoses. Schizophr Bull. 2009; 35(3): 482490CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calvo, A., Delvecchio, G., Altamura, A.C., Soares, J.C., Brambilla, P., Gray matter volume differences between affective and non-affective first episode psychosis: a review of magnetic resonance Imaging studies. J Affect Disord, 2018; 243: 564574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Peri, L., Crescini, A., Deste, G., Fusar-Poli, P., Sacchetti, E., Vita, A., Brain structural abnormalities at the onset of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis of controlled magnetic resonance imaging studies. Curr Pharm Des. 2012; 18(4): 486494CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewandowski, K.E., Cohen, B.M., Keshavan, M.S., Öngür, D., Relationship of neurocognitive deficits to diagnosis and symptoms across affective and non-affective psychoses. Schizophr Res, 2011; 133(1–3): 212217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caletti, E., Delvecchio, G., Andreella, A., Finos, L., Perlini, C., Tavano, A., et al. Prosody abilities in a large sample of affective and non-affective first episode psychosis patients. Compr Psychiatry, 2018; 86: 3138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perlini, C., Bellani, M., Finos, L., Lasalvia, A., Bonetto, C., Scocco, P., et al. Non literal language comprehension in a large sample of first episode psychosis patients in adulthood. Psychiatry Res, 2018; 260: 7889.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krabbendam, L., Arts, B., van Os, J., Aleman, A., Cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a quantitative review. Schizophr Res, 2005; 80(2–3): 137149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caletti, E., Paoli, R.A., Fiorentini, A., Cigliobianco, M., Zugno, E., Serati, M. et al. Neuropsychology, social cognition and global functioning among bipolar, schizophrenic patients and healthy controls: preliminary data. Front Hum Neurosci, 2013; 7: 661CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rao, N.P., Arasappa, R., Reddy, N.N., Venkatasubramanian, G., Gangadhar, B.N., Antithetical asymmetry in schizophrenia and bipolar affective disorder: a line bisection study. Bipolar Disord. 2010; 12(3): 221229CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alary, M., Razafimandimby, A., Delcroix, N., Leroux, E., Delamillieure, P., Brazo, P., et al. Reduced functional cerebral lateralization: a biomarker of schizophrenia?. Bipolar Disord. 2013; 15(4): 449451CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Royer, C., Delcroix, N., Leroux, E., Alary, M., Razafimandimby, A., Brazo, P., et al. Functional and structural brain asymmetries in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorders. Schizophr Res, 2015; 161(2–3): 210214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zubieta, J.K., Huguelet, P., Lajiness-O’Neill, R., Giordani, B.J., Cognitive function in euthymic bipolar I disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2001; 102(1): 920CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heydebrand, G., Weiser, M., Rabinowitz, J., Hoff, A.L., DeLisi, L.E., Csernansky, J.G., Correlates of cognitive deficits in first episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res, 2004; 68: 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bora, E., Yucel, M., Pantelis, C., Cognitive functioning in schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder and affective psychoses: meta-analytic study. Br J Psychiatry. 2009; 195(6): 475482CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swets, B., Desmet, T., Hambrick, D.Z., Ferreira, F., The role of working memory in syntactic ambiguity resolution: a psychometric approach. J Exp Psychol Gen, 2007; 136(1): 64CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, S.M., Galantucci, S., Tartaglia, M.C., Rising, K., Patterson, D.K., Henry, M.L., et al. Syntactic processing depends on dorsal language tracts. Neuron. 2011; 2(2): 397403CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Socio-demographic and clinical variables in the whole group of first episode psychosis (FEP) patients and healthy controls (HCs).

Figure 1

Table 2 Socio-demographic and clinical variables in the three study groups.

Figure 2

Table 3 Differences in syntactic comprehension between first episode psychosis (FEP) patients and healthy controls (HCs).

Figure 3

Fig. 1. Significant mean differences in TCGB syntactic comprehension measures between first episode psychosis (FEP) patients and healthy controls (HCs). The post-hoc tests were calculated on the coefficient of a generalized linear model with gamma and identity link function corrected for multiple testing with Bonferroni correction.

Figure 4

Table 4 Differences in syntactic comprehension among the three study groups.

Figure 5

Fig. 2. Significant mean differences in TCGB syntactic comprehension measures between affective first episode psychosis (FEP) patients, non-affective FEP patients and healthy controls (HCs). The post-hoc tests were calculated on the coefficient of a generalized linear model with gamma and identity link function, corrected for multiple comparisons with Holm method and for multiple testing with Bonferroni correction.

Supplementary material: File

Delvecchio et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S4
Download Delvecchio et al. supplementary material(File)
File 35 KB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.