Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T11:35:14.790Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sensation-seeking-related DNA methylation and the development of delinquency: A longitudinal epigenome-wide study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 June 2022

Jacintha M. Tieskens*
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical, Neuro and Developmental Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam & Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Pol A. C. van Lier
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical, Neuro and Developmental Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam & Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
J. Marieke Buil
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical, Neuro and Developmental Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam & Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Research Center Urban Talent, Rotterdam University of Applied Sciences, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Edward D. Barker
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
*
Corresponding author: Jacintha M. Tieskens, email: j.m.tieskens@lumc.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Heightened sensation-seeking is related to the development of delinquency. Moreover, sensation-seeking, or biological correlates of sensation-seeking, are suggested as factors linking victimization to delinquency. Here, we focused on epigenetic correlates of sensation-seeking. First, we identified DNA methylation (DNAm) patterns related to sensation-seeking. Second, we investigated the association between sensation-seeking related DNAm and the development of delinquency. Third, we examined whether victimization was related to sensation-seeking related DNAm and the development of delinquency. Participants (N = 905; 49% boys) came from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. DNAm was assessed at birth, age 7 and age 15–17. Sensation-seeking (self-reports) was assessed at age 11 and 14. Delinquency (self-reports) was assessed at age 17–19. Sensation-seeking epigenome-wide association study revealed that no probes reached the critical significance level. However, 20 differential methylated probes reached marginal significance. With these 20 suggestive sites, a sensation-seeking cumulative DNAm risk score was created. Results showed that this DNAm risk score at age 15–17 was related to delinquency at age 17–19. Moreover, an indirect effect of victimization to delinquency via DNAm was found. Sensation-seeking related DNAm is a potential biological correlate that can help to understand the development of delinquency, including how victimization might be associated with adolescent delinquency.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that no alterations are made and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use and/or adaptation of the article.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Youth who show heightened sensation-seeking are at risk of developing delinquent behavior in adolescence (Harden et al., Reference Harden, Quinn and Tucker-Drob2012; Mann et al., Reference Mann, Kretsch, Tackett, Harden and Tucker-Drob2015). In addition, studies have indicated that heightened sensation-seeking is an important mediating factor in the link between environmental stressors, such as childhood victimization, and the development of delinquency (Choy et al., Reference Choy, Raine, Portnoy, Rudo-Hutt, Gao and Soyfer2015; Fagan et al.; Van Goozen et al., Reference Van Goozen, Fairchild, Snoek and Harold2007, Reference Van Goozen, Fairchild and Harold2008). It is suggested that (neuro)biological correlates of sensation-seeking, such as lowered autonomic arousal and disturbances in the dopaminergic system play a pivotal role in these associations (Van Goozen et al., Reference Van Goozen, Fairchild, Snoek and Harold2007, Reference Van Goozen, Fairchild and Harold2008). In the last years, epigenetic processes that regulate gene expression have emerged as another potential biological correlate that may explain the link between childhood risks, and behavioral maladjustment (Barker et al., Reference Barker, Walton and Cecil2018; Cecil et al., Reference Cecil, Zhang and Nolte2020). In this study, we focused on epigenetic correlates of sensation-seeking in three ways. First, we investigated whether children who show heightened sensation-seeking behavior have different DNA methylation patterns. Second, we investigated whether these DNA methylation patterns are related to the development of delinquency. And third, we examined whether these DNA methylation patterns were associated with earlier adverse childhood experiences (i.e., childhood victimization) and thereby, possibly, act as an indirect factor between childhood victimization and delinquency.

Sensation-seeking is defined as “the need for varied, novel and complex sensations and experiences and the willingness to take physical and social risks for the sake of such experiences” (Zuckerman, Reference Zuckerman1979). Previous studies have shown that individuals who show high levels of sensation-seeking during late childhood are at risk of developing delinquent behavior later in adolescence (Harden et al., Reference Harden, Quinn and Tucker-Drob2012; Mann et al., Reference Mann, Kretsch, Tackett, Harden and Tucker-Drob2015). It has been suggested that this sensation-seeking – delinquency association among adolescents might be due to biological correlates, such as lower resting state heart rate (Hammerton et al., Reference Hammerton, Heron, Mahedy, Maughan, Hickman and Murray2018; Ortiz & Raine, Reference Ortiz and Raine2004; Portnoy et al., Reference Portnoy, Raine, Chen, Pardini, Loeber and Jennings2014; Sijtsema et al., Reference Sijtsema, Veenstra, Lindenberg, van Roon, Verhulst, Ormel and Riese2010), dysregulated dopaminergic activation (Chester et al., Reference Chester, DeWall, Derefinko, Estus, Lynam, Peters and Jiang2016), increased gonadal hormone secretion (Aluja & Torrubia, Reference Aluja and Torrubia2004; Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Dreber, Apicella, Eisenberg, Gray, Little, Garcia, Zamore and Lum2010), altered reward-related brain activity (Gjedde et al., Reference Gjedde, Kumakura, Cumming, Linnet and Møller2010), and shared genetic influences (Mann et al., Reference Mann, Patterson, Grotzinger, Kretsch, Tackett, Tucker-Drob and Harden2016).

In the last years, epigenetic processes have emerged as another biological mechanism of interest in the development of mental health problems, including antisocial and delinquent behaviors (Barker et al., Reference Barker, Walton and Cecil2018; Tremblay, Reference Tremblay2015). One of these epigenetic processes, DNA methylation (DNAm), has received increasing attention. In short, DNAm is the process where a methyl group binds to the DNA, which in turn causes changes in gene expression without changing the sequence of the bases in the DNA itself. It has been shown that DNA methylation patterns are related to individual variation in delinquency as well as to other measures of antisocial behavior (Cecil, Walton, Jaffee, et al., Reference Cecil, Walton, Jaffee, O’Connor, Maughan, Relton, Smith, McArdle, Gaunt, Ouellet-Morin and Barker2018; Cecil, Walton, Pingault, et al., Reference Cecil, Walton, Pingault, Provençal, Pappa, Vitaro, Côté, Szyf, Tremblay, Tiemeier, Viding and McCrory2018; Checknita et al., Reference Checknita, Maussion, Labonté, Comai, Tremblay, Vitaro, Turecki, Bertazzo, Gobbi, Côté and Turecki2015; Guillemin et al., Reference Guillemin, Provencal, Suderman, Cote, Vitaro, Hallett and Szyf2014; Provencal et al., Reference Provencal, Suderman, Caramaschi, Wang, Hallett, Vitaro, Tremblay and Szyf2013; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Szyf, Benkelfat, Provençal, Turecki, Caramaschi, Côté, Vitaro, Tremblay and Booij2012). However, behavioral phenotypes, such as delinquency are complex and multiply determined. Barker et al. (Reference Barker, Walton and Cecil2018) therefore proposed that, to be able to identify biologically relevant biomarkers for behaviors such as delinquency, it may be fruitful to examine epigenetic correlates of antecedents of these phenotypes. To date, to our knowledge, there is no study into epigenetic correlates of sensation-seeking and its potential link with delinquency. However, given the clear sensation-seeking – delinquency link, this seems an important avenue to better understand this association. Therefore, in the present study we will investigate DNA methylation patterns that are related to heightened sensation-seeking behavior. Second, we will investigate whether these DNA methylation patterns are related to the development of delinquency.

Another important aspect of DNA methylation in relation to psychopathology is that DNA methylation is influenced both by genetic sequence variation (Gaunt et al., Reference Gaunt, Shihab, Hemani, Min, Woodward, Lyttleton, Zheng, Duggirala, McArdle, Ho, Ring, Evans, Smith and Relton2016) and environmental stressors, including childhood victimization (Cecil et al., Reference Cecil, Zhang and Nolte2020; Szyf et al., Reference Szyf, McGowan and Meaney2008). Previous studies have documented that childhood victimization is related to increases in sensation-seeking (Bornovalova et al., Reference Bornovalova, Gwadz, Kahler, Aklin and Lejuez2008), and to biological correlates of sensation-seeking such as lower heart rate (Miskovic et al., Reference Miskovic, Schmidt, Georgiades, Boyle and MacMillan2009), reduced cortisol (Lovallo, Reference Lovallo2013) and dopamine reward responses (Oswald et al., Reference Oswald, Wand, Kuwabara, Wong, Zhu and Brasic2014). However, it is not known whether childhood victimization is related to changes in DNAm correlates of sensation-seeking. Given the pivotal role of sensation-seeking in the link between childhood adversity and adolescent delinquency, our third goal is to investigate whether DNAm correlates of sensation-seeking are related to earlier adverse childhood experiences and whether this in turn is related to the development of delinquent behavior in late adolescence.

Thus, in this study we have three key research aims. First, we will investigate DNA methylation patterns of sensation-seeking. Second, we will examine whether there is an association between these DNA methylation patterns and delinquency in late adolescence. And our third research question is whether experiences of childhood victimization are related to sensation-seeking-related DNAm correlates and whether these adverse experiences via DNAm associate with the development of delinquent behavior later in adolescence.

Methods

Sample

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) is an ongoing epidemiological study of children born from 14,541 pregnant women residing in Avon, UK, with an expected delivery date between April 1st, 1991 and December 31st, 1992 (Boyd et al., Reference Boyd, Golding, Macleod, Lawlor, Fraser, Henderson, Molloy, Ness, Ring and Davey Smith2013; Fraser et al., Reference Fraser, Macdonald-Wallis, Tilling, Boyd, Golding, Davey Smith, Henderson, Macleod, Molloy, Ness, Ring, Nelson and Lawlor2013; Northstone et al., Reference Northstone, Lewcock, Groom, Boyd, Macleod, Timpson and Wells2019). The current study focuses on the Accessible Resource for Integrated Epigenomics Study substudy (Relton et al., Reference Relton, Gaunt, McArdle, Ho, Duggirala, Shihab, Woodward, Lyttleton, Evans, Reik, Paul, Ficz, Ozanne, Wipat, Flanagan, Lister, Heijmans, Ring and Smith2015), which consists of 1,018 mother-offspring pairs who provided DNA samples at multiple timepoints. Please note that the study website contains details of all the data that is available through a fully searchable data dictionary and variable search tool (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/our-data/). Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Committees. Informed consent for the use of data collected via questionnaires and clinics was obtained from participants following the recommendations of the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee at the time and consent for biological samples has been collected in accordance with the Human Tissue Act (2004). Children included in the present study had at least one available measure of victimization in early childhood (birth-age 7) or mid-childhood (age 8–9) (97% complete data), at least one available measure of DNAm at either birth, age 7 or age 15–17 (81% complete data), and at least one assessment of antisocial activities at age 8 or delinquency at age 17–19 (68% complete data). This resulted in a total sample of 905 children (49% boys, all Caucasian).

Measures

Sensation-seeking was assessed via self-reports using a modified version of the Arnett’s Inventory for Sensation Seeking (Arnett, Reference Arnett1994) at age 11 and age 14 including 19 items. Example items are “I like the feeling of standing next to the edge/looking down” or “When I ride a bike I go as fast as I can whenever possible.” Items were presented on a computer screen. Response categories were 0 = not at all like me, 1 = not much like me, 2 = quite like me, 3 = very like me. We performed an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) to reduce the items into smaller subsets of factors that showed shared variance. EFA on the modified version of the AISS revealed a three-factor structure which was used in the subsequent analyses to identify chronic high sensation-seekers and their associated DNAm patterns. See Table S1 for a description of the EFA.

Delinquent Behavior at 17–19 years of age was assessed with a variety score including 12 items regarding delinquent behavior that were taken from the Edinburgh Study of Youth Transition and Crime (Smith & McVie, Reference Smith and McVie2003). Participants were asked to answer questions about delinquent behavior such as “During last year I stole something from a shop” or “During last year I started a fight” with responses classified into “yes” (= 1) or “no” (= 0). All items were summed into an overall delinquency variety score (range 0–12).

Childhood victimization was obtained two times via mother reports about the period between child’s birth and age 7 and when the children were between the age of 8 and 9. Items about whether the child has been bullied (“child is bullied/picked on”), physically hurt by someone (“child has been physically hurt by someone”) or has been sexually abused (“child has been sexually abused”) were assessed plus two more specific items on peer victimization, namely, “child has been overtly victimized” and “child has been relationally victimized.” The scale was derived from an overall adversity score that was previously estimated and validated by Cecil et al. (Reference Cecil, Lysenko, Jaffee, Pingault, Smith, Relton, Woodward, McArdle, Mill and Barker2014).

DNA methylation data were extracted from cord blood on delivery, and from peripheral blood samples in childhood (age 7) and in adolescence (age 15–17). DNA methylation of over 450,000 CpG sites was quantified using Illumina Infinium HumanMethykation450K BeadChip assay (HM450; Illumina Inc., CA). Arrays were scanned using the Illumina iScan. To extract signal intensities and to assess initial quality review GenomeStudio was used. For each sample, the estimated methylation level at each CpG site is expressed as a beta value (β), which is the ratio of the methylated probe intensity to the overall intensity and ranges from 0 (no cytosine methylation) to 1 (complete cytosine methylation). Background correction and functional normalization were performed using Meffil in R (Min et al., Reference Min, Hemani, Davey Smith, Relton and Suderman2018). Samples with >10% of sites with a detection p value > .01 or a bead count < 3 in > 10% of probes were removed from further analysis. Nonspecific probes and probes on sex chromosomes were removed. Following QC procedures, data were available for 381,871 probes. Probes were annotated using information provided by Illumina (genome build: hg19). To account for potential differences in methylation arising from cell composition in whole-blood samples, cell counts were estimated using the Houseman algorithm and included as covariates. The cell counts in cord blood were estimated using the Gervin panel (Gervin et al., Reference Gervin, Salas, Bakulski, Van Zelm, Koestler, Wiencke, Duijts, Moll, Kelsey and Kobor2019). However, for longitudinal analyses including data from cord blood and whole-blood samples, analyses instead included the first 20 independent surrogate variable components to account for both heterogeneity between cord blood and peripheral blood samples as well as batch effects. Previous research has indicated that surrogate variables derived in this way account for cell count heterogeneity as well as estimated cell counts (Kaushal et al., Reference Kaushal, Zhang, Karmaus, Ray, Torres, Smith and Wang2017; McGregor et al., Reference McGregor, Bernatsky, Colmegna, Hudson, Pastinen, Labbe and Greenwood2016).

Control variables

Substance use was assessed and included as a control variable to prevent potential confounding of the DNAm levels by participant’s substance use (Dogan et al., Reference Dogan, Lei, Beach, Brody and Philibert2016). Tobacco and cannabis use was assessed with the Cannabis Abuse Screening Test at age 14 (Legleye et al., Reference Legleye, Piontek, Kraus, Morand and Falissard2013) and the Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence at age 14 (Heatherton et al., Reference Heatherton, Kozlowski, Frecker and Fagerstrom1991).

Antisocial activities at age 8 was assessed via 11 questions regarding antisocial activities that were taken from the self-reported antisocial behavior for young children questionnaire (Loeber et al., Reference Loeber, Stouthamer-Loeber, Van Kammen and Farrington1989). Items regarded stealing (bicycles, from a shop, from a house/garden, from a car, entered a building to steal, pick-pocketing), substance use (drank alcohol, smoked cigarettes without parental permission), set fire, carried a weapon and cruelty to animals. It was conducted as a structured interview and was provided in the format of a posting task. Each of the questions was written on a different envelope. The children were asked to place the envelope into one of the two boxes marked as “ever” or “never.” Scores were summed into a total antisocial activity score.

DNA methylation levels were corrected for maternal smoking, sex, batch and cell-type proportions (CD8 T-lymphocytes, CD4 T-lymphocytes, natural killer cells, B-lymphocytes, monocytes) at birth, age 7 and age 15–17 (Houseman et al., Reference Houseman, Accomando, Koestler, Christensen, Marsit, Nelson, Wiencke and Kelsey2012). Age 15–17 (M = 16 years, SD = 0.97)

Sex was dummy coded, 0 = boys; 1 = girls.

Statistical analyses

Our first research goal was to identify DNA methylation patterns related to sensation-seeking. To do so the following steps were undertaken. First, to identify children with a stable high sensation-seeking profile across ages 11 and 14 a Latent Transition Analysis (LTA) was performed. The objective of the LTA is to identify the smallest number of classes of children who may follow distinct profiles of sensation-seeking across ages 11 and 14 years (e.g., stable high versus lower levels of sensation-seeking across timepoints). Entropy and the percentage of youth in each profile (>5% children in each class) were used to come to the optimal number of classes. LTA was performed in Mplus (Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén1998-2012). We then performed an epigenome-wide association study (EWAS) of DNA methylation levels (at age 15–17) between children with a stable high sensation-seeking profile across age 11 and age 14 years versus all other children using a general linear model. Statistical significance was determined using a Bonferroni correction, giving a threshold of p < 1.3 × 10−7. Tests with p < 5 × 10−5 were defined as reaching suggestive significance (Roberts et al., Reference Roberts, Suderman, Zammit, Watkins, Hannon, Mill, Relton, Arseneault, Wong and Fisher2019). Methylation analyses were performed in R using the package Meffil (Min et al., Reference Min, Hemani, Davey Smith, Relton and Suderman2018). All (suggestive) hits will be examined for possible genetic influences on the levels of methylation by searching the mQTLdb from the GoDMC study (http://mqtldb.godmc.org.uk/).

Our second research goal was to investigate whether there is an association between these DNA methylation patterns and delinquency in late adolescence. To minimize multiple testing burden, we grouped the top probes (significant or suggestive significant probes) found in the EWAS into a single cumulative DNAm risk score. Therefore, we multiplied the methylation values by their respective regression weights form the EWAS, and then summed these weighted methylation values into the sensation-seeking DNAm risk score (Shah et al., Reference Shah, Bonder, Marioni, Zhu, McRae, Zhernakova, Harris, Liewald, Henders, Mendelson, Liu, Joehanes, Liang, Levy, Martin, Starr, Wijmenga, Wray, Yang and Visscher2015). Also, we calculated DNAm risk scores at birth and age 7 years using the same probes and weights from the DNAm risk score at age 15–17 years. This was done to enable studying change in DNAm risk across childhood into adolescence. We disentangled within-person variation from between-person variation in DNAm over time by specifying a random intercept for DNAm over the three timepoints (Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015). This enables us to assess changes in DNAm over time while accounting for static DNAm levels that differed between individuals at birth, age 7 and age 15–17. To investigate the association between these DNA methylation patterns and delinquency in late adolescence, we fitted a path model in Mplus in which delinquency at age 17–19 was predicted by DNAm at age 15–17, while controlling (both DNAm and delinquency) for antisocial activities at age 8, substance use and sex.

Our third research question was whether experiences of childhood victimization are related to sensation-seeking DNAm correlates and whether these adverse experiences via DNAm associate with the development of delinquent behavior later in adolescence. Therefore, a model testing indirect effects as depicted in Figure 1 was fitted. We included childhood victimization in the model and allowed for paths of childhood victimization at age 8–9 to the (changes in) DNAm risk score at age 15–17 and subsequent delinquency at age 17–19. In this model, we controlled for antisocial activities at age 8, childhood victimization between birth - age 7, substance use and stable DNAm risk score differences over time between participants. Bootstrapped (10,000 times) estimates of the indirect effect of childhood victimization via DNAm change to delinquency were estimated with bias-corrected 95% confidence intervals (CIs).

Figure 1. Graphical representation of the model including regression path estimates of the indirect effect of childhood victimization via DNAm changes to delinquency. Note. All other estimates can be found in Table 3. For sake of simplicity control paths for age, sex and substance use are not represented in this figure. * = p < .05.

All models were fitted in Mplus version 7.1. Model fit was determined through the CFI and TLI (acceptable fit = > 0.90; Bentler & Bonett, Reference Bentler and Bonett1980) and RMSEA (acceptable fit = < 0.08; Marsh et al., Reference Marsh, Hau and Wen2004). Maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors was used to estimate the model parameters, and missing data were handled through full information maximum likelihood estimation.

Results

Preliminary analyses

To identify the children with stable high sensation-seeking profiles over time, an LTA was performed and yielded a three class-profile solution. Among those profiles, there was a discernable group of children with stable high sensation-seeking at both ages 11 and age 14 years. This high sensation-seeking class (15%) consisted of n = 140 children and was contrasted in the EWAS with the other children (n = 765). For a detailed description of the LTA see Table S2.

Epigenome-wide association analysis of sensation-seeking

Results from the EWAS showed that that none of the probes reached the critical significance level of p < 1.3 × 10−7. However, 20 methylation probes were nominally significantly (p < 5 × 10−5, depicted in Table 1) differentially methylated in children with stable high sensation-seeking scores compared to the rest of the sample. See Table S3 for detailed information about the exact positions and the effects of the probes. These probes were annotated to 12 genes, see Table S3. The most strongly associated probe, cg16495212, is located in CPEB1, which is broadly expressed in brain tissue (see Table S4) and plays an important role in synaptic efficacy, plasticity and hippocampal-dependent learning (Ivshina et al., Reference Ivshina, Lasko and Richter2014). CPEB1 is also broadly expressed in the uterus and testis, important tissues for the production of gonadal hormones, which have been implicated in reward sensitivity (Harden et al., Reference Harden, Mann, Grotzinger, Patterson, Steinberg, Tackett and Tucker-Drob2018) and in the dopaminergic system (Kuhn et al., Reference Kuhn, Johnson, Thomae, Luo, Simon, Zhou and Walker2010). Other annotated genes include TEKT1(cg12685753) and SNED1(cg08233654), also both predominantly expressed in the testis and uterus; WSCD1 (cg04869532), broadly expressed in the brain and TNXB (cg19609334) and PRR14 (cg00879206) two genes that have been implicated in risk-taking behavior in a previous genome wide analysis study (Linnér et al., Reference Linnér, Biroli, Kong, Meddens, Wedow, Fontana, Lebreton, Abdellaoui, Hammerschlag, Nivard, Okbay, Rietveld, Timshel, Tino, Trzaskowski, de Vlaming, Zünd, Bao, Buzdugan, Caplin and Beauchamp2018). To investigate whether the DMPs were located on regulatory sites of the chromatin, DMPs were uploaded in Genome Browser for functional characterization, based on ENCODE data on regulatory elements (http://genome.ucsc.edu/ENCODE/). All DMPs overlapped with histone marks; 55% coincide with transcription factor binding sites; and 80% were located within DNAse I hypersensitive clusters. Overall, 40% of DMPs were mapped to all three regulatory elements, see Table S3, indicating that a great part of the DMPs have a functional relevance. After running the suggestive hits through the mQTL database of the GoDMC consortium study, 60% of the suggestive hits showed evidence to be associated with known mQTL.

Table 1. EWAS top 20 probes: Methylation at age 15–17 and sensation-seeking profile (high vs. others)

Sensation-seeking DNAm patterns and the development of delinquency

Sensation-seeking-related cumulative DNAm risk scores were computed based on the 20 suggestive hits (i.e., the probed that came most close to the critical value of p < 1.3 × 10−7, which were all probes with p < 5 × 10−5). Correlations between the cumulative DNAm risk score and all other study variables are presented in Table 2. To test the association of sensation-seeking-related DNA methylation patterns and late adolescent delinquency, a path model was fitted. We included the DNAm risk scores at birth, age 7 and age 15–17 years in the model to be able to assess change in DNAm over time. Next, we included delinquency at age 17–19, our outcome measure, and antisocial activities at age 8 years, to be able to control for possible reverse effects. Pathways were controlled for sex and substance use. This model showed good fit to the data: χ2(11) = 18.615, p = .07; CFI = 0.978; TLI = 0.940; RMSEA = 0.028, 90% CI [0.000–0.049]. Results showed that DNAm risk scores at age 15–17 years were related to delinquency at age 17–19 years, with a small effect size (β = 0.146, B = 0.042, SE = 0.015, p =.007).

Table 2. Correlations between study variables

Note. * p < .05; ** p < .01.

Sensation-seeking DNAm patterns linking childhood victimization to adolescent delinquency

Our third research question focused on the link between childhood victimization – and sensation-seeking DNAm correlates and the possible indirect association between childhood victimization, sensation-seeking DNAm correlates and delinquency. Childhood victimization between age 8–9 years was added to the model (see Figure 1). Results of all path estimates are presented in Table 3. This model showed adequate fit to the data: χ2(21) = 41.317, p =.005; CFI = 0.961; TLI = 0.909; RMSEA = 0.033, 90% CI [0.018–0.047]. Results show that the exposure to childhood victimization around age 8–9 years was associated with increases in DNAm risk score at age 15–17 years, with a small effect size (β = 0.082, B = 0.31, SE = 0.14, p = .02), which, in turn, was associated with increased delinquency at age 17–19 years (B = 0.04, SE = 0.02, β = 0.141, p < .01). The indirect effect of victimization to delinquency via change in sensation-seeking-related DNAm risk score was significant (B = 0.012; 95% CI [0.002–0.036]. For a graphical representation of the results, see Figure 1. For all path estimates in the model, see Table 3.

Table 3. Path estimates of the full indirect effect model including study and control variables

Note.p < .10, * p < .05, ** p < .01.

Discussion

This study was set out to identify DNAm patterns related to sensation-seeking, to investigate the association between these DNAm patterns with delinquency in late adolescence, and to investigate whether experiences of childhood victimization were related to sensation-seeking DNAm patterns and the development of delinquent behavior in adolescence. Key findings of the present study were threefold. First, the EWAS on sensation-seeking did not reveal significant probes but we followed up on 20 suggestive sites that showed up to be marginally significantly differential methylated in children who show stable high sensation-seeking at age 11 and age14. Second, these differential methylated loci, collectively, were related to the development of delinquent behavior in late adolescence. Third, early experiences of childhood victimization were associated with these sensation-seeking-related DNA methylation patterns and indirectly with the development of delinquency.

Epigenetic variation associated with sensation-seeking

To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the epigenomic profile of children who show stable high sensation-seeking across time. The EWAS revealed no differential methylated loci reaching p < 1.3 × 10−7. However, 20 suggestive hits reaching p < 5 × 10−5 were identified in children who show stable high sensation-seeking behavior. These loci were annotated to 12 different genes and were related to a range of biological processes, including neural processes related to reward sensitivity. The locus with the highest difference in methylation levels between high versus non-high sensations seekers was CPEB1. CPEB1 is highly expressed in brain tissue, including regions implicated in reward-seeking behavior (e.g., in the nucleus accumbens, substantia nigra and the hippocampus). This finding is in accordance with the found link between reward sensitivity and sensation-seeking behavior (Gjedde et al., Reference Gjedde, Kumakura, Cumming, Linnet and Møller2010) and might indicate that differential methylation of this gene underlies differences in reward sensitivity. Other relevant differentially methylated loci were located at TEKT1 (cg12685753) and SNED1(cg08233654) that are broadly expressed in the testis and uterus, which are important tissues for gonadal hormone production and transmission. Dysregulations of these gonadal hormones, such as testosterone, have been linked to sensation-seeking behavior before (Aluja & Torrubia, Reference Aluja and Torrubia2004; Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Dreber, Apicella, Eisenberg, Gray, Little, Garcia, Zamore and Lum2010). However, those functional annotations have to be interpreted with caution. The specific functionality of those genes was not the focus of the current study and to understand the specific biological function of those genes as well as their interaction and the link with sensation-seeking behavior, more in depth research into the functionality of those genes is needed.

Sensation-seeking-related DNA methylation patterns, delinquency and childhood victimization

To investigate the potential role of these sensation-seeking-related DNA methylation patterns in a developmental perspective, we focused our research on two pathways of interest. First, we showed that changes in DNA methylation patterns between age 7 and age 15–17 related to sensation-seeking, are associated with the development of delinquency in late adolescence. Second, we showed that early experiences of childhood victimization are associated with changes in those sensation-seeking-related DNA methylation patterns. Moreover, we found that childhood victimization was indirectly related to delinquency via changes in those sensation-seeking-related DNAm patterns. The finding that a biological correlate of sensation-seeking is related to the development of delinquency is in line with previous research where the link between sensation-seeking and delinquency is shown (Harden et al., Reference Harden, Quinn and Tucker-Drob2012; Mann et al., Reference Mann, Kretsch, Tackett, Harden and Tucker-Drob2015). In addition, the finding that early adverse childhood experiences are related to a biological correlate of sensation-seeking is also in line with previous research where the association between childhood victimization and sensation-seeking have been documented (Bornovalova et al., Reference Bornovalova, Gwadz, Kahler, Aklin and Lejuez2008). In this study, we provide evidence for the proposed developmental pathway from childhood victimization via sensation-seeking to delinquency (Van Goozen et al., Reference Van Goozen, Fairchild and Harold2008).

Moreover, we extended earlier findings by showing that these associations might also be present at the epigenetic level. Especially the finding that DNA methylation correlates of sensation-seeking are related to both a relevant environmental factor and a developmental maladaptive outcome shows that these DNA methylation patterns are a promising avenue for further research into the working mechanisms behind the possible cascadic events of childhood victimization, via sensation-seeking to the development of delinquency (Van Goozen et al., Reference Van Goozen, Fairchild and Harold2008). Further research into the specific biological mechanisms related to the identified probes here will generate important information on the working mechanisms behind the link between sensation-seeking, childhood victimization and delinquency.

Limitations and future directions

This study has several limitations that should be considered while interpreting the results. First, the results of our study are based on a modestly sized population-based sample of youth and to be able to test the robustness of our findings replication on other samples is needed. However, to date there is, to our knowledge no other sample that assess DNA methylation in relation to sensation-seeking in the same age period as we did. This led to another limitation, that we were not able to use external weights to calculate our sensation-seeking DNAm risk scores (Hüls & Czamara, Reference Hüls and Czamara2020) which can increase the risk of the observed association reported being influenced by overfitting. As such, our findings should be considered as hypothesis-generating and replication studies are needed to draw more conclusive interpretations. Second, it should be kept in mind that our findings are based on DNAm from peripheral samples. For obvious reasons, we could not test whether the DNAm results found in blood samples mirrored those in brain tissues. However, Hannon et al. (Reference Hannon, Lunnon, Schalkwyk and Mill2015) showed that a considerable proportion of DNAm sites show crosstissue concordance. Third, in this study we assessed a variety score of delinquent acts and we did not investigate frequency of delinquency. Although variety and frequency scores are thought to be highly concordant, specific (environmental) factors may have different influences on variety versus frequency scores (Monahan & Piquero, Reference Monahan and Piquero2009) and future studies may want to investigate whether childhood victimization is also related to the frequency of delinquent acts via sensation-seeking and DNA methylation changes. In addition, here we show a specific link between a possible sensation-seeking-related biomarker and delinquency. However, the sensation-seeking DNAm risk score might also be related to the development of various other forms of impulse control behaviors such as for example oppositional behavior, or even general risk-taking behavior or mild rule-breaking behavior such as truancy. Future research is important to further unravel the possible role of the sensation-seeking biomarker in a range of impulse control behaviors and mild deviancy throughout adolescence. Fourth, 60% of the suggestive hits found in this study are associated with known mQTL and consequently likely to be under significant genetic control. The identified SNPs are important to further investigate in relation to sensation-seeking and delinquency, in order to disentangle potential genetic and environmental influences on the development of delinquency (via sensation-seeking). We note that we were unpowered to carry out such analyses. Fifth, despite the fact that we used longitudinal data and our DNAm is measured prospectively to delinquency measures and was controlled for potential reverse effects of prior antisocial behavior on DNAm changes, it is not possible to establish causality in the course of events. Finally, we focused exclusively on DNA methylation, while other epigenetics processes, such as histone modification are also likely to play a role.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579422000049.

Acknowledgments

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses.

Funding statement

The UK Medical Research Council and Wellcome (Grant ref: 217065/Z/19/Z) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. A comprehensive list of grants funding is available on the ALSPAC website (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/external/documents/grant-acknowledgements.pdf). The research presented in this study was specifically supported by National Institute of Child Health and Human Development of the National institutes of Health Grant R01HD068437 (Dr. Barker), European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement No707404 (dr. Cecil), and Economic and Social Research Council Grants ES/N001273/1 (dr. Cecil.) and ES/R005516/1 (dr. Barker). The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

References

Aluja, A., & Torrubia, R. (2004). Hostility-aggressiveness, sensation seeking, and sex hormones in men: re-exploring their relationship. Neuropsychobiology, 50, 102107. https://doi.org/10.1159/000077947 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnett, J. (1994). Sensation seeking: A new conceptualization and a new scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 289296. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(94)90165-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barker, E. D., Walton, E., & Cecil, C. A. (2018). Annual Research Review: DNA methylation as a mediator in the association between risk exposure and child and adolescent psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59, 303322. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12782 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.88.3.588 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornovalova, M. A., Gwadz, M. A., Kahler, C., Aklin, W. M., & Lejuez, C. (2008). Sensation seeking and risk-taking propensity as mediators in the relationship between childhood abuse and HIV-related risk behavior. Child Abuse & Neglect, 32, 99109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.04.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, A., Golding, J., Macleod, J., Lawlor, D. A., Fraser, A., Henderson, J., Molloy, L., Ness, A., Ring, S., & Davey Smith, G. (2013). Cohort profile: the ‘children of the 90s’—the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42, 111127. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dys064 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, B. C., Dreber, A., Apicella, C. L., Eisenberg, D. T., Gray, P. B., Little, A. C., Garcia, J. R., Zamore, R. S., & Lum, J. K. (2010). Testosterone exposure, dopaminergic reward, and sensation-seeking in young men. Physiology & Behavior, 99, 451456. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.12.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cecil, C. A., Lysenko, L. J., Jaffee, S. R., Pingault, J.-B., Smith, R. G., Relton, C. L., Woodward, G., McArdle, W., Mill, J., & Barker, E. D. (2014). Environmental risk, Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR) methylation and youth callous-unemotional traits: A 13-year longitudinal study. Molecular Psychiatry, 19, 10711077. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2014.95 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cecil, C. A., Walton, E., Jaffee, S. R., O’Connor, T., Maughan, B., Relton, C. L., Smith, R. G., McArdle, W., Gaunt, T. R., Ouellet-Morin, I., & Barker, E. D. (2018). Neonatal DNA methylation and early-onset conduct problems: A genome-wide, prospective study. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 383397. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941700092X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cecil, C. A., Walton, E., Pingault, J. B., Provençal, N., Pappa, I., Vitaro, F., Côté, S., Szyf, M., Tremblay, R. E., Tiemeier, H., Viding, E., & McCrory, E. J. (2018). DRD4 methylation as a potential biomarker for physical aggression: An epigenome-wide, cross-tissue investigation. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 177, 746764. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32689 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cecil, C. A., Zhang, Y., & Nolte, T. (2020). Childhood maltreatment and DNA methylation: A systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 112, 392409. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.02.019 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Checknita, D., Maussion, G., Labonté, B., Comai, S., Tremblay, R., Vitaro, F., Turecki, N., Bertazzo, A., Gobbi, G., Côté, G., & Turecki, G. (2015). Monoamine oxidase A gene promoter methylation and transcriptional downregulation in an offender population with antisocial personality disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 206, 216222. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.114.144964 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chester, D. S., DeWall, C. N., Derefinko, K. J., Estus, S., Lynam, D. R., Peters, J. R., & Jiang, Y. (2016). Looking for reward in all the wrong places: Dopamine receptor gene polymorphisms indirectly affect aggression through sensation-seeking. Social Neuroscience, 11, 487494. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2015.1119191 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choy, O., Raine, A., Portnoy, J., Rudo-Hutt, A., Gao, Y., & Soyfer, L. (2015). The mediating role of heart rate on the social adversity-antisocial behavior relationship: A social neurocriminology perspective. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 52, 303341. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022427814565905 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dogan, M. V., Lei, M.-K., Beach, S. R., Brody, G. H., & Philibert, R. A. (2016). Alcohol and tobacco consumption alter hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis DNA methylation. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 66, 176184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.01.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fagan, S. E., Zhang, W., & Gao, Y. (2017). Social adversity and antisocial behavior: mediating effects of autonomic nervous system activity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45, 15531564. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-017-0262-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraser, A., Macdonald-Wallis, C., Tilling, K., Boyd, A., Golding, J., Davey Smith, G., Henderson, J., Macleod, J., Molloy, L., Ness, A., Ring, S., Nelson, S. M., & Lawlor, D. A. (2013). Cohort profile: The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42, 97110. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dys066 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaunt, T. R., Shihab, H. A., Hemani, G., Min, J. L., Woodward, G., Lyttleton, O., Zheng, J., Duggirala, A., McArdle, W. L., Ho, K., Ring, S. M., Evans, D. M., Smith, G. D., & Relton, C. L. (2016). Systematic identification of genetic influences on methylation across the human life course. Genome Biology, 17, 114. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13059-016-0926-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gervin, K., Salas, L. A., Bakulski, K. M., Van Zelm, M. C., Koestler, D. C., Wiencke, J. K., Duijts, L., Moll, H.A., Kelsey, K.T., & Kobor, M. S. (2019). Systematic evaluation and validation of reference and library selection methods for deconvolution of cord blood DNA methylation data. Clinical Epigenetics, 11, 115. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13148-019-0717-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gjedde, A., Kumakura, Y., Cumming, P., Linnet, J., & Møller, A. (2010). Inverted-U-shaped correlation between dopamine receptor availability in striatum and sensation seeking. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 38703875. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0912319107 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guillemin, C., Provencal, N., Suderman, M., Cote, S. M., Vitaro, F., Hallett, M., Tremblay, R. E., & Szyf, M. (2014). DNA methylation signature of childhood chronic physical aggression in T cells of both men and women. PloS One, 9, e86822. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0086822 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamaker, E. L., Kuiper, R. M., & Grasman, R. P. (2015). A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychological Methods, 20, 102116. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038889 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammerton, G., Heron, J., Mahedy, L., Maughan, B., Hickman, M., & Murray, J. (2018). Low resting heart rate, sensation seeking and the course of antisocial behaviour across adolescence and young adulthood. Psychological Medicine, 48, 21942201. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717003683 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hannon, E., Lunnon, K., Schalkwyk, L., & Mill, J. (2015). Interindividual methylomic variation across blood, cortex, and cerebellum: Implications for epigenetic studies of neurological and neuropsychiatric phenotypes. Epigenetics, 10, 10241032. https://doi.org/10.1080/15592294.2015.1100786 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harden, K. P., Mann, F. D., Grotzinger, A. D., Patterson, M. W., Steinberg, L., Tackett, J. L., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2018). Developmental differences in reward sensitivity and sensation seeking in adolescence: Testing sex-specific associations with gonadal hormones and pubertal development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 115, 161178. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000172 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harden, K. P., Quinn, P. D., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2012). Genetically influenced change in sensation seeking drives the rise of delinquent behavior during adolescence. Developmental Science, 15, 150163. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01115.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heatherton, T. F., Kozlowski, L. T., Frecker, R. C., & Fagerstrom, K. O. (1991). The Fagerström test for nicotine dependence: A revision of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. British Journal of Addiction, 86, 11191127. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01879.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Houseman, E. A., Accomando, W. P., Koestler, D. C., Christensen, B. C., Marsit, C. J., Nelson, H. H., Wiencke, J. K., & Kelsey, K. T. (2012). DNA methylation arrays as surrogate measures of cell mixture distribution. BMC Bioinformatics, 13, 86103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hüls, A., & Czamara, D. (2020). Methodological challenges in constructing DNA methylation risk scores. Epigenetics, 15, 111. https://doi.org/10.1080/15592294.2019.1644879 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ivshina, M., Lasko, P., & Richter, J. D. (2014). Cytoplasmic polyadenylation element binding proteins in development, health, and disease. Annual Review of Cell and Developmental Biology, 30, 393415. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-cellbio-101011-155831 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaushal, A., Zhang, H., Karmaus, W. J., Ray, M., Torres, M. A., Smith, A. K., & Wang, S.-L. (2017). Comparison of different cell type correction methods for genome-scale epigenetics studies. BMC Bioinformatics, 18, 216. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12859-017-1611-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, C., Johnson, M., Thomae, A., Luo, B., Simon, S. A., Zhou, G., & Walker, Q. D. (2010). The emergence of gonadal hormone influences on dopaminergic function during puberty. Hormones and Behavior, 58, 122137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.10.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legleye, S., Piontek, D., Kraus, L., Morand, E., & Falissard, B. (2013). A validation of the Cannabis Abuse Screening Test (CAST) using a latent class analysis of the DSM-IV among adolescents. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 22, 1626. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.1378 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linnér, R. K., Biroli, P., Kong, E., Meddens, S. F. W., Wedow, R., Fontana, M. A., Lebreton, M., Abdellaoui, A., Hammerschlag, A. R., Nivard, M. G., Okbay, A., Rietveld, C. A., Timshel, P. N., Tino, S. P., Trzaskowski, M., de Vlaming, R., Zünd, C., Bao, Y., Buzdugan, L., Caplin, A. H., … Beauchamp, J. P. (2018). Genome-wide study identifies 611 loci associated with risk tolerance and risky behaviors. BioRxiv, 261081. https://doi.org/10.1101/261081 Google Scholar
Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., Van Kammen, W. B., & Farrington, D. P. (1989). Development of a new measure of self-reported antisocial behavior for young children: Prevalence and reliability. In M. W. Klein (Ed.), Cross-national research in self-reported crime and delinquency (pp. 203225). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-1001-0_10 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovallo, W. R. (2013). Early life adversity reduces stress reactivity and enhances impulsive behavior: Implications for health behaviors. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 90, 816. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.10.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, F. D., Kretsch, N., Tackett, J. L., Harden, K. P., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2015). Person× environment interactions on adolescent delinquency: Sensation seeking, peer deviance and parental monitoring. Personality and Individual Differences, 76, 129134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.11.055 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, F. D., Patterson, M. W., Grotzinger, A. D., Kretsch, N., Tackett, J. L., Tucker-Drob, E. M., & Harden, K. P. (2016). Sensation seeking, peer deviance, and genetic influences on adolescent delinquency: Evidence for person-environment correlation and interaction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125, 679. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000160 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, H. W., Hau, K.-T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules: Comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler’s (1999) findings. Structural Equation Modeling, 11, 320341. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem1103_2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, K., Bernatsky, S., Colmegna, I., Hudson, M., Pastinen, T., Labbe, A., & Greenwood, C. M. (2016). An evaluation of methods correcting for cell-type heterogeneity in DNA methylation studies. Genome Biology, 17, 84. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13059-016-0935-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Min, J. L., Hemani, G., Davey Smith, G., Relton, C., & Suderman, M. (2018). Meffil: Efficient normalization and analysis of very large DNA methylation datasets. Bioinformatics, 34, 39833989. https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/bty476 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miskovic, V., Schmidt, L. A., Georgiades, K., Boyle, M., & MacMillan, H. L. (2009). Stability of resting frontal electroencephalogram (EEG) asymmetry and cardiac vagal tone in adolescent females exposed to child maltreatment. Developmental Psychobiology, 51, 474487. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20387 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monahan, K. C., & Piquero, A. R. (2009). Investigating the longitudinal relation between offending frequency and offending variety. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 36, 653673. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854809335527 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B.O. (1998–2012). Mplus user’s guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Northstone, K., Lewcock, M., Groom, A., Boyd, A., Macleod, J., Timpson, N., & Wells, N. (2019). The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC): An update on the enrolled sample of index children in 2019. Wellcome Open Research, 4, 51. https://doi.org/10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15132.1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ortiz, J., & Raine, A. (2004). Heart rate level and antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 154162. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200402000-00010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oswald, L. M., Wand, G. S., Kuwabara, H., Wong, D. F., Zhu, S., & Brasic, J. R. (2014). History of childhood adversity is positively associated with ventral striatal dopamine responses to amphetamine. Psychopharmacology, 231, 24172433. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-013-3407-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Portnoy, J., Raine, A., Chen, F. R., Pardini, D., Loeber, R., & Jennings, J. R. (2014). Heart rate and antisocial behavior: The mediating role of impulsive sensation seeking. Criminology, 52, 292311. https://doi.org/10.1111/1745-9125.12038 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Provencal, N., Suderman, M. J., Caramaschi, D., Wang, D., Hallett, M., Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R. E., & Szyf, M. (2013). Differential DNA methylation regions in cytokine and transcription factor genomic loci associate with childhood physical aggression. PloS One, 8, e71691. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071691 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Relton, C. L., Gaunt, T., McArdle, W., Ho, K., Duggirala, A., Shihab, H., Woodward, G., Lyttleton, O., Evans, D. M., Reik, W., Paul, Y.-L., Ficz, G., Ozanne, S. E., Wipat, A., Flanagan, K., Lister, A., Heijmans, B. T., Ring, S. M., & Smith, G. D. (2015). Data resource profile: accessible resource for integrated epigenomic studies (ARIES). International Journal of Epidemiology, 44, 11811190. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyv072 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, S., Suderman, M., Zammit, S., Watkins, S. H., Hannon, E., Mill, J., Relton, C., Arseneault, L., Wong, C. C. Y., & Fisher, H. L. (2019). Longitudinal investigation of DNA methylation changes preceding adolescent psychotic experiences. Translational Psychiatry, 9, 112. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-019-0407-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shah, S., Bonder, M. J., Marioni, R. E., Zhu, Z., McRae, A. F., Zhernakova, A., Harris, S. E., Liewald, D., Henders, A. K., Mendelson, M. M., Liu, C., Joehanes, R., Liang, L., BIOS Consortium, Levy, D., Martin, N. G., Starr, J. M., Wijmenga, C., Wray, N. R., Yang, J.Visscher, P. M. (2015). Improving phenotypic prediction by combining genetic and epigenetic associations. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 97, 7585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2015.05.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sijtsema, J. J., Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., van Roon, A. M., Verhulst, F. C., Ormel, J., & Riese, H. (2010). Mediation of sensation seeking and behavioral inhibition on the relationship between heart rate and antisocial behavior: The TRAILS study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 493502. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2010.02.005 Google ScholarPubMed
Smith, D. J., & McVie, S. (2003). Theory and method in the Edinburgh study of youth transitions and crime. British Journal of Criminology, 43, 169195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szyf, M., McGowan, P., & Meaney, M. J. (2008). The social environment and the epigenome. Environmental and Molecular Mutagenesis, 49, 4660. https://doi.org/10.1002/em.20357 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tremblay, R. E. (2015). Developmental origins of chronic physical aggression: An international perspective on using singletons, twins and epigenetics. European Journal of Criminology, 12, 551561. https://doi.org/10.1177/1477370815600617 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Goozen, S. H., Fairchild, G., & Harold, G. T. (2008). The role of neurobiological deficits in childhood antisocial behavior. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 224228. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00579.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Goozen, S. H., Fairchild, G., Snoek, H., & Harold, G. T. (2007). The evidence for a neurobiological model of childhood antisocial behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 149. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.149 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, D., Szyf, M., Benkelfat, C., Provençal, N., Turecki, G., Caramaschi, D., Côté, S. M., Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R. E., & Booij, L. (2012). Peripheral SLC6A4 DNA methylation is associated with in vivo measures of human brain serotonin synthesis and childhood physical aggression. PloS One, 7, e39501. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0039501 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking and risk taking. In C. E. Izard (Ed.), Emotions in personality and psychopathology (pp. 161197). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-2892-6_7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Graphical representation of the model including regression path estimates of the indirect effect of childhood victimization via DNAm changes to delinquency. Note. All other estimates can be found in Table 3. For sake of simplicity control paths for age, sex and substance use are not represented in this figure. * = p < .05.

Figure 1

Table 1. EWAS top 20 probes: Methylation at age 15–17 and sensation-seeking profile (high vs. others)

Figure 2

Table 2. Correlations between study variables

Figure 3

Table 3. Path estimates of the full indirect effect model including study and control variables

Supplementary material: File

Tieskens et al. supplementary material

Tieskens et al. supplementary material
Download Tieskens et al. supplementary material(File)
File 28.6 KB