Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-75dct Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-09T04:03:51.023Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Tom Moore and Xoseé-Lois Armada, eds. Atlantic Europe in the First Millennium BC. Crossing the Divide (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2011 690pp., 141 illustrations, hbk, ISBN 978-0-19-956795-9)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 January 2017

Emiliano Bruner*
Affiliation:
Centro Nacional de Investigación sobre la Evolución Humana (CENIEH), Spain

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Book Reviews
Copyright
Copyright © 2015 the European Association of Archaeologists 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bruner, E., de la Cuétara, J.M., Masters, M., Amano, H. & Ogihara, N. 2014. Functional Craniology and Brain Evolution: From Paleontology to Biomedicine. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy, 8: 19.Google Scholar
Bruner, E. & Iriki, A. 2015. Extending Mind, Visuospatial Integration, and the Evolution of the Parietal Lobes in the Human Genus. Quaternary International, [online] DOI:10.1016/j.quaint.2015.05.019, [accessed 2 June 2015]. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1040618215005054.Google Scholar
Bruner, E., Manzi, G. & Arsuaga, J.L. 2003. Encephalization and Allometric Trajectories in the Genus Homo: Evidence from the Neandertal and Modern Lineages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 100: 15335–40.Google Scholar
Coolidge, F. & Wynn, T. 2005. Working Memory, its Executive Functions, and the Emergence of Modern Thinking. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 15: 526.Google Scholar
Coward, F. & Gamble, C. 2008. Big Brains, Small Worlds: Material Culture and the Evolution of the Mind. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, London, 363B: 1969–79.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R.I.M. 1992. Neocortex Size as a Constraint on Group Size in Primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 20: 469–93.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R.I.M. 1998. The Social Brain Hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 6: 178–90.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. 1989. Human Ethology. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter Inc.Google Scholar
Gunz, P., Neubauer, S., Maureille, B. & Hublin, J.J. 2010. Brain Development after Birth Differs between Neanderthals and Modern Humans. Current Biology, 20: R92122.Google Scholar
Iriki, A. & Taoka, M. 2012. Triadic (Ecological, Neural, Cognitive) Niche Construction: A Scenario of Human Brain Evolution Extrapolating Tool Use and Language from the Control of Reaching Actions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences, 367: 1023.Google Scholar
Machin, A.J. & Dunbar, R.I.M. 2011. The Brain Opiod Theory of Social Attachment: A Review of the Evidence. Behaviour, 148: 9851025.Google Scholar
Malafouris, L. 2010. The Brain-Artefact Interface (BAI): A Challenge for Archaeology and Cultural Neuroscience. Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience, 5: 264–73.Google Scholar
Pearce, E., Stringer, C. & Dunbar, R.I.M. 2013. New Insights into Differences in Brain Organization between Neanderthals and Anatomically Modern Humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B 280: 20130168. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.0168.Google Scholar