Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T20:55:21.885Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The roles of familial transmission and smoking during pregnancy on executive function skills: A sibling-comparison study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 August 2022

Valerie S. Knopik*
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Purdue University, 1202 West State St, West Lafayette, USA, IN, 47906
Lauren Micalizzi
Affiliation:
Center for Alcohol and Addiction Studies, Brown University, Box G-S121-5 Providence, RI, 02903, USA Department of Behavioral and Social Sciences, Box G-S121-5, Brown University School of Public Health, Providence, RI, 02912, USA
Kristine Marceau
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Purdue University, 1202 West State St, West Lafayette, USA, IN, 47906
Amy M. Loviska
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Purdue University, 1202 West State St, West Lafayette, USA, IN, 47906
Li Yu
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Purdue University, 1202 West State St, West Lafayette, USA, IN, 47906
Alexandra Bien
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Purdue University, 1202 West State St, West Lafayette, USA, IN, 47906
Emily Rolan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, 316 Physics Rd., East Lansing, MI, 48823, USA
Allison S. Evans
Affiliation:
Concord Comprehensive Neuropsychological Services, 86 Baker Avenue Extension #301, Concord, MA, 01742, USA
Rohan H. C. Palmer
Affiliation:
Behavioral Genetics of Addiction Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Emory University, 36 Eagle Row, Atlanta, GA, 30322, USA
Andrew C. Heath
Affiliation:
Midwest Alcoholism Research Center, Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, 660 S Euclid Ave, St. Louis, MO, 63110, USA
*
Corresponding author: Dr. Valerie Knopik, email: vknopik@purdue.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This research examines maternal smoking during pregnancy and risk for poorer executive function in siblings discordant for exposure. Data (N = 173 families) were drawn from the Missouri Mothers and Their Children study, a sample, identified using birth records (years 1998–2005), in which mothers changed smoking behavior between two pregnancies (Child 1 [older sibling]: Mage = 12.99; Child 2 [younger sibling]: Mage = 10.19). A sibling comparison approach was used, providing a robust test for the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and different aspects of executive function in early-mid adolescence. Results suggested within-family (i.e., potentially causal) associations between maternal smoking during pregnancy and one working memory task (visual working memory) and one response inhibition task (color-word interference), with increased exposure associated with decreased performance. Maternal smoking during pregnancy was not associated with stop-signal reaction time, cognitive flexibility/set-shifting, or auditory working memory. Initial within-family associations between maternal smoking during pregnancy and visual working memory as well as color-word interference were fully attenuated in a model including child and familial covariates. These findings indicate that exposure to maternal smoking during pregnancy may be associated with poorer performance on some, but not all skills assessed; however, familial transmission of risk for low executive function appears more important.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

EF is an umbrella term that involves a series of cognitive processes essential for intentional, goal-directed behaviors (Anderson, Reference Anderson2002; Gioia et al., Reference Gioia, Isquith and Guy2001; Rabinovici et al., Reference Rabinovici, Stephens and Possin2015). Processes of EF include anticipation, planning, behavior initiation, self-regulation, mental flexibility, deployment of attention, and utilization of feedback (Anderson, Reference Anderson2002). These processes have been further categorized into three subdomains of EF: (1) inhibition control (also known as response inhibition), which allows individuals to cognitively inhibit information and behavioral responses that are unrelated to the current task; (2) set-shifting (also called cognitive flexibility), which is the function of flexibly shifting attention between multiple tasks (Shields et al., Reference Shields, Bonner and Moons2015); and (3) working memory, which enables individuals to integrate new information into existing information networks.

EF develops throughout childhood and adolescence (Anderson, Reference Anderson2002), and maturation of brain regions, such as prefrontal cortex and inferior parietal regions, set the foundation of EF development (Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Mohamed, Carmody, Malik, Faro and Lewis2013; Grattan & Eslinger, Reference Grattan and Eslinger1991). Consequently, both function and dysfunction of executive processes depend on factors that can alter the structure and functioning of related brain regions. These factors include, but are not limited to, genetic influences and exposures (such as maternal smoking during pregnancy) that might perturb various neurotransmitter systems in the brain (Barnes et al., Reference Barnes, Dean, Nandam, O'Connell and Bellgrove2011).

Etiology of individual differences in executive function

Individual differences in quantitative measures of EF are strongly driven by familial and genetic differences and many clinical outcomes marked by executive deficits are highly heritable (e.g., ADHD, dyslexia). Twin studies suggest a heritable latent (common) factor that affects all EF subdomains and accounts for 99% of the variance common to all three skills (Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Miyake, Young, DeFries, Corley and Hewitt2008). Specific EF skills (i.e., inhibition, set-shifting, and working memory) also generally show moderate heritability, with ranges between 25 and 81% depending on assessment, age, and sex of the sample (e.g., Anokhin et al., Reference Anokhin, Golosheykin, Grant and Heath2010; Coolidge et al., Reference Coolidge, Thede and Young2000; Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Miyake, Young, DeFries, Corley and Hewitt2008; Gustavson et al., Reference Gustavson, Reynolds, Corley, Wadsworth, Hewitt and Friedman2022; Li & Roberts, Reference Li and Roberts2017); noting, however, that reports of heritability of EF skills in toddlers are mixed (e.g., Leve et al., Reference Leve, DeGarmo, Bridgett, Neiderhiser, Shaw, Harold, Natsuaki and Reiss2013). While there have been several genome-wide association studies (GWAS) of EF, few have identified any genome-wide significant variants. Wendel et al. (Reference Wendel, Papiol, Andlauer, Zimmermann, Wiltfang, Spitzer, Senner, Schulte, Schmauß and Schaupp2021) identified nine genome-wide significant SNPs on chromosome 5 that may be associated with performance on a set-shifting task, though these results have yet to be replicated (Wendel et al., Reference Wendel, Papiol, Andlauer, Zimmermann, Wiltfang, Spitzer, Senner, Schulte, Schmauß and Schaupp2021). Thus, EF is likely highly polygenic, with few and weak specific genetic variants directly related to any phenotype (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Yang, Wang and Faraone2020).

Beyond familial and genetic etiological risk for EF skills, Friedman et al. (2018) note that EF skills remain sensitive to unique environmental influences. As noted above, investigations into the potential for other causal pathways have also identified variables such as neurological disorders, traumatic brain injury, brain structural differences, and differences in brain neurotransmitter levels, which help the brain maintain focus and control (e.g., Barnes et al., Reference Barnes, Dean, Nandam, O'Connell and Bellgrove2011; Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Mohamed, Carmody, Malik, Faro and Lewis2013; Grattan & Eslinger, Reference Grattan and Eslinger1991). Maternal smoking during pregnancy has been identified as one exposure with associated teratological effects on brain development and related mechanisms that adversely affect EF (England et al., Reference England, Aagaard, Bloch, Conway, Cosgrove, Grana, Gould, Hatsukami, Jensen, Kandel, Lanphear, Leslie, Pauly, Neiderhiser, Rubinstein, Slotkin, Spindel, Stroud and Wakschlag2017).

Maternal smoking during pregnancy

Maternal smoking during pregnancy has been associated with numerous offspring outcomes, such as low birth weight (Knopik et al., Reference Knopik, Marceau, Palmer, Smith and Heath2016b), disruptive behavior problems (Ekblad et al., Reference Ekblad, Marceau, Rolan, Palmer, Todorov, Heath and Knopik2020a, Reference Ekblad, Rolan, Marceau, Palmer, Todorov, Heath and Knopik2020b), neurodevelopmental disorders such as ADHD (Buck et al., Reference Buck, Yu, Knopik and Stitzel2021; Knopik et al., Reference Knopik, Marceau, Bidwell, Palmer, Smith, Todorov, Evans and Heath2016a; Micalizzi et al., Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Brick, Palmer, Todorov, Heath, Evans and Knopik2018), reading and language performance (Micalizzi et al., Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Evans, Brick, Palmer, Heath and Knopik2021), and academic difficulties (Martin & Dombrowski, Reference Martin and Dombrowski2008). Despite its well-documented risks, maternal smoking during pregnancy remains prevalent, with roughly 7% of women reporting use during the prenatal period (Azagba et al., Reference Azagba, Manzione, Shan and King2020). Rates of use are higher in women aged 20–24 (9.9%) and in those with less education (12.2%) (Azagba et al., Reference Azagba, Manzione, Shan and King2020).

There are multiple reviews on the mode of action of prenatal smoke exposure from both the animal and human model perspective (e.g., Buck et al., Reference Buck, Yu, Knopik and Stitzel2021; Ernst et al., Reference Ernst, Moolchan and Robinson2001; Slikker Jr et al., Reference Slikker, Xu, Levin and Slotkin2005). Maternal smoking during pregnancy alters neuronal differentiation, results in lower proportions of neurons in the developing dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and is associated with reduced brain volume. Maternal smoking during pregnancy may exert these influences, in part, through its interaction with several neurotransmitter systems in the brain that act as key mediators of the actions of neuronal differentiation, maturation, and morphology. More specifically, maternal smoking during pregnancy has been associated with altered nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (nAChR) receptor patterns in both placental tissue of smoking mothers and in brains of children exposed to maternal smoking during pregnancy (e.g., Falk et al., Reference Falk, Nordberg, Seiger, Kjaeldgaard and Hellstrom-Lindahl2005; Lavezzi, Reference Lavezzi2018; Machaalani et al., Reference Machaalani, Ghazavi, Hinton, Waters and Hennessy2014), decreased levels of norepinephrine (Oncken et al., Reference Oncken, Henry, Campbell, Kuhn, Slotkin and Kranzler2003) and perturbations in the catecholaminergic system (Barnes et al., Reference Barnes, Dean, Nandam, O'Connell and Bellgrove2011; Buck et al., Reference Buck, Yu, Knopik and Stitzel2021). Taken together, maternal smoking during pregnancy may, during the critical developmental period of pregnancy, affect brain regions and neurotransmitter systems thought to be implicated in executive processes (Knopik et al., Reference Knopik, Marceau, Bidwell, Palmer, Smith, Todorov, Evans and Heath2016a).

Association of maternal smoking during pregnancy and executive function

Maternal smoking during pregnancy appears to be a risk factor for EF abilities, given that prenatal smoke exposure has been found to alter the developing brain regions responsible for executive processes. Children exposed to maternal smoking during pregnancy are at higher risk of having thinner parahippocampal, middle frontal, and orbitofrontal cortices (Toro et al., Reference Toro, Leonard, Lerner, Lerner, Perron, Pike, Richer, Veillette, Pausova and Paus2008), and smaller cortical gray matter and parenchymal volumes (Rivkin et al., Reference Rivkin, Davis, Lemaster, Cabral, Warfield, Mulkern, Robson, Rose-Jacobs and Frank2008). At present, however, evidence concerning the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and EF has been mixed, and the inconsistency in findings might be both age and assessment dependent, as outlined below.

Maternal smoking during pregnancy is associated with lower EF scores in early childhood, with assessment tools ranging from parent-rated scales to cognitive ability tasks. In an ethnically-diverse sample in the United States, maternal smoking during pregnancy predicted poorer EF, assessed by the Nebraska Barnyard task and a Go/NoGo task, in 5-year-old children (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Espy and Wakschlag2016). Studies in international samples also found similar associations between maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring EF scores, using the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function – Preschool Version (BRIEF) (Daseking et al., Reference Daseking, Petermann, Tischler and Waldmann2015) and the McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (MCSA) (MCSA: Julvez et al., Reference Julvez, Ribas-Fito, Torrent, Forns, Garcia-Esteban and Sunyer2007).

There is a paucity of research in adolescence. To our knowledge, only two studies to date have examined the relationship between maternal smoking during pregnancy and EF skills in adolescents and results are mixed. Maternal smoking during pregnancy was associated with poorer teacher-rated EF scores using the BRIEF (Rose-Jacobs et al., Reference Rose-Jacobs, Richardson, Buchanan-Howland, Chen, Cabral, Heeren, Liebschutz, Forman and Frank2017); however, this association was not found in an earlier study with EF tested by a variety of tasks, such as the Stroop Test (Fried et al., Reference Fried, Watkinson and Gray2003).

One major challenge when considering the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and EF is, as exemplified in the narrative above, the sheer variety of EF assessments and subsequent approaches to calculate EF. Specifically, a summed score of all subdomains was a common EF outcome of interest. EF factor scores were also considered consistent with research suggesting that the developmental pattern of EF progresses from unity to diversity. More specifically, EF has been shown to be unitary as it emerges in early childhood (Wiebe et al., Reference Wiebe, Sheffield, Nelson, Clark, Chevalier and Espy2011) such that a single factor EF model best explains EF performance in middle childhood. However, diversity begins to emerge around age 13 (e.g., Xu et al., Reference Xu, Han, Sabbagh, Wang, Ren and Li2013). Further, the three subdomains of EF have been shown to be related to different brain regions and functioning mechanisms (Fiske & Holmboe, Reference Fiske and Holmboe2019), such that when these subdomains were considered separately, distinct associations have been suggested. For example, maternal smoking during pregnancy has been related to set-shifting and inhibition domains more robustly, rather than to the domain of working memory (Fried et al., Reference Fried, Watkinson and Gray1998; Oh et al., Reference Oh, Xu, Terrizzi, Lanphear, Chen, Kalkbrenner and Yolton2020). Therefore, research examining the three subdomains of EF separately is warranted to provide clearer implications for the relationship between maternal smoking during pregnancy and EF. One further point that contributes to inconsistent findings in this area is the assessment of maternal smoking during pregnancy, which ranged from prospective report of amount of cigarette smoking during the 2nd and 3rd trimesters of pregnancy (Cornelius et al., Reference Cornelius, Ryan, Day, Goldschmidt and Willford2001) to a retrospective binary question of yes/no (Thakur et al., Reference Thakur, Sengupta, Grizenko, Choudhry and Joober2012).

Association of maternal smoking during pregnancy and working memory

Working memory is an online information processing and manipulation system which can be separated into visual and auditory components (Mesulam, Reference Mesulam2002). While perhaps related, working memory is different from memory and is usually considered an element of EF. Given its executive nature, working memory is important for successful inhibitory control (Pennington et al., Reference Pennington, Bennetto, McAleer and Roberts1996; Roberts Jr & Pennington, Reference Roberts and Pennington1996) and, therefore, is considered central to outcomes such as ADHD. Working memory is critical for advanced cognitive tasks, including language comprehension, spatial thinking, learning, and reasoning (Shah & Miyake, Reference Shah and Miyake1996) and, by extension, academic performance (Giofre et al., Reference Giofre, Borella and Mammarella2017; Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Hwang, Toplak, Weiss and Tannock2011).

Imaging studies have suggested that maternal smoking during pregnancy affects the structural maturation of the developing brain (Rivkin et al., Reference Rivkin, Davis, Lemaster, Cabral, Warfield, Mulkern, Robson, Rose-Jacobs and Frank2008; Toro et al., Reference Toro, Leonard, Lerner, Lerner, Perron, Pike, Richer, Veillette, Pausova and Paus2008), including regions suggested to be related to working memory functioning. In children across multiple studies and countries, maternal smoking during pregnancy has been associated with deficits in various memory-related tasks, including (i) the Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning-Screening (WRAML-S), which requires learning a list of words and recreating geometric designs from memory (Cornelius et al., Reference Cornelius, Ryan, Day, Goldschmidt and Willford2001), (ii) Listening Recall, Backwards Digit Recall, Verbal fluency and Unusual Uses task (Vandenbroucke et al., Reference Vandenbroucke, Verschueren, Ceulemans, De Smedt, De Roover and Baeyens2016), as well as (iii) the Self-Ordered Pointing Task (SOPT) and the Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning (WRAML: Thakur et al., Reference Thakur, Sengupta, Grizenko, Choudhry and Joober2012).

In adolescents and young adults, the relationship between maternal smoking during pregnancy and working memory is inconsistent. Adolescent tobacco smokers with exposure to maternal smoking during pregnancy experienced greater nicotine withdrawal-related deficits in both immediate and delayed visuospatial memory as assessed by the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R: Jacobsen et al., Reference Jacobsen, Slotkin, Westerveld, Mencl and Pugh2006); although maternal smoking during pregnancy effects may be confounded with youth current smoking status. Another study found no effect of maternal smoking during pregnancy on short-term working memory, verbal working memory, and immediate and long-term memory for auditory/verbal material as assessed by the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children III (WISC-III) and the Stories Immediate, Stories Delayed, and Stories Recognition subtests of Children’s Memory Scale (CMS: Kafouri et al., Reference Kafouri, Leonard, Perron, Richer, Seguin, Veillette, Pausova and Paus2009). In addition, fMRI studies in adolescents and young adults reported that those exposed to maternal smoking during pregnancy displayed similar task performance to unexposed control participants in a N-Back working memory task; however, exposed children showed greater brain activation in the regions responsible for verbal working memory, memory maintenance, and inhibitory control suggesting that exposed and unexposed children may use different brain regions and approaches to succeed when engaged in working memory tasks (Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Mohamed, Carmody, Malik, Faro and Lewis2013; Longo et al., Reference Longo, Fried, Cameron and Smith2013).

Besides the possible age-dependent effect of maternal smoking during pregnancy on working memory, the assessment of working memory may also matter. Specifically, the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and visuospatial working memory has been more robust irrespective of assessment (Jacobsen et al., Reference Jacobsen, Slotkin, Westerveld, Mencl and Pugh2006; Thakur et al., Reference Thakur, Sengupta, Grizenko, Choudhry and Joober2012), whereas the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and verbal working memory has been inconsistent across studies (Cornelius et al., Reference Cornelius, Ryan, Day, Goldschmidt and Willford2001; Longo et al., Reference Longo, Fried, Cameron and Smith2013; Marcelle et al., Reference Marcelle, Oliva and Hinshaw2020; Vandenbroucke et al., Reference Vandenbroucke, Verschueren, Ceulemans, De Smedt, De Roover and Baeyens2016).

Association of maternal smoking during pregnancy and set-shifting

In early childhood, no robust relationship between maternal smoking during pregnancy and set-shifting has been reported. In preschool aged children, mean ratings of set-shifting did not differ between mothers who smoked and mothers who abstained (Daseking et al., Reference Daseking, Petermann, Tischler and Waldmann2015; Wiebe et al., Reference Wiebe, Espy, Stopp, Respass, Stewart, Jameson, Gilbert and Huggenvik2009).

In adolescents, similar to other EF components already discussed, there is a general trend of little to no association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and set-shifting; although results were mixed. Generally, maternal smoking during pregnancy was not associated with set-shifting in adolescents when assessed using a Trail Making Task (Cornelius et al., Reference Cornelius, Ryan, Day, Goldschmidt and Willford2001; Fried & Watkinson, Reference Fried and Watkinson2000) and the Mirsky’s shift/be flexible factor (Fried & Watkinson, Reference Fried and Watkinson2001). However, when set-shifting was assessed using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST) or the Stroop task, results were more mixed. This suggests that the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and set-shifting during adolescence may be task specific. More specifically, when set-shifting was assessed using the WCST, two studies found no association (Fried & Watkinson, Reference Fried and Watkinson2001; Fried et al., Reference Fried, Watkinson and Gray2003) whereas one study found a positive association such that children whose mothers smoked at any point during the pregnancy displayed more perseverative behavior on the WCST than children whose mothers did not smoke (Cornelius et al., Reference Cornelius, Ryan, Day, Goldschmidt and Willford2001). When set-shifting was assessed using the Stroop task, two studies found no association (Cornelius et al., Reference Cornelius, Ryan, Day, Goldschmidt and Willford2001; Fried et al., Reference Fried, Watkinson and Gray2003) and one study found an association when maternal smoking during pregnancy was measured continuously as opposed to dichotomously (yes/no) (Cornelius et al., Reference Cornelius, De Genna, Leech, Willford, Goldschmidt and Day2011).

Association of maternal smoking during pregnancy and inhibitory control

In children, maternal smoking during pregnancy is associated with poorer performance in inhibitory domains. Preschool children exposed to maternal smoking during pregnancy exhibited increased inhibitory deficits, as much as four times higher than non-exposed children, as measured by parent and teacher ratings on the BRIEF-Preschool Version (BRIEF-P, German version: Daseking et al., Reference Daseking, Petermann, Tischler and Waldmann2015). School-aged children exposed to maternal smoking during pregnancy also showed poorer inhibitory control on the Delay Frustration Task (Huijbregts et al., Reference Huijbregts, Warren, de Sonneville and Swaab-Barneveld2008) and the Stroop task (Cornelius et al., Reference Cornelius, De Genna, Leech, Willford, Goldschmidt and Day2011). Imaging studies suggested similar findings with those exposed to maternal smoking during pregnancy making more errors when completing a Go/NoGo response-inhibition task (Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Mohamed, Carmody, Bendersky, Patel, Khorrami, Faro and Lewis2009). Additionally, maternal smoking during pregnancy was associated with greater brain activation in exposed children, suggesting, as with working memory, that exposed and unexposed children may use different brain regions and approaches to succeed when engaged in response inhibition tasks (Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Mohamed, Carmody, Bendersky, Patel, Khorrami, Faro and Lewis2009).

In emerging adults, a longitudinal imaging study suggested that those exposed to maternal smoking during pregnancy displayed similar task performance as the control group when assessed by a Go/NoGo task; however, maternal smoking during pregnancy exposure was associated with greater brain activation in the regions responsible for response inhibition (Longo et al., Reference Longo, Fried, Cameron and Smith2013). Similar to the EF subdomains of working memory and set-shifting, findings appear to be assessment specific. For example, reports of greater brain activation as a function of maternal smoking during pregnancy were not supported in other samples of emerging adults exposed to maternal smoking during pregnancy, who exhibited less brain activity in the areas responsible for response inhibition while doing an Erikson flanker/NoGo task (Holz et al., Reference Holz, Boecker, Baumeister, Hohm, Zohsel, Buchmann, Blomeyer, Jennen-Steinmetz, Hohmann, Wolf, Plichta, Meyer-Lindenberg, Banaschewski, Brandeis and Laucht2014).

Maternal smoking during pregnancy-EF associations examined using genetically-informed designs

As noted above, EF displays familial transmission and has been shown to be highly heritable. Maternal smoking during pregnancy has also been shown to have genetic influences (Agrawal et al., Reference Agrawal, Knopik, Pergadia, Waldron, Bucholz, Martin, Heath and Madden2008). Yet, few studies have examined the relationship between maternal smoking during pregnancy and EF while also accounting for genetic risk (Micalizzi & Knopik, Reference Micalizzi and Knopik2018). In fact, in many epidemiological studies of the relationship between maternal smoking during pregnancy and EF, there is very limited, if any, inclusion of maternal and paternal covariates, which may contribute to overestimated associations.

Quasi-experimental designs, such as sibling comparison models and other genetically-informed approaches, test for the likely possibility that risk factors that co-occur with maternal smoking during pregnancy might better explain poor outcomes than a teratogenic effect of maternal smoking during pregnancy itself (Knopik, Reference Knopik2009). More specficially, the sibling comparison design allows for the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring outcome to be examined both within a family and between families. Results can strengthen our inferences about causality in associations of maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring outcomes. The between-family association can be considered consistent with the historically standard comparison of looking at offspring of mothers who smoke during pregnancy relative to offspring of mothers who do not. This comparison does not control for influences that siblings share (i.e., genetic and familial effects) that might confound the maternal smoking during pregnancy-outcome association; however, it does allow replication of historical associations found in the literature. Conversely, a significant within-family association provides the strongest support for a causal influence of maternal smoking during pregnancy on an offspring outcome (e.g., child EF): i.e., the exposed sibling has larger EF skill deficits than the unexposed sibling. Because the design controls for mother- and family-level characteristics, and to a certain extent genetic influences, the within-family association is the strongest evidence that maternal smoking during pregnancy may be causally linked with the outcome. Note, however, it is not proof of causation. In the case of EF, women with EF deficits may be more likely to smoke during pregnancy and thus, may pass down both correlated genes and environmental influences that are associated with EF problems. This possibility might contribute to a non-causal association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and EF.

It is also the case that there are other confounding variables, such as secondhand smoke exposure or other risk factors, that may contribute to what appears to be a causal relationship between maternal smoking during pregnancy and EF, when in reality, the association is due to associated genes, behaviors, or contexts that influence both EF and maternal smoking during pregnancy. Utilizing an adopted-at-birth design, prenatal risk (as assessed by maternal substance use during pregnancy as well as pregnancy illness/complications) was associated with worse set-shifting scores as assessed by the shape Stroop task in children approximately 2 years in age (Leve et al., Reference Leve, DeGarmo, Bridgett, Neiderhiser, Shaw, Harold, Natsuaki and Reiss2013). However, prenatal risk was nonsignificant in the presence of genetic influences within the model (Leve et al., Reference Leve, DeGarmo, Bridgett, Neiderhiser, Shaw, Harold, Natsuaki and Reiss2013). A limitation of this particular adoption study was that maternal smoking during pregnancy was not specifically examined but rather included in the larger context of risk. Additionally, child and maternal covariates were limited.

Micalizzi et al. (Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Brick, Palmer, Todorov, Heath, Evans and Knopik2018) used the present sample (MO-MATCH) to examine the relationship between maternal smoking during pregnancy and one aspect of EF, inhibitory control, using a sibling-comparison design. As outlined above, this design can directly inform questions of the etiology (i.e., genetic and environmental influences) of individual differences in offspring outcomes and provide a rigorous test of potentially causal effects of maternal smoking during pregnancy on these outcomes. In that study, maternal smoking during pregnancy was associated with poorer inhibitory control (i.e., a significant within-family effect) as measured by the Delis Kaplan Executive Function System (DKEFS) Color Word Interference task; however, once measured familial confounding variables (i.e., child and maternal covariates) were accounted for, the effect did not hold, suggesting that familial transmission is a more important consideration in offspring inhibitory control (Micalizzi et al., Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Brick, Palmer, Todorov, Heath, Evans and Knopik2018).

Present study

Given: (1) the paucity of maternal smoking during pregnancy-EF investigations that account for familial confounding; (2) the inconsistent nature of the existing literature focused on the relationship between maternal smoking during pregnancy and EF as reviewed above, and (3) the knowledge from animal studies that maternal smoking during pregnancy affects brain regions known to be involved in EF skills (see Buck et al., Reference Buck, Yu, Knopik and Stitzel2021 and Knopik, Reference Knopik2009 for reviews), the present study attempts to add clarity to existing research on the maternal smoking during pregnancy-EF relationship using a sibling comparison design of early to middle adolescents. The dataset used for the present examination was collected specifically to attempt to disentangle familial influences from maternal smoking during pregnancy-exposure influences on four key domains, where based on animal work and existing human work, we might expect to find true deficits: memory, executive function, reading/language, and ADHD. For the purposes of this current study, measures assessing set-shifting, working memory and inhibitory control will be considered. This study also capitalizes on data regarding sociodemographic characteristics (used as covariates in the present study) which have been shown to be associated with maternal smoking during pregnancy and child EF.

It was hypothesized, based on the existing literature summarized above, that maternal smoking during pregnancy would be associated with poorer EF skills (set-shifting, working memory, and inhibitory control), but that, due to the familial nature of EF skills, the sibling comparison design would yield attenuated associations between maternal smoking during pregnancy and EF skills. If these hypotheses are supported, it would suggest that confounding factors – i.e., genetic and environmental influences that siblings share (familial transmission) – play a more important role in predicting EF skills in children and adolescents. If, however, the maternal smoking during pregnancy-EF association persists in the sibling comparison models, it would be suggestive of a potential causal role of maternal smoking during pregnancy above and beyond familial transmission.

Methods

Participants and procedure

The MO-MATCH (Knopik et al., Reference Knopik, Heath, Marceau, Palmer, McGeary, Todorov and Evans2015) sample consists of 173 families (N = 344 total children) in which mothers smoked (or smoked more) during one pregnancy but did not smoke (or smoked less) in the other pregnancy. Birth records (years 1998–2005) were obtained from the Missouri Department of Health and Senior Services Bureau of Health Informatics and used to identify families (N > 4,000 identified) with two children for which birth records indicated discordant smoke exposure. Mothers (N = 1,520) were screened to determine eligibility to participate in the current study. Consistent with reports regarding accuracy and reliability of birth record data (e.g., Bradford et al., Reference Bradford, Cardenas, Camacho-Carr and Lydon-Rochelle2007; Stout et al., Reference Stout, Macones and Tuuli2017), 27% agreed with the birth record and were deemed eligible for recruitment.

Following a maternal interview, (M age at assessment = 39.83, SD = 5.62) where mothers reported on their pregnancies, mental health, and her children’s behavior, families (and when possible, fathers n = 96 [M age at assessment = 44.04, SD = 6.34]) completed in-depth neuropsychological assessments. Families in which fathers participated were not different from families in which fathers did not participate on any focal variable (Knopik et al., Reference Knopik, Heath, Marceau, Palmer, McGeary, Todorov and Evans2015). Assessments of both children occurred simultaneously in the laboratory when youth were age 7–16 years (Child 1 M age = 12.99, SD = 1.94, 53% male; Child 2 M age = 10.19, SD = 1.80, 51% male). A project coordinator and four research assistants with backgrounds in psychiatric nursing, psychology, behavioral science, or related fields were trained to administer laboratory assessments by a pediatric clinical neuropsychologist. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Purdue University, Rhode Island Hospital, Washington University and the State of Missouri Department of Health and Senior Services.

Parents were primarily White (96%, n = 250). Most mothers and fathers completed at least some college education (77.2 and 65%, respectively) and 83.3% were married at the time of Child 1’s [older sibling] birth and 81.7% were married at Child 2’s [younger sibling] birth. Mothers typically smoked (or smoked more) in the first pregnancy (64%). Few families received food stamps at the time of birth of Child 1 (9.74%) and Child 2 (13.73%).

Measures

Smoking during pregnancy

A modified version of the Missouri Assessment of Genetics Interview for Children–Parent on Child (Todd et al., Reference Todd, Joyner, Heath, Neuman and Reich2003) was used to assess maternal smoking during pregnancy in each pregnancy. Any smoking during pregnancy was assessed as a discrete indicator specific to each trimester (0 = No, 1 = Yes), overall quantity of smoking during pregnancy (0= no smoking during pregnancy, 1 = 21 or less, 2 = 21–99, 3 = 100 + cigarettes), and the quantity of cigarettes smoked in each trimester (continuous variable from 0 to 98 cigarettes) were assessed. Using the above, a severity score was created that was specific to each child’s exposure to smoking during pregnancy, where 1 = no maternal smoking during pregnancy, 2 = maternal smoking during pregnancy in the first trimester only, 1–10 cigarettes per day, 3 = maternal smoking during pregnancy in the first trimester only, 11–19 cigarettes per day, 4 = maternal smoking during pregnancy in the first trimester only, 20+ cigarettes per day, 5 = maternal smoking during pregnancy beyond the first trimester, 1–10 cigarettes per day (max of all trimesters), 6 = maternal smoking during pregnancy beyond the first trimester, 11–19 cigarettes per day (max of all trimesters), 7 = maternal smoking during pregnancy beyond the first trimester, 20+ cigarettes per day (max of all trimesters). The score was calculated as such because: a) literature suggests exposure in late pregnancy may be more harmful than earlier in pregnancy (e.g., Dwyer et al., Reference Dwyer, McQuown and Leslie2009; Hebel et al., Reference Hebel, Fox and Sexton1988); and b) many maternal smoking during pregnancy effects follow a dose-response pattern (e.g., Knopik et al., Reference Knopik, Marceau, Palmer, Smith and Heath2016b).Footnote 1

Executive function

Inhibitory control

Two tasks were used to assess inhibitory control.

  1. (1) The inhibition condition of the Color-Word Interference Test on the Delis-Kaplin Executive Function System (DKEFS; Delis et al., Reference Delis, Kaplan and Kramer2001a). This one variable was examined in an earlier report (Micalizzi et al., Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Brick, Palmer, Todorov, Heath, Evans and Knopik2018). In the present study, we aimed to comprehensively examine all subdomains of executive function using two assessments per domain. Thus, we include it here for a complete description of all EF tasks administered in MO-MATCH and, due to the prior examination in Micalizzi et al. (Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Brick, Palmer, Todorov, Heath, Evans and Knopik2018), limit discussion of the results to summary form (Table 2) and text (full results in Supplementary Table 6). For DKEFS inhibitory control, participants were required to report the ink color of words that spelled a dissonant color word. For example, if the word red was printed in green ink, the correct response was “green” rather than “red.” Two baseline conditions (naming of color patches and naming of words that denote colors printed in black ink) preceded 10 practice items. If participants required four corrections the task was discontinued. Test trials included 50 color words printed in dissonant ink colors. If three consecutive errors were made on test trials, participants were prompted to name the ink color once. Raw completion time in seconds (maximum 180 seconds) were normed (mean = 10, SD = 3) and corrected for the appropriate age group. Higher scaled scores indicate better performance.

  2. (2) The Logan Stop-signal Task (SSRT; implemented in E-Prime; Verbruggen et al., Reference Verbruggen, Logan and Stevens2008), requires the participant to react to a stimulus by either withholding their response (i.e., stop-signal) or actively responding (i.e., go-signal). Performance was determined by go-signal obedience. The response is considered inhibited if the go-signal was not obeyed, and not inhibited if the go-signal was obeyed (Matzke et al., Reference Matzke, Love and Heathcote2017). Inhibitory control is measured by calculating the delay time between introduction of stimuli and reaction time (Logan & Cowan, Reference Logan and Cowan1984; Logan et al., Reference Logan, Cowan and Davis1984). Poor task performance, i.e., requiring more time to inhibit responses to stop-signals, indicates slower cognitive processes characterized by decreased accuracy and impulsive responding (Dimoska & Johnstone, Reference Dimoska and Johnstone2008). SSRT has been shown to be relatively constant, at approximately 200 ms, in healthy adults across a number of different forms of movement, including typing, button-pressing, and eye movement (Logan, Reference Logan1994).

Working memory

Two measures of working memory in the present study:

  1. 1. To assess auditory working memory, the WISC (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2003) Digit Span Backward subtest assessed children’s ability to manipulate verbally-presented information while in short term memory storage. The task requires children to repeat strings of numbers that increase in length in reverse order following verbal presentation (one number per second) from the experimenter. Digit Span Backward Total Raw Score (the sum of item scores) was used in the current analyses.

  2. 2. Visual working memory was assessed via the Spatial Span Backward subtest of the WISC in children. Spatial span backward is considered the nonverbal analog to the digit span backward task (Teixeira et al., Reference Teixeira, Zachi, Roque, Taub and Ventura2011). In parents, visual working memory is assessed via WMS-III Symbol Span (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997).

Set-shifting/cognitive flexibility

In this study, we considered two measures of cognitive flexibility in order to capture (i) cognitive flexibility in the visual motor domain and (ii) cognitive flexibility in the verbal domain.

  1. 1. The DKEFS Trail Making Test (Delis et al., Reference Delis, Kaplan and Kramer2001b) consists of five conditions. Condition 1 is a visual search task requiring participants to scan a page and cross out circles containing a given number. Condition 2 is a number sequencing task, in which participants draw lines to connect numbers in sequential order. Condition 3 is identical to condition 2, except that participants connect letters rather than numbers. Condition 4 measures set-shifting ability as it requires participants to switch between connecting numbers and letters. Condition 5 is a motor speed task that requires participants to trace a dotted line that connects a series of open circles. The contrast score from the Number Letter Switching Score (Condition 4) minus the Combined Number and Letter Sequencing Score was used to assess set-shifting, or more specifically cognitive flexibility in the visual motor domain.

  2. 2. Cognitive flexibility in the verbal domain was assessed with the DKEFS Verbal Fluency Test, which consists of four conditions: Letter Fluency, Category Fluency, Category Switching, and Category Switching Accuracy. The contrast score between the Category-Switching subtest minus the Category Fluency score will provide us with an additional measure of verbal cognitive flexibility. This contrast score accounts for the more basic verbal fluency abilities in order to assess (higher order) cognitive flexibility.

Covariates

Maternal and familial characteristics that could confound the association of maternal smoking during pregnancy and EF were included as covariates. These were selected based on the literature (e.g., Camerota & Willoughby, Reference Camerota and Willoughby2020) and to be consistent with prior reports using this sample and approach (Ekblad et al., Reference Ekblad, Marceau, Rolan, Palmer, Todorov, Heath and Knopik2020; Knopik et al., Reference Knopik, Heath, Marceau, Palmer, McGeary, Todorov and Evans2015; Marceau et al., Reference Marceau, Cinnamon Bidwell, Karoly, Evans, Todorov, Palmer, Heath and Knopik2018; Micalizzi et al., Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Brick, Palmer, Todorov, Heath, Evans and Knopik2018, Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Evans, Brick, Palmer, Heath and Knopik2021): maternal report of her marital status, food stamp usage, age, and education at birth of each child, child birth order, child sex, and second-hand smoke exposure during pregnancy (by the father). There is some debate in the field as to the utility of including IQ in models considering EF (Arffa, Reference Arffa2007). This debate is largely due to mixed findings on the association between IQ and aspects of EF, with some studies finding strong correlations (e.g., Colom et al., Reference Colom, Abad, Angeles Quiroga, Chun Shih and Flores-Mendoza2008) and others finding no associations (e.g., Welsh et al., Reference Welsh, Pennington and Groisser1991). In order to be consistent with prior work using MO-MATCH (Micalizzi et al., Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Brick, Palmer, Todorov, Heath, Evans and Knopik2018, Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Evans, Brick, Palmer, Heath and Knopik2021) and in order to determine the relationship between maternal smoking during pregnancy and EF that is independent of IQ, both child and mother IQ (Wechsler et al., Reference Wechsler, Kaplan, Fein, Kramer, Morris, Delis and Maerlender2004; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2003) were controlled for in all analyses. Finally, the corresponding mother EF variables were included as covariates, with the only exception being the Logan Stop Task (i.e., SSRT) since that was not assessed in the parents. Because only approximately half of fathers completed the study, only maternal covariates were included in the models reported in this paper. Sensitivity analyses were used to probe (a) inclusion of father covariates and (b) exclusion of IQ given the aforementioned debate in the field are available upon request. For the second set of sensitivity models, removing IQ did not change the pattern of findings.

Analytic plan

Factor structure of EF variables

A parallel analysis (Horn, Reference Horn1965) was conducted to evaluate the number of factors underlying the indicators of executive function using the psych package in R (Revelle, Reference Revelle2013). Parallel analysis is a method for determining the number of factors to retain from a factor analysis from random data simulation. The parallel analysis method involves the generation of a series of correlation matrices among random variables that correspond to the sample size and number of variables in the actual dataset. An average is obtained of the eigenvalues from the randomly generated data and compared to the eigenvalues of the actual data. The number of factors for the actual data that are greater than random eigenvalues obtained from the parallel analysis should be retained (Hayton et al., Reference Hayton, Allen and Scarpello2004).

Hierarchical linear models (HLM)

In MO-MATCH, siblings (level 1) are nested in families (level 2). Our sibling comparison approach (identical to the approach detailed in Knopik et al., Reference Knopik, Marceau, Bidwell, Palmer, Smith, Todorov, Evans and Heath2016a, Reference Knopik, Marceau, Palmer, Smith and Heath2016b; Micalizzi et al., Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Brick, Palmer, Todorov, Heath, Evans and Knopik2018, Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Evans, Brick, Palmer, Heath and Knopik2021) involved fitting a series of HLM to account for non-independence of data, as well as to assess the within- and between-family associations of maternal smoking during pregnancy and EF. Standard models (i.e., those that do not leverage the sibling comparison aspect of the data) were conducted to test hypothesis 1 (i.e., that increased exposure to maternal smoking during pregnancy would be associated with lower EF) in a way more directly comparable to findings from non-genetically informed samples in the literature.

The standard models compared children whose mothers smoked (or smoked more) during pregnancy to those whose mothers who did not smoke (or smoked less) on EF. This approach does not capitalize on the family structure/sibling comparison aspect of the data, but does adjust for the non-independence of observations. The standard model was run without (Zero-order; Model 1) and with (Covariate Adjusted; Model 2) covariates.

The sibling comparison models parsed the extent to which maternal smoking during pregnancy operates at a within-family (i.e., contributing to differences in EF in one sibling versus another, within families) and/or between-family level (i.e., contributing to differences in overall, average levels of siblings’ EF across families). Two variables were computed to capture maternal smoking during pregnancy severity and covaried in the sibling-comparison models: a) the family average maternal smoking during pregnancy score (i.e., the average score for maternal smoking during pregnancy across both siblings) and b) child-specific maternal smoking during pregnancy severity relative to family average for each child (i.e., resulting value when the family average maternal smoking during pregnancy was subtracted from each child-specific maternal smoking during pregnancy severity score; or within-family centering). Family average maternal smoking during pregnancy severity is included as a covariate to control for the between-family association between maternal smoking during pregnancy severity and EF (i.e., the overall associations between maternal smoking during pregnancy and related familial factors on EF, comparing across families). The association between the child-specific maternal smoking during pregnancy severity relative to family average and EF assesses a potentially causal within-family effect of maternal smoking during pregnancy in that familial confounding is controlled by design. That is, this association compares across siblings within a family, and thus is a test of any unique association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and child specific outcomes beyond familial and genetic factors that siblings share. The child-specific relative to family average maternal smoking during pregnancyseverity score was entered as a level 1 predictor, whereas the family average maternal smoking during pregnancy severity score was entered as a level 2 predictor. The sibling comparison model was also run without (Zero-order; Model 3) and with (Covariate Adjusted; Model 4) covariates.

Results

Means and standard deviations for study variables are presented in Table 1. A summary of the main findings (beta-weights from the maternal smoking during pregnancy variables for all outcomes from the zero-order and covariate adjusted sibling-comparison models) is provided in Table 2. More detailed tables providing the full context of models (all parameter estimates, including covariates, variance estimates, and model fit statistics from the covariate-adjusted standard and sibling-comparison models) are presented in supplemental materials.

Table 1. Sample characteristics

1 Analyzed in Micalizzi et al. (Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Brick, Palmer, Todorov, Heath, Evans and Knopik2018) and reported here for completeness.

2 Because fewer fathers completed the assessment, only maternal covariates were included in the models reported in this paper. Father covariates were explored in sensitivity analyses and the results are available on request.

3 Child 1 = older sibling; Child 2 = younger sibling.

Table 2. Summary of maternal smoking during prengnacy effects from sibling-comparison models

Note. ***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05

Covariates included in the covariate adjusted models were maternal age, education, marital status, food stamp usage, SES, birth order, child sex, secondhand smoke exposure by fathers, and child IQ.

Individual models controlled for the parallel mother variable (e.g., we controlled for mother set-shifting when child set-shifting was the focal outcome).

See Supplemental Tables for full model results including covariate estimates.

Factor analysis of EF variables

The correlations between the EF variables in the present study ranged from −0.12 to 0.27) and were largely non-significant. Those that were significant are modest in magnitude (e.g., r TRAILS-ColorWordInterf = 0.25, r TRAILS-SpatialSpanBwd = 0.27, r SSRT-SpatialSpanBwd = 0.19, r DigitSpanBwd-ColorWordInterf = 0.19). These results are consistent with findings that the DKEFS subscales show only low positive correlations—suggesting that the tasks measure unique aspects of EF with low overlap in variance (Swanson, Reference Swanson2005). Thus, one explanation for this pattern of findings is a function of the use of the DKEFS assessment. One other plausible explanation for the discrepant findings with the previously observed three-factor structure is publication bias (Karr et al. (Reference Karr, Areshenkoff, Rast, Hofer, Iverson and Garcia-Barrera2018).

Results from the parallel analysis revealed that the eigenvalues from the random data were larger than the eigenvalues from the factor analysis. In fact, the eigenvalue of a factor extracted from random data exceeded even the first factor extracted from these data. As such, results from the parallel analysis indicated that there was no meaningful factor solution underlying the EF variables and, thus, no information to extract. As a result, all variables were treated as separate dependent variables in the HLMs. Because the DKEFS Color Word Interference task was the focus of a previously published paper from our group (Micalizzi et al., Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Brick, Palmer, Todorov, Heath, Evans and Knopik2018), we do not discuss detailed results in the present report and instead only mention a summary of those findings as well as an extension of those models for a complete picture of the EF variables used in this analysis.

Hierarchical models examining associations between maternal smoking during pregnancy and component EF skills

In our early- to middle-adolescent sample, in the standard models without covariates (Model 1), maternal smoking during pregnancy predicted poorer performance in visual working memory as measured by WISC Spatial Span Backward (b = −0.14, SE = 0.06, p < 0.01; Supplemental Table 5) and response inhibition as measured by the DKEFS Color-Word Interference task (b = −0.18, SE = 0.07, p < 0.05; see detailed parameter estimates and model fit statistics in Supplemental Table 6 and report by Micalizzi et al., Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Brick, Palmer, Todorov, Heath, Evans and Knopik2018). However, neither maternal smoking during pregnancy association survived covariate adjustment (Model 2). In the adjusted standard models for visual working memory and response inhibition, consistent with familial transmission, the corresponding maternal executive function variables were also associated with child visual working memory and child response inhibition, respectively. Maternal smoking during pregnancy was not associated with SSRT, set-shifting (visual motor or verbal fluency variables), or auditory working memory (see Table 2 and corresponding Supplemental Tables).

The significant maternal smoking during pregnancy-EF associations from the standard model were fully attenuated in the sibling comparison models. In the sibling comparison models without covariates, there was a significant within-family association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and visual working memory (WISC Spatial Span Backward; b = −0.13, SE = 0.07, p < 0.05; Table 2 and Supplemental Table 5) and inhibitory control (DKEFS Color Word Interference; b = −0.17, SE = 0.07, p < 0.05; Table 2, see detailed parameter estimates and model fit statistics in Supplemental Table 6 and prior report by Micalizzi et al., Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Brick, Palmer, Todorov, Heath, Evans and Knopik2018). By themselves, these results would suggest a potentially causal effect of maternal smoking during pregnancy on these EF component skills. However, for both variables, this relationship with maternal smoking during pregnancy did not remain significant once covariates were included in the model (Model 4), indicating that familial confounding is present for visual working memory and response inhibition. Similar to the standard models, child IQ and respective maternal executive function variables significantly predicted outcome.

As a way to investigate nuances of familial transmission, post hoc analyses of the covariate-adjusted sibling comparison models were examined. That is, for the two models in which there was an effect of the maternal EF variable (i.e., for visual working memory and response inhibition), we examined one submodel where the respective maternal EF variable was dropped from the full covariate-adjusted sibling comparison model (i.e., Model 4). For each phenotype, this submodel was compared to the full covariate-adjusted sibling comparison model (Model 4) to evaluate the intergenerational transmission of these two EF phenotypes after controlling for the other covariates and maternal smoking during pregnancy. Specifically, model comparisons were evaluated using χ2 difference tests and Akaike’s information criterion, with lower Akaike’s representing better fitting models. For both EF variables, maternal EF could not be dropped from the model without a significant decrement in model fit. When considering visual working memory (WISC Spatial Span Backward), when maternal visual working memory was dropped from the covariate-adjusted sibling comparison model (-2LL = 1322.4, AIC = 1330.4 vs. -2LL = 1332.5; AIC = 1340.5), the change in χ2 was significant (Δχ2 = 10.1, df = 1, p < 0.002). Similarly, for response inhibition, when maternal DKEFS Color-Word Interference was dropped from the model, the change in χ2 was significant (Δχ2 = 11.6, df = 1, p < 0.001). These post hoc analyses of response inhibition as measured by the DKEFS Color Word Interference were not examined in Micalizzi et al (Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Brick, Palmer, Todorov, Heath, Evans and Knopik2018), thus we provide additional details here. Fit statistics from covariate-adjusted sibling comparison model including the maternal EF variable from Micalizzi et al. (Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Brick, Palmer, Todorov, Heath, Evans and Knopik2018): -2LL = 1333.9, AIC = 1341.9 vs dropping maternal EF variable -2LL = 1345.5, AIC = 1353.5. These analyses suggest that, above other child and familial variables that siblings share, there is no potentially causal effect of maternal smoking during pregnancy on EF in our sample. Further, in line with the literature suggesting that EF is highly heritable, maternal EF is one of the most significant predictors of child visual working memory and response inhibition.

Discussion

Using a genetically-sensitive sibling-comparison design, this study examined the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and EF skills. This sample was specifically designed with the intent to examine maternal smoking during pregnancy separately from other familial confounding variables. That is, the sibling-comparison approach partially controls for measured and unmeasured genetic and environmental influences, enabling a more rigorous test of the maternal smoking during pregnancy-EF relationship. Results revealed no association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and Stop-Signal Reaction Time (one measure of inhibitory control), DKEFS Trails (visual motor set-shifting), DKEFS Verbal Fluency (verbal set-shifting), and WISC Digit Span Backward (auditory working memory). For WISC Spatial Span Backward (visual working memory) and DKEFS Color-Word Interference (response inhibition/inhibitory control), initial results suggested a within-family association with maternal smoking during pregnancy, which is one step closer to indicating potentially causal effects of maternal smoking during pregnancy than provided in studies where familial confounds are not controlled by design. However, these initial within-family associations between maternal smoking during pregnancy and visual working memory and response inhibition were fully attenuated following covariate adjustment, making causal effects highly unlikely. Further, consistent with the reported high heritability of EF skills (e.g., Anokhin et al., Reference Anokhin, Golosheykin, Grant and Heath2010; Coolidge et al., Reference Coolidge, Thede and Young2000; Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Miyake, Young, DeFries, Corley and Hewitt2008; Gustavson et al., Reference Gustavson, Reynolds, Corley, Wadsworth, Hewitt and Friedman2022; Li & Roberts, Reference Li and Roberts2017), maternal EF significantly predicted offspring EF, above other familial variables that siblings share.

The current study provides additional insight into the maternal smoking during pregnancy-EF association. Epidemiological, non-genetic studies report mixed results with regard to the effects of maternal smoking during pregnancy on EF. When associations are found, the general pattern is increased maternal smoking during pregnancy exposure with poorer performance. However, as explicated earlier, there is considerable variability in these reports, such as developmental stage, measurement of maternal smoking during pregnancy, and assessment of EF. Further, there is often little to no consideration of parental EF skills and by extension, a lack of control for genetic and unmeasured environmental confounding. Maternal smoking during pregnancy, in humans, is correlated with a constellation of outcomes in mothers, and thus it may not be maternal smoking during pregnancy that is causing EF deficits in children, but rather the possibility that mothers who smoke during pregnancy also exhibit deficits in EF. Thus, children of mothers who smoke may present with EF deficits because maternal smoking during pregnancy and EF problems are both caused by common familial (genetic and environmental) influences. This nuance has not been comprehensively addressed to date.

We are aware of only three studies, one of which uses the current sample, that investigated the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and EF skills using genetically-informed designs (Ellingson et al., Reference Ellingson, Goodnight, Van Hulle, Waldman and D'Onofrio2014; Leve et al., Reference Leve, DeGarmo, Bridgett, Neiderhiser, Shaw, Harold, Natsuaki and Reiss2013; Micalizzi et al., Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Brick, Palmer, Todorov, Heath, Evans and Knopik2018). These studies examined three different skills. Ellingson et al. (Reference Ellingson, Goodnight, Van Hulle, Waldman and D'Onofrio2014) examined the relationship between maternal smoking during pregnancy and WISC Digit Span and found no within-family association of maternal smoking during pregnancy; however, it is unclear which Digit Span test (forward vs backward vs total) was used and thus which specific EF skill was tapped. Leve et al. (Reference Leve, DeGarmo, Bridgett, Neiderhiser, Shaw, Harold, Natsuaki and Reiss2013) used an adopted-at-birth design and found that a broad index of prenatal risk (which included maternal smoking during pregnancy) was associated with worse set-shifting; however, this prenatal effect was attenuated in the presence of genetic influences. Finally, Micalizzi et al. (Reference Micalizzi, Marceau, Brick, Palmer, Todorov, Heath, Evans and Knopik2018) used the present sample (MO-MATCH) to examine the relationship between maternal smoking during pregnancy and inhibitory control as we discuss and summarize in this report. In all three quasi-experimental studies to date, the samples are of different ages, maternal smoking during pregnancy was measured differently, and a variety of EF-skills were examined.

EF is comprised of multiple component skills – response inhibition, cognitive flexibility/set-shifting, and working memory – each of which can also be measured such that different aspects of that skill are assessed (e.g., visual vs auditory working memory). Consistent with that complexity, our results reflect mixed findings in the relationship between maternal smoking during pregnancy and each component skill. One possibility as to why we see maternal smoking during pregnancy associated with some EF skills but not all may be due to the brain regions and neurotransmitter systems involved in these skills. As noted earlier in this report, research has identified brain regions involved in EF and there is evidence from animal models that these some of these same areas are impacted by maternal smoking during pregnancy. Further, imaging studies show that adolescents and adults exposed to maternal smoking during pregnancy draw on a wider variety of brain regions to complete working memory and response inhibition tasks, but not with set-shifting (Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Mohamed, Carmody, Bendersky, Patel, Khorrami, Faro and Lewis2009; Longo et al., Reference Longo, Fried, Cameron and Smith2013). Thus, it seems within reason that we would see different patterns of findings with regard to different EF component skills.

The results presented should be interpreted within the context of the following limitations. First, our findings are dependent on our assessment of maternal smoking during pregnancy, which is collected via retrospective report. There is a growing literature supporting the use and reliability of retrospective report (Estabrook et al., Reference Estabrook, Massey, Clark, Burns, Mustanski, Cook, O'Brien, Makowski, Espy and Wakschlag2016; Knopik et al., Reference Knopik, Marceau, Bidwell, Palmer, Smith, Todorov, Evans and Heath2016a, Reference Knopik, Marceau, Palmer, Smith and Heath2016b) and we conducted additional verification of smoking behaviors during pregnancy to supplement maternal report (see Knopik et al., Reference Knopik, Marceau, Palmer, Smith and Heath2016b). Nonetheless, our results hinge on our assessment and operationalization of maternal smoking during pregnancy to accurately reflect the amount of maternal smoking during pregnancy exposure. Second, to the extent to which executive function skills are taught, children exposed to maternal smoking during pregnancy and who live in poorer neighborhoods and attend potentially less effective schools may be at particularly high risk for EF deficits. We are currently working with Census data to consider neighborhood disadvantage in our sample and will be able to explore this question in future work. Third, given the nature of data collection, this sample is necessarily a community sample which includes the full range of executive function skills as opposed to the most extreme deficits; such a clinical population might afford additional exploration into the maternal smoking during pregnancy-EF association. Fourth, while the statistical covariates included were carefully and purposefully selected, we could not measure or include all covariates that differ between siblings. There are likely confounds that were not considered here that influence the sibling comparison. For example, we have not examined reasons why these sibling pairs differ in their exposure to SDP. More specifically, why have these mothers changed their smoking behaviors from one pregnancy to another? These data were indeed collected as part of the larger project and will be used in future extensions of this work. Fifth, our methodological approach included a series of models that are hierarchical in nature and overlapping. These models included multiple EF outcomes and multiple tests per outcome. Thus, depending on the multiple testing correction strategy used (i.e., correcting for number of outcomes, number of tests, or number of outcomes*number of tests), some of our results may not survive correction. We thus suggest that results be interpreted using that lens and should be replicated. Finally, since fathers participated in about ∼60% of families, we chose to report the maternal covariate models to capitalize on power. When we restrict to families where both parents participated, the pattern does remain consistent across all variables (results available upon request).

In summary, our findings are consistent with a very small number of genetically-informed studies indicating familial confounding for cognitive-related phenotypes. We expand on prior work however, with the inclusion of maternal EF covariates, which appear to be a more salient risk factor for child EF skills, particularly visual working memory and response inhibition. That said, maternal smoking during pregnancy may be one indicator of a constellation of risks for EF and should be assessed in diagnostic evaluation. As such, intervention and prevention efforts focused on factors that contribute to maternal smoking during pregnancy could be one pathway to reduce poor outcomes in children exposed to smoking during pregnancy.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457942200075X.

Acknowledgments

This work supported by NIH grants: DA023134 (Knopik), DA17671 (Knopik), K01DA048135 (Micalizzi), K01DA039288 (Marceau), AA07728 (Heath.), AA09022 (Heath), AA11998 (Heath), HD049024 (Heath), AA017688 (Heath), AA021492 (Heath), R01DA04272 (Palmer). We would like to thank Rebecca Gordan for technical assistance with the Logan data. The invaluable contributions of the families who participated in this study are gratefully acknowledged.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Footnotes

1 Sensitivity analyses were conducted to evaluate the correspondence across various operationalizations of maternal smoking during pregnancy exposure. Correlations across quantifications were high and are reported in supplemental materials of Knopik et al. (Reference Knopik, Marceau, Bidwell, Palmer, Smith, Todorov, Evans and Heath2016a).

References

Agrawal, A., Knopik, V. S., Pergadia, M. L., Waldron, M., Bucholz, K. K., Martin, N. G., Heath, A. C., & Madden, P. A. F. (2008). Correlates of cigarette smoking during pregnancy and its genetic and environmental overlap with nicotine dependence. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 10(4), 567578. https://doi.org/10.1080/14622200801978672 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8(2), 7182. https://doi.org/10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anokhin, A. P., Golosheykin, S., Grant, J. D., & Heath, A. C. (2010). Developmental and genetic influences on prefrontal function in adolescents: A longitudinal twin study of WCST performance. Neuroscience Letters, 472(2), 119122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2010.01.067 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arffa, S. (2007). The relationship of intelligence to executive function and non-executive function measures in a sample of average, above average, and gifted youth. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22(8), 969978. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Azagba, S., Manzione, L., Shan, L., & King, J. (2020). Trends in smoking during pregnancy by socioeconomic characteristics in the united States, 2010-2017. BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth, 20(1) Article 52.https://doi.org/10.1186/s12884-020-2748-y,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, J. J. M., Dean, A. J., Nandam, L. S., O'Connell, R. G., & Bellgrove, M. A. (2011). The molecular genetics of executive function: Role of monoamine system genes. Biological Psychiatry, 69(12), E127E143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.12.040 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett, D. S., Mohamed, F. B., Carmody, D. P., Bendersky, M., Patel, S., Khorrami, M., Faro, S. H., & Lewis, M. (2009). Response inhibition among early adolescents prenatally exposed to tobacco: An fMRI study. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 31(5), 283290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ntt.2009.03.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett, D. S., Mohamed, F. B., Carmody, D. P., Malik, M., Faro, S. H., & Lewis, M. (2013). Prenatal tobacco exposure predicts differential brain function during working memory in early adolescence: A preliminary investigation. Rutgers University Community Repository, https://doi.org/10.7282/T3NG4NSH CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradford, H. M., Cardenas, V., Camacho-Carr, K., & Lydon-Rochelle, M. T. (2007). Accuracy of birth certificate and hospital discharge data: A certified nurse-midwife and physician comparison. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 11(6), 540548. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-007-0178-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buck, J. M., Yu, L., Knopik, V. S., & Stitzel, J. A. (2021). DNA methylome perturbations: An epigenetic basis for the emergingly heritable neurodevelopmental abnormalities associated with maternal smoking and maternal nicotine exposure. Biology of Reproduction, 105(3), 644666. https://doi.org/10.1093/biolre/ioab138 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camerota, M., & Willoughby, M. T. (2020). Prenatal risk predicts preschooler executive function: A cascade model. Child Development, 91(3), E682E700. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13271 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, S., Yang, L., Wang, Y., & Faraone, S. V. (2020). Shared polygenic risk for ADHD, executive dysfunction and other psychiatric disorders. Translational Psychiatry, 10(1, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-00872-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, C. A. C., Espy, K. A., & Wakschlag, L. (2016). Developmental pathways from prenatal tobacco and stress exposure to behavioral disinhibition. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 53, 6474. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ntt.2015.11.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colom, R., Abad, F. J., Angeles Quiroga, M., Chun Shih, P., & Flores-Mendoza, C. (2008). Working memory and intelligence are highly related constructs, but why? Intelligence, 36(6), 584606. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2008.01.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coolidge, F. L., Thede, L. L., & Young, S. E. (2000). Heritability and the comorbidity of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with behavioral disorders and executive function deficits: A preliminary investigation. Developmental Neuropsychology, 17(3), 273287. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn1703_1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornelius, M. D., De Genna, N. M., Leech, S. L., Willford, J. A., Goldschmidt, L., & Day, N. L. (2011). Effects of prenatal cigarette smoke exposure on neurobehavioral outcomes in 10-year-old children of adolescent mothers. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 33(1), 137144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ntt.2010.08.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornelius, M. D., Ryan, C. M., Day, N. L., Goldschmidt, L., & Willford, J. A. (2001). Prenatal tobacco effects on neuropsychological outcomes among preadolescents. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 22(4), 217225. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004703-200108000-00002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daseking, M., Petermann, F., Tischler, T., & Waldmann, H. C. (2015). Smoking during pregnancy is a risk factor for executive function deficits in preschool-aged children. Geburtshilfe Und Frauenheilkunde, 75(1), 6471. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0034-1383419 Google ScholarPubMed
Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001a). Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS): Examiner’s manual. The Psychological Corportation.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001b). The Delis-Kaplan executive function system: Technical manual. The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Dimoska, A., & Johnstone, S. J. (2008). Effects of varying stop-signal probability on ERPs in the stop-signal task: Do they reflect variations in inhibitory processing or simply novelty effects? Biological Psychology, 77(3), 324336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dwyer, J. B., McQuown, S. C., & Leslie, F. M. (2009). The dynamic effects of nicotine on the developing brain. Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 122(2), 125139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pharmthera.2009.02.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekblad, M. O., Marceau, K., Rolan, E., Palmer, R. H. C., Todorov, A., Heath, A. C., & Knopik, V. S. (2020a). The effect of smoking during pregnancy on severity and directionality of externalizing and internalizing symptoms: A genetically informed approach. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(21, https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17217921 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekblad, M. O., Rolan, E., Marceau, K., Palmer, R., Todorov, A., Heath, A. C., & Knopik, V. S. (2020b). Disruptive behavior in siblings discordant for exposure to maternal smoking during pregnancy: A multi-rater approach. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 22(8), 13301338. https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/ntz214 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellingson, J. M., Goodnight, J. A., Van Hulle, C. A., Waldman, I. D., & D'Onofrio, B. M. (2014). A sibling-comparison study of smoking during pregnancy and childhood psychological traits. Behavior Genetics, 44(1), 2535. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-013-9618-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
England, L. J., Aagaard, K., Bloch, M., Conway, K., Cosgrove, K., Grana, R., Gould, T. J., Hatsukami, D., Jensen, F., Kandel, D., Lanphear, B., Leslie, F., Pauly, J. R., Neiderhiser, J., Rubinstein, M., Slotkin, T. A., Spindel, E., Stroud, L., Wakschlag, L. (2017). Developmental toxicity of nicotine: A transdisciplinary synthesis and implications for emerging tobacco products. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 72, 176189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ernst, M., Moolchan, E. T., & Robinson, M. L. (2001). Behavioral and neural consequences of prenatal exposure to nicotine. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(6), 630641. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200106000-00007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Estabrook, R., Massey, S. H., Clark, C. A. C., Burns, J. L., Mustanski, B. S., Cook, E. H., O'Brien, T. C., Makowski, B., Espy, K. A., Wakschlag, L. S. (2016). Separating family-level and direct exposure effects of smoking during pregnancy on offspring externalizing symptoms: Bridging the behavior genetic and behavior teratologic divide. Behavior Genetics, 46(3), 389402. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-015-9762-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falk, L., Nordberg, A., Seiger, A., Kjaeldgaard, A., & Hellstrom-Lindahl, E. (2005). Smoking during early pregnancy affects the expression pattern of both nicotinic and muscarinic acetylcholine receptors in human first trimester brainstem and cerebellum. Neuroscience, 132(2), 389397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.12.049 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, A., & Holmboe, K. (2019). Neural substrates of early executive function development. Developmental Review, 52, 4262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2019.100866 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, P. A., & Watkinson, B. (2000). Visuoperceptual functioning differs in 9-to 12-year olds prenatally exposed to cigarettes and marihuana. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 22(1), 1120. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0892-0362(99)00046-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, P. A., & Watkinson, B. (2001). Differential effects on facets of attention in adolescents prenatally exposed to cigarettes and marihuana. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 23(5), 421430. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0892-0362(01)00160-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, P. A., Watkinson, B., & Gray, R. (1998). Differential effects on cognitive functioning in 9- to 12-year olds prenatally exposed to cigarettes and marihuana. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 20(3), 293306. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0892-0362(97)00091-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, P. A., Watkinson, B., & Gray, R. (2003). Differential effects on cognitive functioning in 13-to 16-year-olds prenatally exposed to cigarettes and marihuana. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 25(4), 427436. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0892-0362(03)00029-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 137(2), 201225. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giofre, D., Borella, E., & Mammarella, I. C. (2017). The relationship between intelligence, working memory, academic self-esteem, and academic achievement. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 29(6), 731747. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2017.1310110 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., & Guy, S. C. (2001). Assessment of executive functions in children with neurological impairment. In Psychological and developmental assessment: Children with disabilities and chronic conditions (pp. 317356). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Grattan, L. M., & Eslinger, P. J. (1991). Frontal-lobe damage in children and adults - A comparative review. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7(3), 283326. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649109540496 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gustavson, D. E., Reynolds, C. A., Corley, R. P., Wadsworth, S. J., Hewitt, J. K., & Friedman, N. P. (2022). Genetic associations between executive functions and intelligence: A combined twin and adoption study. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001168 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayton, J. C., Allen, D. G., & Scarpello, V. (2004). Factor retention decisions in exploratory factor analysis: A tutorial on parallel analysis. Organizational Research Methods, 7(2), 191205. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428104263675 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hebel, J. R., Fox, N. L., & Sexton, M. (1988). Dose-response of bith weight to various measures of maternal smoking during pregnancy. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 41(5), 483489. https://doi.org/10.1016/0895-4356(88)90050-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holz, N. E., Boecker, R., Baumeister, S., Hohm, E., Zohsel, K., Buchmann, A. F., Blomeyer, D., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., Hohmann, S., Wolf, I., Plichta, M. M., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Banaschewski, T., Brandeis, D., Laucht, M. (2014). Effect of prenatal exposure to tobacco smoke on inhibitory control neuroimaging results from a 25-year prospective study. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(7), 786796. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.343 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika, 30(2), 179185. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02289447 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huijbregts, S. C. J., Warren, A. J., de Sonneville, L. M. J., & Swaab-Barneveld, H. (2008). Hot and cool forms of inhibitory control and externalizing behavior in children of mothers who smoked during pregnancy: An exploratory study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(3), 323333. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-007-9180-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobsen, L. K., Slotkin, T. A., Westerveld, M., Mencl, W. E., & Pugh, K. R. (2006). Visuospatial memory deficits emerging during nicotine withdrawal in adolescents with prenatal exposure to active maternal smoking. Neuropsychopharmacology, 31(7), 15501561. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300981 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Julvez, J., Ribas-Fito, N., Torrent, M., Forns, M., Garcia-Esteban, R., & Sunyer, J. (2007). Maternal smoking habits and cognitive development of children at age 4 years in a population-based birth cohort. International Journal of Epidemiology, 36(4), 825832. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dym107 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kafouri, S., Leonard, G., Perron, M., Richer, L., Seguin, J. R., Veillette, S., Pausova, Z., & Paus, T. (2009). Maternal cigarette smoking during pregnancy and cognitive performance in adolescence. International Journal of Epidemiology, 38(1), 158172. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyn250 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karr, J. E., Areshenkoff, C. N., Rast, P., Hofer, S. M., Iverson, G. L., & Garcia-Barrera, M. A. (2018). The unity and diversity of executive functions: A systematic review and reanalysis of latent variable studies. Psychological Bulletin, 144(11), 11471185. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000160 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knopik, V. S. (2009). Maternal smoking during pregnancy and child outcomes: Real or spurious effect? Developmental Neuropsychology, 34(1), 136. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565640802564366,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knopik, V. S., Heath, A. C., Marceau, K., Palmer, R. H. C., McGeary, J. E., Todorov, A., & Evans, A. S. (2015). Missouri mothers and their children: A family study of the effects of genetics and the prenatal environment. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 18(5), 485496. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2015.46 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knopik, V. S., Marceau, K., Bidwell, L. C., Palmer, R. H., Smith, T. F., Todorov, A., Evans, A. S., & Heath, A. C. (2016a). Smoking during pregnancy and ADHD risk: A genetically-informed, multiple-rater approach. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics : the Official Publication of the International Society of Psychiatric Genetics, 171(7), 971981. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32421 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knopik, V. S., Marceau, K., Palmer, R. H. C., Smith, T. F., & Heath, A. C. (2016b). Maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring birth weight: A genetically-informed approach comparing multiple raters. Behavior Genetics, 46(3), 353364. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-015-9750-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lavezzi, A. M. (2018). Toxic effect of cigarette smoke on brainstem nicotinic receptor expression: Primary cause of sudden unexplained perinatal death. Toxics, 6(4), Article 63. https://doi.org/10.3390/toxics6040063 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leve, L. D., DeGarmo, D. S., Bridgett, D. J., Neiderhiser, J. M., Shaw, D. S., Harold, G. T., Natsuaki, M. N., & Reiss, D. (2013). Using an adoption design to separate genetic, prenatal, and temperament influences on toddler executive function. Developmental Psychology, 49(6), 10451057. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029390 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, J. J., & Roberts, D. K. (2017). Genetic influences on executive functions across the life span. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logan, G. D., & Cowan, W. B. (1984). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychological Review, 91(3), 295327. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.91.3.295 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logan, G. D., Cowan, W. B., & Davis, K. A. (1984). On th ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: A model and a method. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 10(2), 276291. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.10.2.276 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A user’s guide to the stop signal paradigm, <Go to ISI>://BCI:BCI199497248555.://BCI:BCI199497248555.>Google Scholar
Longo, C. A., Fried, P. A., Cameron, I., & Smith, A. M. (2013). The long-term effects of prenatal nicotine exposure on response inhibition: An fMRI study of young adults. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 39, 918. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ntt.2013.05.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Machaalani, R., Ghazavi, E., Hinton, T., Waters, K. A., & Hennessy, A. (2014). Cigarette smoking during pregnancy regulates the expression of specific nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (nAChR) subunits in the human placenta. Toxicology and Applied Pharmacology, 276(3), 204212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.taap.2014.02.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marceau, K., Cinnamon Bidwell, L., Karoly, H. C., Evans, A. S., Todorov, A. A., Palmer, R. H., Heath, A. C., & Knopik, V. C. (2018). Within-Family Effects of Smoking during Pregnancy on ADHD: the Importance of Phenotype. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 46(4), 685699. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-017-0320-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcelle, E. T., Oliva, M. T., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2020). Gestational smoking and hypertension as predictors of working memory functioning in childhood Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 497. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01950.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, R. P., & Dombrowski, S. C. (2008). Prenatal exposures: Psychological and educational consequences for children. Springer Science.Google Scholar
Matzke, D., Love, J., & Heathcote, A. (2017). A Bayesian approach for estimating the probability of trigger failures in the stop-signal paradigm. Behavior Research Methods, 49(1), 267281. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0695-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesulam, M. M. (2002). The human frontal lobes: Transcending the default mode through contingent encoding. In Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 830). Oxford University Press, https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195134971.003.0002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Micalizzi, L., & Knopik, V. S. (2018). Maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring executive function: What do we know and what are the next steps? Development and Psychopathology, 30(4), 13331354. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579417001687 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Micalizzi, L., Marceau, K., Brick, L. A., Palmer, R. H., Todorov, A. A., Heath, A. C., Evans, A., & Knopik, V. S. (2018). Inhibitory control in siblings discordant for exposure to maternal smoking during pregnancy. Developmental Psychology, 54(2), 199208. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000423 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Micalizzi, L., Marceau, K., Evans, A. S., Brick, L. A., Palmer, R. H. C., Heath, A. C., & Knopik, V. S. (2021). A sibling-comparison study of smoking during pregnancy and risk for reading-related problems. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 84, Article 106961. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ntt.2021.106961.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oh, K., Xu, Y., Terrizzi, B. F., Lanphear, B., Chen, A., Kalkbrenner, A. E., & Yolton, K. (2020). Associations between early low-level tobacco smoke exposure and executive function at age 8 years. Journal of Pediatrics, 221, 174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2019.11.032 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oncken, C. A., Henry, K. M., Campbell, W. A., Kuhn, C. M., Slotkin, T. A., & Kranzler, H. R. (2003). Effect of maternal smoking on fetal catecholamine concentrations at birth. Pediatric Research, 53(1), 119124. https://doi.org/10.1203/01.Pdr.0000042443.65546.E1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennington, B. F., Bennetto, L., McAleer, O., & Roberts, R. J. (1996). Executive functions and working memory: Theoretical and measurement issues. In Attention, memory, and executive function (pp. 327348). Paul H Brookes Publishing Co, https://doi.org/10.1097/00004703-199608000-00014 Google Scholar
Rabinovici, G. D., Stephens, M. L., & Possin, K. L. (2015). Executive dysfunction. Continuum (Minneap Minn), 21((3 Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry)), 646659, https://doi.org/10.1212/01.Con.0000466658.05156.54.Google ScholarPubMed
Revelle, W. (2013). How to do a factor analysis with the psych package. Northwestern University.Google Scholar
Rivkin, M. J., Davis, P. E., Lemaster, J. L., Cabral, H. J., Warfield, S. K., Mulkern, R. V., Robson, C. D., Rose-Jacobs, R., & Frank, D. A. (2008). Volumetric MRI study of brain in children with intrauterine exposure to cocaine, alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana. Pediatrics, 121(4), 741750. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2007-1399 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, R. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1996). An interactive framework for examining prefrontal cognitive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 12(1), 105126. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649609540642 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, M., Hwang, H., Toplak, M., Weiss, M., & Tannock, R. (2011). Inattention, working memory, and academic achievement in adolescents referred for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychology, 17(5), 444458. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2010.544648 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose-Jacobs, R., Richardson, M. A., Buchanan-Howland, K., Chen, C. A., Cabral, H., Heeren, T. C., Liebschutz, J., Forman, L., & Frank, D. A. (2017). Intrauterine exposure to tobacco and executive functioning in high school. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 176, 169175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.02.022 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shah, P., & Miyake, A. (1996). The separability of working memory resources for spatial thinking and language processing: An individual differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 125(1), 427. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.125.1.4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shields, G. S., Bonner, J. C., & Moons, W. G. (2015). Does cortisol influence core executive functions? A meta-analysis of acute cortisol administration effects on working memory, inhibition, and set-shifting. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 58, 91103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.04.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slikker, W., Xu, Z. A., Levin, E. D., & Slotkin, T. A. (2005). Mode of action: Disruption of brain cell replication, second messenger, and neurotransmitter systems during development leading to cognitive dysfunction—developmental neurotoxicity of nicotine. Critical Reviews in Toxicology, 35(8-9), 703711. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/10408440591007421 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stout, M. J., Macones, G. A., & Tuuli, M. G. (2017). Accuracy of birth certificate data for classifying preterm birth. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 31(3), 245249. https://doi.org/10.1111/ppe.12352 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, J. (2005). The Delis-Kaplan executive function system: A review. Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 20(1-2), 117128. https://doi.org/10.1177/0829573506295469 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teixeira, R. A. A., Zachi, E. C., Roque, D. T., Taub, A., & Ventura, D. F. (2011). Memory span measured by the spatial span tests of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery in a group of Brazilian children and adolescents. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 5(2), 129134. https://doi.org/10.1590/s1980-57642011dn05020012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thakur, G. A., Sengupta, S. M., Grizenko, N., Choudhry, Z., & Joober, R. (2012). Comprehensive phenotype/genotype analyses of the norepinephrine transporter gene (SLC6A2) in ADHD: Relation to maternal smoking during pregnancy. Plos One, 7(11, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049616 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Todd, R. D., Joyner, C. A., Heath, A. C., Neuman, R. J., & Reich, W. (2003). Reliability and stability of a semistructured DSM-IV interview designed for family studies. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42(12), 14601468. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200312000-00013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toro, R., Leonard, G., Lerner, J. V., Lerner, R. M., Perron, M., Pike, G. B., Richer, L., Veillette, S., Pausova, Z., Paus, T. (2008). Prenatal exposure to maternal cigarette smoking and the adolescent cerebral cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33(5), 10191027. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301484 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vandenbroucke, L., Verschueren, K., Ceulemans, E., De Smedt, B., De Roover, K., & Baeyens, D. (2016). Family demographic profiles and their relationship with the quality of executive functioning subcomponents in kindergarten. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 34(2), 226244. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12127 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verbruggen, F., Logan, G. D., & Stevens, M. A. (2008). STOP IT: Windows executable software for the stop-signal paradigm. Behavior Research Methods, 40(2), 479483. https://doi.org/10.3758/brm.40.2.479 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler memory scale (3rd edn. The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler intelligence scale for children (4th edn. The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D., Kaplan, E., Fein, D., Kramer, J., Morris, R., Delis, D., & Maerlender, A. (2004). Wechsler intelligence scale for children (4th, Integrated ed.). Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
Welsh, M. C., Pennington, B. F., & Groisser, D. B. (1991). A normative-developmental study of executive function: A window of prefrontal function in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7(2), 131149. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649109540483 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wendel, B., Papiol, S., Andlauer, T. F., Zimmermann, J., Wiltfang, J., Spitzer, C., Senner, F., Schulte, E. C., Schmauß, M., Schaupp, S. K. (2021). A genome-wide association study of the longitudinal course of executive functions. Translational Psychiatry, 11(1), 18. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-021-01510-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiebe, S. A., Espy, K. A., Stopp, C., Respass, J., Stewart, P., Jameson, T. R., Gilbert, D. G., & Huggenvik, J. I. (2009). Gene-environment interactions across development: Exploring DRD2 genotype and prenatal smoking effects on self-regulation. Developmental Psychology, 45(1), 3144. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014550 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiebe, S. A., Sheffield, T., Nelson, J. M., Clark, C. A. C., Chevalier, N., & Espy, K. A. (2011). The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 436452. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, F., Han, Y., Sabbagh, M. A., Wang, T., Ren, X., & Li, C. (2013). Developmental differences in the structure of executive function in middle childhood and adolescence. Plos One, 8(10, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0077770 Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample characteristics

Figure 1

Table 2. Summary of maternal smoking during prengnacy effects from sibling-comparison models

Supplementary material: File

Knopik et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S6

Download Knopik et al. supplementary material(File)
File 60 KB