Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-17T04:19:16.367Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Preterm birth and risk for language delays before school entry: A sibling-control study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 January 2020

Imac Maria Zambrana*
Affiliation:
Department of Special Needs Education, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway The Norwegian Center for Child Behavioral Development, Oslo, Norway
Margarete E. Vollrath
Affiliation:
Department of Mental and Physical Health, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway Section of Health, Developmental and Personality Psychology, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Bo Jacobsson
Affiliation:
Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, Sahlgrenska Academy, Gothenburg University, Sweden Department of Genetics and Bioinformatics, Domain of Health Data and Digitalisation, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway
Verena Sengpiel
Affiliation:
Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, Sahlgrenska Academy, Gothenburg University, Sweden Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Gothenburg, Sweden
Eivind Ystrom
Affiliation:
Section of Health, Developmental and Personality Psychology, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Department of Mental Disorders, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway PharmacoEpidemiology and Drug Safety Research Group, School of Pharmacy, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway.
*
Author for Correspondence: Imac M. Zambrana, Department of Special Needs Education, Faculty of Educational Sciences, University of Oslo, P.O Box 1140 Blindern, N-0318Oslo, Norway; E-mail: i.m.zambrana@isp.uio.no.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

We investigated whether children born preterm are at risk for language delay using a sibling-control design in the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa), conducted by the Norwegian Institute of Public Health. Participants included 26,769 siblings born between gestational weeks 23 and 42. Language delay was assessed when the children were 1.5, 3, and 5 years old. To adjust for familial risk factors, comparisons were conducted between preterm and full-term siblings. Pregnancy-specific risk factors were controlled for by means of observed variables. Findings showed that preterm children born before week 37 had increased risk for language delays at 1.5 years. At 3 and 5 years, only children born before week 34 had increased risk for language delay. Children born weeks 29–33 and before week 29 had increased risk for language delay at 1.5 years (RR = 4.51, 95% CI [3.45, 5.88]; RR = 10.32, 95% CI [6.7, 15.80]), 3 years (RR = 1.50, 95% CI [1.02, 2.21]; RR = 2.78, 95% CI [1.09, 7.07]), and 5 years (RR = 1.63, 95% CI [1.06, 2.51]; RR = 2.98, 95% CI [0.87, 10.26]), respectively. In conclusion, children born preterm are at risk for language delays, with familial confounders only explaining a moderate share of the association. This suggests a cause-effect relationship between early preterm birth and risk for language delay in preschool children.

Type
Regular Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2020

Preterm birth is associated with neurodevelopmental risks, such as lower language skills in toddlers and preschoolers (Guarini et al., Reference Guarini, Sansavini, Fabbri, Alessandroni, Faldella and Karmiloff-Smith2009; Menyuk, Liebergott, & Schultz, Reference Menyuk, Liebergott and Schultz1995; Palumbi et al., Reference Palumbi, Peschechera, Margari, Craig, Cristella, Petruzzelli and Margari2018; Ribeiro et al., Reference Ribeiro, Zachrisson, Schjolberg, Aase, Rohrer-Baumgartner and Magnus2011; Wolke, Samara, Bracewell, & Marlow, Reference Wolke, Samara, Bracewell and Marlow2008; Zambrana, Vollrath, Sengpiel, Jacobsson & Ystrom, Reference Zambrana, Vollrath, Sengpiel, Jacobsson and Ystrom2016). This might be explained by the fact that the third trimester is crucial for fetal brain development, with rapid development of neurons and wiring. This association has been found to be inverse linear, i.e., shorter gestational length is related to poorer language skills and higher risks for language delays (Boyle et al., Reference Boyle, Poulsen, Field, Kurinczuk, Wolke, Alfirevic and Quigley2012; de Jong, Verhoeven, & van Baar, Reference de Jong, Verhoeven and van Baar2012; Dong, Chen, & Yu, Reference Dong, Chen and Yu2012; McGowan, Alderdice, Holmes, & Johnston, Reference McGowan, Alderdice, Holmes and Johnston2011; Quigley et al., Reference Quigley, Poulsen, Boyle, Wolke, Field, Alfirevic and Kurinczuk2012; Stene-Larsen et al., Reference Stene-Larsen, Brandlistuen, Lang, Landolt, Latal and Vollrath2014). On one hand, it is expected that the preterm children's language development will to a certain degree catch up with time (Fitzpatrick, Carter, & Quigley, Reference Fitzpatrick, Carter and Quigley2016; Luu, Vohr, Allan, Schneider, & Ment, Reference Luu, Vohr, Allan, Schneider and Ment2011; Luu et al., Reference Luu, Vohr, Schneider, Katz, Tucker, Allan and Ment2009). On the other hand, early severe language delays, which are prevalent in children of the lowest gestational age, predict a higher risk for persistent language delays throughout the preschool years. These delays are in turn related to academic struggles as well as socioemotional and behavioral problems (Beitchman et al., Reference Beitchman, Wilson, Johnson, Atkinson, Young, Adlaf and Douglas2001; de Jong et al., Reference de Jong, Verhoeven and van Baar2012; Dong et al., Reference Dong, Chen and Yu2012; Wolke et al., Reference Wolke, Samara, Bracewell and Marlow2008). Therefore, one major question is whether and for how long the early risks for language delays persist.

A meta-analysis that compiled longitudinal change and stability trends from several smaller studies found that preterm-born children still scored lower than term-born children on selected language tests at age 12 years (van Noort-van der Spek, Franken, & Weisglas-Kuperus, Reference van Noort-van der Spek, Franken and Weisglas-Kuperus2012). Most earlier studies have adjusted for an array of well-known risk factors for preterm birth, among them family risk factors such as socioeconomic status, and maternal risk factors, such as hypertension or smoking, which may be confounded with language delays (Hauge, Aarø, Torgersen, & Vollrath, Reference Hauge, Aarø, Torgersen and Vollrath2013). However, the associations between gestational age and language delays may also be caused by unmeasured family risk factors such as a common genetic risk or a genetic risk for language delay that is related to risk-related maternal behavior that affects the prenatal environment and risk for preterm birth (Bishop, Price, Dale, & Robert, Reference Bishop, Price, Dale and Robert2003; Kovas et al., Reference Kovas, Hayiou-Thomas, Oliver, Dale, Bishop and Plomin2005). For example, previous studies have found that not only prenatal and neonatal health complications predict negative outcomes in preterm-born children (Hauge et al., Reference Hauge, Aarø, Torgersen and Vollrath2013; Poehlmann-Tynan et al., Reference Poehlmann-Tynan, Gerstein, Burnson, Weymouth, Bolt, Maleck and Schwichtenberg2015) but also negative parenting style (Poehlmann-Tynan et al., Reference Poehlmann-Tynan, Gerstein, Burnson, Weymouth, Bolt, Maleck and Schwichtenberg2015), which can be considered to be relatively stable across siblings and strongly related with genetic contributions to language skills (Dale, Tosto, Hayiou-Thomas, & Plomin, Reference Dale, Tosto, Hayiou-Thomas and Plomin2015). Traditional case–control and cohort designs do not allow taking such factors into account in a satisfactory way. This raises the question of the extent to which language delay is distinctively associated with gestational age as compared with unmeasured family factors.

Sibling-control studies provide a stronger design by comparing language skills among siblings who are discordant with regard to gestational age but share both genes and family background (Brandlistuen, Ystrom, Nulman, Koren, & Nordeng, Reference Brandlistuen, Ystrom, Nulman, Koren and Nordeng2013; D'Onofrio et al., Reference D'Onofrio, Class, Rickert, Larsson, Långström and Lichtenstein2013; Susser, Eide, & Begg, Reference Susser, Eide and Begg2010; Zambrana et al., Reference Zambrana, Vollrath, Sengpiel, Jacobsson and Ystrom2016). In addition, sibling studies allow for the control of observed risk factors that may vary between pregnancies in the same family (e.g., hypertension or maternal smoking; Hauge et al., Reference Hauge, Aarø, Torgersen and Vollrath2013). Such adjusted sibling designs consequently distinguish between effects that are due to family-invariant (e.g., stable) factors, pregnancy-specific factors, and gestational age (Zambrana et al., Reference Zambrana, Vollrath, Sengpiel, Jacobsson and Ystrom2016). Using a sibling-control design, we recently presented findings showing that risk for language delays in children that were born preterm decreased between 1.5 and 3 years. We also showed (for spontaneous births) that any remaining influence of preterm birth was explained by familial and pregnancy-related risk factors (Zambrana et al., Reference Zambrana, Vollrath, Sengpiel, Jacobsson and Ystrom2016).

The objective of this study was to extend the follow-up period of roughly the same sample to 5 years in order to understand whether the association between gestational age and risk for language delays continues to decline as children approach school age and to identify factors that may explain any remaining association. More specifically, we used a sibling-control design to examine the nature of the associations between (a) gestational age at birth and relative risks for language delays from age 1.5 to 5 years, and (b) whether the associations are brought about by stable (e.g., genetic risk) and variable (e.g., perinatal conditions) risk factors within families or the direct effects of gestational age at birth. The current study builds on and extends previous work by examining the risks for language delays that are associated with preterm birth at a period when children are approaching school age, an age when language delays have been found to be associated with general learning challenges with potential effects on children's well being.

Method

Sample

This study is based on data from the Norwegian Mother and Child Birth Cohort Study (MoBa), which is conducted at the Norwegian Institute of Public Health (Magnus et al., Reference Magnus, Irgens, Haug, Nystad, Skjaerven and Stoltenberg2006). The sample for MoBa includes 114,744 children and 95,242 mothers, who were recruited from across Norway during the period 1999–2008 in connection with an ultrasound examination that is routinely offered around pregnancy week 18. Informed consents were obtained upon recruitment, and 40.6% consented to participate. With respect to procedures, MoBa has received a license from the Norwegian Data Inspectorate and approval from The Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics. The current study used data from the Medical Birth Registry of Norway (MBRN) and maternal-reported MoBa questionnaires at the child chronological ages of 1.5 years, 3 years, and 5 years (the response rates were 77, 62, and 50% of those giving informed consent, respectively), and it is based on version 8B of the quality-assured data files that were released in 2015 (http://www.fhi.no/moba-en). Starting with 33,026 children of mothers that were participating in MoBa with several pregnancies, only first-borns from multiple births with either older or younger participating siblings from other pregnancies were included. We only retained children having outcome data at 1.5 years (n = 24,665), 3 years (n = 19,795), or 5 years (n = 13,394), amounting to a total of 26,769 (51.5% boys) children nested within 14,703 maternal clusters (see Table 1 for descriptive sample information). Of the 26,769 children, 2,104, 6,974, and 13,375 had missing data at 1.5, 3, and 5 years, respectively. In addition to nonresponse, the high amount of missing data at 5 years was because the questionnaire was not sent out to the cohorts of children who had already turned 6 years.

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of the sibling sample from MoBa (N = 26,769)

Note: N, sample size; M, mean; SD, standard deviation; SGA, small for gestational age; BMI, body mass index.

Preterm birth

Gestational age at birth was determined by ultrasound examinations or on the date of last menstruation registered in MBRN (for 2% of the sample). Five gestational age groups were created: very early preterm (gestation weeks ≤ 27/6), early preterm (weeks 28/0 to 33/6), late preterm (weeks 34/0 to 36/6), early term (weeks 37/0 to 38/6), and full term (≥ weeks 39/0). We calculated the average age of gestation across all children of each mother (i.e., group mean). In the sibling-control analyses, the differences between the average gestational age of the sibling group and the individual score provided gestational age deviations from the particular family mean (Zambrana et al., Reference Zambrana, Vollrath, Sengpiel, Jacobsson and Ystrom2016)

Language delay risk

Risks for language delays at ages 1.5, 3, and 5 years were measured by maternal-reported items from the Ages and Stages Questionnaires (ASQ) communication subscales (Dale et al., Reference Dale, Tosto, Hayiou-Thomas and Plomin2015), for which good test-retest agreement and concurrent validity have been reported (Janson & Squires, Reference Janson and Squires2004; Richter & Janson, Reference Richter and Janson2007; Squires, Bricker, & Potter, Reference Squires, Bricker and Potter1997). For 1.5-year-olds, MoBa selected four of the six age-corresponding ASQ items. For 3-year olds, MoBa selected four of the six age-corresponding ASQ items and mixed this with two items from the ASQ scales for ages 1.5 and 4 years. For 5-year-olds, MoBa included all six original items from the 5-year Communication scale plus one item from the original 4-year scale. The response categories were 1 = yes, very often, 2 = yes, sometimes, and 3 = not yet. In order to take into consideration that each item discriminates children at different loci on the latent continuum of risk for language delay in the child population, we examined the item function and reliability at different loci of the scales by using item response theory analyses. To get a more reliable basis for making cut points, we subsequently saved out factor scores separately for the three points. Following Zambrana et al. (Reference Zambrana, Pons, Eadie and Ystrom2014), the cut points for risk of language delay were set at 1.5 standard deviations (SD) above the mean at all ages (Squires et al., Reference Squires, Bricker and Potter1997).

Confounders

We controlled for risk factors related to preterm birth that can be unique to the specific pregnancy as well as for the health status of the mother and child in the given pregnancy. Pregnancy-related confounders were parity, multiple birth status, hypertensive condition, bleeding between weeks 13 and 28, recurrent urinary tract infections, gestational diabetes, and type of onset of delivery (i.e., nonspontaneous or spontaneous). Mother-related confounders were high body mass index (>30 kg/m2), levels of self-reported anxiety and depressive symptoms above the clinical cutoff on the Symptom Checklist-5 during pregnancy (Strand, Dalgard, Tambs, & Rognerud, Reference Strand, Dalgard, Tambs and Rognerud2003), smoking and alcohol intake during pregnancy, and maternal age. Child-related confounders were sex, serious malformations at birth, and being small for gestational age (> 2 SD below the uterine growth curve; Maršál et al., Reference Maršál, Persson, Larsen, Lilja, Selbing and Sultan1996).

Statistical Analyses

All of the analyses were run in STATA 15.0 statistical software, with all three models (i.e., unadjusted cohort, unadjusted sibling, and adjusted sibling-control analyses) using the same sibling sample for comparison purposes (StataCorp, 2017). First, we compared the preterm groups and the full-term group by applying a regular cohort design. Subsequently, we planned to adjust our crude associations in two steps: first, adjusting for all risk factors common to siblings by adjusting for the mother's average time of delivery (sibling control) and second, adjusting for risk factors specific to each pregnancy by using observed covariates (adjusted sibling control) For details about the sibling models see (Zambrana et al., Reference Zambrana, Vollrath, Sengpiel, Jacobsson and Ystrom2016). We modeled risk for language delay across time by using a Poisson mixed model with a random intercept (set at age 5 years) and a fixed slope for effects at 1.5 and 3 years. Importantly, we subtracted gestational age from (and added time of reporting to) the panel data time variable set to calendar birth day, effectively centering each child´s measurement to its biological age as given by date of gestation and date of report. In this way, the effect of preterm birth could not be explained by different timing of measurement. To model the effect of age of gestation we allowed for a fixed quadratic effect of time. In the sample, we had four sibling cluster groups with varying numbers of children in them: Cluster 1 (children with no siblings), n = 3,316; Cluster 2 (children with 1 sibling), n = 21,454; Cluster 3 (children with 2 siblings), n = 1,923; and Cluster 4 (children with 3 siblings), n = 76. The overall, between-mother, and within-mother standard deviation of gestational age was 12, 10, and 7 days. We calculated the relative risk point estimates at the selected weeks of gestation for the three different outcome points by using the linear combination of estimates (lincom) function of STATA (https://www.stata.com/manuals/rlincom.pdf).

As with most cohort studies, the voluntary participation and attrition is far from random in MoBa (R. Nilsen et al., Reference Nilsen, Vollset, Gjessing, Skjaerven, Melve, Schreuder and Magnus2009; R. M. Nilsen et al., Reference Nilsen, Surén, Gunnes, Alsaker, Bresnahan, Hirtz and Stoltenberg2013). We have recently shown that the earliest preterm births are slightly underrepresented in MoBa, while the older preterm births are somewhat overrepresented (Zambrana et al., Reference Zambrana, Vollrath, Sengpiel, Jacobsson and Ystrom2016). We estimated missing data in the outcome variables by using full information maximum likelihood and all of the available outcome data so that the full analysis sample was 57,854 measures of risk for language delay, nested within 26,769 children, nested within 14,703 mothers.

Results

The unadjusted cohort results in Table 2 (left column) show that early preterm birth is associated with increased risk for language delay at all ages and that this risk is inversely linear within each point. For instance, the very early preterm group had an elevenfold increase in relative risk (RR) at 1.5 years compared with the reference term group, RR = 11.49, 95% CI [8.55, 15.49], while the RRs for the early preterm, the late preterm, and the early term groups were correspondingly 5.37, 95% CI [4.46, 6.48]; 2.56, 95% CI [2.29, 2.87]; and 1.81, 95% CI [1.65, 1.99], respectively, at this age. By age 5 years, the very early preterm group still showed a threefold increase in RR for language delay compared with the reference term group, RR = 3.19, 95% CI [0.96, 10.57], while the early term group only showed a 1.06 increased risk at this age, 95% CI [0.98, 1.16].

Table 2. Relative risk (RR) for language delay at 1.5, 3, and 5 years by gestational weeks (N = 26,769)

Note: aAnalyses with the same sibling sample used in a conventional cohort design. bAnalyses using the sibling-control design, controlling for unobserved familial risks, but not for measured confounders. cAnalyses using the sibling-control design, controlling for unobserved familial risks, and for child sex, multiple birth status, serious malformations at birth, small for gestational age, parity, pregnancy smoking and alcohol intake, hypertensive condition, bleeding between weeks 13 and 28, recurrent urinary tract infections, gestational diabetes, and nonspontaneous delivery as well as high maternal BMI, levels of self-reported anxiety and depressive symptoms during pregnancy, and maternal age. dGestational weeks and groups: ≥ 39/0 weeks = Full term, 37/0–38/6 weeks = Early term, 34/0–36/6 weeks = Late preterm, 28/0–33/6 wks = Early preterm, and ≤ 27/6 weeks = Very early preterm.

Adjusting for family-invariant risk factors (sibling control) did not alter the estimates considerably at 1.5 years (see Table 2, middle column). However, family-invariant factors accounted for a substantial amount of the sizable associations found at 3 years. At this period, only the very early preterm and early preterm groups showed an increased risk for language delay, which now dropped from a sevenfold to a threefold increased risk and a fourfold to less than a twofold increased risk, respectively. At 5 years, although the associations that were found in the cohort analyses were not accounted for by family-invariant factors, the initial associations were less sizable in the cohort analysis at this age.

Largely, adjusting for pregnancy-specific factors (observed covariates) did not alter the estimates considerably after the sibling control (Table 2, right column). Neither invariant nor variant familial factors accounted for much of the risk at 1.5 years. All preterm gestational age groups were still associated with an increased risk for language delay at 1.5 years, with associations increasing linearly from RR = 1.82, 95% CI [1.64, 2.02], for children born early term to RR = 10.32, 95%CI [6.74, 15.80], for children born very preterm. At 3 years, only the early preterm, RR = 1.50, 95% CI [1.02, 2.21], and very preterm, RR = 2.78, 95% CI [1.09, 7.07], siblings had a higher risk for language delay than their full-term siblings did. At age 5 years, siblings that were born early and very early preterm had an elevated risk for language delay, RR = 1.63, 95% CI [1.0, 2.51] and RR = 2.98, 95% CI [0.87, 10.26], respectively. However, the confidence intervals indicate substantial uncertainty in the exact effect sizes.

Previous work from the same cohort found that risks for language delays at 18 and 36 months were higher for provider-initiated births than for spontaneous births following sibling control (Zambrana et al., Reference Zambrana, Vollrath, Sengpiel, Jacobsson and Ystrom2016). Thus, to assess the potential differential effects of preterm birth on risk for language delay across spontaneous and provider-initiated births, we compared the three models with and without this interaction. There were no differential effects of preterm birth on risk for language delay according to provider-initiated vs. spontaneous birth for the three models: unadjusted cohort, χ2 (7) = 1.58, p = .98; AIC = -12.42, unadjusted sibling control, χ2 (8) = 6.20, p = .62; AIC = -9.80, adjusted sibling control, χ2 (7) = 3.27, p=.86; AIC = -10.73. However, neither the current growth models nor the measures are the same as in Zambrana et al. (Reference Zambrana, Vollrath, Sengpiel, Jacobsson and Ystrom2016).

Discussion

Overall, we found that lower gestational age was associated with a linear increase in risk of language delay at all ages. However, the finding that there is a diminished risk for language delays over time and that most preterm children are not born at very early gestational ages indicates that the majority of the preterm children have caught up with their siblings by age 5. When examining the degree to which other factors could account for the increased risks, the results varied according to age. At age 1.5 years, the sibling-control analyses showed that the effects of preterm birth on risk for language delays were substantial and not due to factors that are stable within the families or covariates specific to the pregnancies. At age 3 years, however, family invariant and pregnancy-specific confounders accounted for persistent relative risk for language delays to a large degree, except for the very early preterm group. At 5 years, a similar pattern was evident, but the effects in the unadjusted analyses were much smaller initially.

The results are both in line with and in contrast to findings from previous studies. First of all, prior studies have also found that preterm birth is a major risk factor for early language delays (Menyuk et al., Reference Menyuk, Liebergott and Schultz1995; Ribeiro et al., Reference Ribeiro, Zachrisson, Schjolberg, Aase, Rohrer-Baumgartner and Magnus2011; Zambrana et al., Reference Zambrana, Vollrath, Sengpiel, Jacobsson and Ystrom2016) and that the delays are less pronounced by age 3 years (Zambrana et al., Reference Zambrana, Vollrath, Sengpiel, Jacobsson and Ystrom2016). This study adds to the literature by showing that, although the unadjusted analyses show that the risk for language delays is decreasing further throughout the preschool years, the language delays appear to be more stable in this period following the sibling adjustment. In fact, the earliest preterm born children are in particular continuing to display a considerable increased risk for language delay at 5 years. Future studies will show to what extent this risk decreases further as the children enter school.

Secondly, the sibling-control models enabled us not only to examine the role of the measured covariates but also to adjust for the effects of unmeasured familial risk factors. In fact, our sibling design revealed that family factors accounted for a substantial amount of the increased and continued risk of language delay that the preterm-born children showed up to ages 3 and 5 years. This is in line with our previous findings about the association between preterm birth and language production and comprehension up to 3 years, at least for children that were delivered spontaneously (Zambrana et al., Reference Zambrana, Vollrath, Sengpiel, Jacobsson and Ystrom2016; Ask et al., Reference Ask, Gustavson, Ystrom, Havdahl, Tesli, Askeland and Reichborn-Kjennerud2018) recently found preterm birth to be associated with innatention, but not hyperactivity, within sibling pairs. Therefore, language delay and inattention could be markers of a broader spectrum of brain health risk following preterm birth.

Some limitations should be mentioned. The ASQ communication subscales are short scales that were developed for screening purposes, especially at 18 months, implying that the reliability is strongest at the low pole of the scales. Therefore, while our findings address the degree to which gestational age is associated with a decrease in relative risk for early language delays (i.e., a catch-up), more research is needed to examine subtler differences at more advanced levels of language. Yet, the growth-model design ensures that all associations are estimated simultaneously and allows for adjusting the time of measurement for age of gestation and time of maternal report, so it provides more robust measures than does examining one point at the time. It should also be mentioned that the sibling-control design reduces the statistical power of the analyses. In particular, the 5-year estimates for the very early preterm birth group have large and uncertain confidence intervals. For example, while there was a significant association between early preterm birth and language delay at 5 years, the confidence intervals for very early preterm crossed 1.00. However, the most likely value (i.e., the point estimate) for very early preterm birth was 2.98 not 1.00. It would be illogical to argue based on the conventions of statistical significance that it is “unsafe” to be born early preterm and at the same time “safe” to be born very early preterm.

Finally, given that mothers with preterm birth are more likely to drop out of the study (introducing bias), as presented in our previous paper (Zambrana et al., Reference Zambrana, Vollrath, Sengpiel, Jacobsson and Ystrom2016), and that this risk is increased further if the children also have language delays, there may be an additional bias in the rates of language delays in our sample such that they are also more prone to the effects of mothers who have dropped out of the study. Including all of the available outcome data and estimating missing data by using full information maximum likelihood addresses some of the problems with attrition. However, sibling-design estimates can also be biased if the covariates that are included are more familial than the exposure and random measurement error is not accounted for (Frisell, Öberg, Kuja-Halkola, & Sjölander, Reference Frisell, Öberg, Kuja-Halkola and Sjölander2012).

Altogether, the present and previous findings suggest that children that are born late preterm to early term catch up with their peers before starting school. However, even if the risk for language delays diminishes with age, the delays are still substantial for children born before week 34, especially for children born very early preterm, who still display a striking threefold relative risk at both ages 3 and 5. Considering the importance of language abilities for cognitive and social adjustment as well as mental health (Beitchman et al., Reference Beitchman, Wilson, Johnson, Atkinson, Young, Adlaf and Douglas2001; de Jong et al., Reference de Jong, Verhoeven and van Baar2012; Dong et al., Reference Dong, Chen and Yu2012; Wolke et al., Reference Wolke, Samara, Bracewell and Marlow2008), all of which are crucial in transition to school, this is of particular importance to practitioners working with this group of children. Of particular practical significance, the results of the present study also suggest that the etiological picture for children who are born preterm who display persistent risk for language delays may be more complex than that of children who catch up with their peers. However, additional studies are needed to further our understanding of the complexity of the delays and the factors underlying them.

Conclusion

This population-based sibling-cohort study shows that preterm birth is associated with an increased risk for language delays at 1.5, 3, and 5 years of age. Moreover, this association is not confounded by familial risk factors and it is potentially causal in nature. However, the magnitude of this risk decreases as the children approach school age. This suggests that the factors explaining the increased risk for language delay in children who are delivered preterm change as these children approach school age, which potentially can have implications for practitioners who are working with preterm-born children with persistent risk for language delays.

Supplementary Material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419001536.

Acknowledgments

The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study is supported by the Norwegian Ministry of Health and Care Services and the Ministry of Education and Research, NIH/NIEHS (contract no N01-ES-75558), NIH/NINDS (grant no.1 UO1 NS 047537-01 and grant no.2 UO1 NS 047537-06A1). The authors are grateful to all the families in Norway who participate in this ongoing cohort study.

References

Ask, H., Gustavson, K., Ystrom, E., Havdahl, K. A., Tesli, M., Askeland, R. B., & Reichborn-Kjennerud, T. (2018). Association of gestational age at birth with symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. JAMA Pediatrics, 172, 749756. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.1315CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beitchman, J. H., Wilson, B., Johnson, C. J., Atkinson, L., Young, A., Adlaf, E., … Douglas, L. (2001). Fourteen-year follow-up of speech/language-impaired and control children: psychiatric outcome. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 7582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V., Price, T. S., Dale, P. S., & Robert, P. (2003). Outcomes of early language delay: II. Etiology of transient and persistent language difficulties. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 46, 561575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyle, E. M., Poulsen, G., Field, D. J., Kurinczuk, J. J., Wolke, D., Alfirevic, Z., & Quigley, M. A. (2012). Effects of gestational age at birth on health outcomes at 3 and 5 years of age: Population based cohort study. BMJ, 344, e896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandlistuen, R. E., Ystrom, E., Nulman, I., Koren, G., & Nordeng, H. (2013). Prenatal paracetamol exposure and child neurodevelopment: A sibling-controlled cohort study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42, 17021713. doi:10.1093/ije/dyt183CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, P. S., Tosto, M. G., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., & Plomin, R. (2015). Why does parental language input style predict child language development? A twin study of gene–environment correlation. Journal of Communication Disorders, 57, 106117. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2015.07.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Jong, M., Verhoeven, M., & van Baar, A. L. (2012). School outcome, cognitive functioning, and behaviour problems in moderate and late preterm children and adults: A review. Seminars in Fetal and Neonatal Medicine, 17, 163169. doi:10.1016/j.siny.2012.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dong, Y., Chen, S., & Yu, J. (2012). A systematic review and meta-analysis of long-term development of early term infants. Neonatology, 102, 212221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Onofrio, B. M., Class, Q. A., Rickert, M. E., Larsson, H., Långström, N., & Lichtenstein, P. (2013). Preterm birth and mortality and morbidity: A population-based quasi-experimental study. JAMA Psychiatry, 70, 12311240. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.2107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitzpatrick, A., Carter, J., & Quigley, M. A. (2016). Association of gestational age with verbal ability and spatial working memory at age 11. Pediatrics, 138, e20160578. doi:10.1542/peds.2016-0578Google ScholarPubMed
Frisell, T., Öberg, S., Kuja-Halkola, R., & Sjölander, A. (2012). Sibling comparison designs: Bias from non-shared confounders and measurement error. Epidemiology, 23, 713720. doi:10.1097/EDE.0b013e31825fa230CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guarini, A., Sansavini, A., Fabbri, C., Alessandroni, R., Faldella, G., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2009). Reconsidering the impact of preterm birth on language outcome. Early Human Development, 85, 639645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauge, L. J., Aarø, L. E., Torgersen, L., & Vollrath, M. E. (2013). Smoking during consecutive pregnancies among primiparous women in the population-based Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 15, 428434. doi:10.1093/ntr/nts147CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janson, H., & Squires, J. (2004). Parent-completed developmental screening in a Norwegian population sample: A comparison with US normative data. Acta Paediatrica, 93, 15251529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kovas, Y., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Oliver, B., Dale, P. S., Bishop, D. V. M., & Plomin, R. (2005). Genetic influences in different aspects of language development: The etiology of language skills in 4.5-year-old twins. Child Development, 76, 632651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luu, T. M., Vohr, B. R., Allan, W., Schneider, K. C., & Ment, L. R. (2011). Evidence for catch-up in cognition and receptive vocabulary among adolescents born very preterm. Pediatrics, 128, 313322. doi:10.1542/peds.2010-2655CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luu, T. M., Vohr, B. R., Schneider, K. C., Katz, K. H., Tucker, R., Allan, W. C., & Ment, L. R. (2009). Trajectories of receptive language development from 3 to 12 years of age for very preterm children. Pediatrics, 124, 333341. doi:10.1542/peds.2008-2587CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnus, P., Irgens, L. M., Haug, K., Nystad, W., Skjaerven, R., Stoltenberg, C., & The Moba Study Group (2006). Cohort profile: The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa). International Journal of Epidemiology, 35, 11461150. doi:10.1093/ije/dyl170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maršál, K., Persson, P. H., Larsen, T., Lilja, H., Selbing, A., & Sultan, B. (1996). Intrauterine growth curves based on ultrasonically estimated foetal weights. Acta Paediatrica, 85, 843848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGowan, J. E., Alderdice, F. A., Holmes, V. A., & Johnston, L. (2011). Early childhood development of late-preterm infants: A systematic review. Pediatrics, 127, 11111124. doi:10.1542/peds.2010-2257CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menyuk, P., Liebergott, J., & Schultz, M. (1995). Early language development in full –term and in premature infants. Hillsdale, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Nilsen, R. M., Surén, P., Gunnes, N., Alsaker, E. R., Bresnahan, M., Hirtz, D., … Stoltenberg, C. (2013). Analysis of self-selection bias in a population-based cohort study of autism spectrum disorders. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 27, 553563. doi:10.1111/ppe.12077CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nilsen, R., Vollset, S., Gjessing, H., Skjaerven, R., Melve, K., Schreuder, P., … Magnus, P. (2009). Self-selection and bias in a large prospective pregnancy cohort in Norway. Paediatric Perinatal Epidemiology, 23, 597608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palumbi, R., Peschechera, A., Margari, M., Craig, F., Cristella, A., Petruzzelli, M. G., Margari, L. (2018). Neurodevelopmental and emotional-behavioral outcomes in late-preterm infants: An observational descriptive case study. BMC Pediatrics, 18, 318. doi: 10.1186/s12887-018-1293-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poehlmann-Tynan, J., Gerstein, E. D., Burnson, C., Weymouth, L., Bolt, D. M., Maleck, S., & Schwichtenberg, A. J. (2015). Risk and resilience in preterm children at age 6. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 843858. doi:10.1017/S095457941400087XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quigley, M. A., Poulsen, G., Boyle, E., Wolke, D., Field, D., Alfirevic, Z., & Kurinczuk, J. J. (2012). Early term and late preterm birth are associated with poorer school performance at age 5 years: a cohort study. Archives of Disease in Childhood–Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 97, F167F1773. doi:10.1136/archdischild-2011-300888CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ribeiro, L., Zachrisson, H. D., Schjolberg, S., Aase, H., Rohrer-Baumgartner, N., & Magnus, P. (2011). Attention problems and language development in preterm low-birth-weight children: Cross-lagged relations from 18 to 36 months. BMC Pediatrics, 11, 59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richter, J., & Janson, H. (2007). A validation study of the Norwegian version of the Ages and Stages Questionnaires. Acta Paediatrica, 96, 748752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squires, J., Bricker, D., & Potter, L. (1997). Revision of a parent-completed development screening tool: Ages and Stages Questionnaires. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 22, 313328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
StataCorp. (2017). Stata Statistical Software: Release 15. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP.Google Scholar
Stene-Larsen, K., Brandlistuen, R. E., Lang, A. M., Landolt, M. A., Latal, B., & Vollrath, M. E. (2014). Communication impairments in early term and late preterm children: A prospective cohort study following children to age 36 months. The Journal of Pediatrics, 165, 11231128. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2014.08.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strand, B. H., Dalgard, O. S., Tambs, K., & Rognerud, M. (2003). Measuring the mental health status of the Norwegian population: A comparison of the instruments SCL-25, SCL-10, SCL-5 and MHI-5 (SF-36). Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 57, 113118. doi:10.1080/08039480310000932CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Susser, E., Eide, M. G., & Begg, M. (2010). Invited commentary: The use of sibship studies to detect familial confounding. American Journal of Epidemiology, 172, 537539. doi:10.1093/aje/kwq196CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Noort-van der Spek, I. L. Franken, M. C., & Weisglas-Kuperus, N. (2012). Language functions in preterm-born children: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 129, 745754. doi:10.1542/peds.2011-1728CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolke, D., Samara, M., Bracewell, M., & Marlow, N. (2008). Specific language difficulties and school achievment in children born at 25 weeks gestation or less. The Journal of Pediatrics, 152, 256262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zambrana, I. M., Pons, F., Eadie, P., & Ystrom, E. (2014). Trajectories of language delay from age 3 to 5: Persistence, recovery and late onset. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 49, 304316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zambrana, I. M., Vollrath, M. E., Sengpiel, V., Jacobsson, B., & Ystrom, E. (2016). Preterm delivery and risk for early language delays: A sibling-control cohort study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 45, 151159. doi:10.1093/ije/dyv329CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of the sibling sample from MoBa (N = 26,769)

Figure 1

Table 2. Relative risk (RR) for language delay at 1.5, 3, and 5 years by gestational weeks (N = 26,769)

Supplementary material: File

Zambrana et al. supplementary material

Table S2

Download Zambrana et al. supplementary material(File)
File 30.8 KB