Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-2xdlg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-04T17:10:57.206Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Age Differences in the Effects of Reverberation and Pause on Sentence Intelligibility

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 November 2010

Heather Davidson
Affiliation:
University of Calgary
David Schonfield
Affiliation:
University of Calgary
Richard Winkelaar
Affiliation:
Foothills Hospital, Calgary

Abstract

Sentence intelligibility as a function of reverberation time and the presence or absence of a pause was measured in three age groups by means of the Kent State Speech Discrimination Test. Young, middle aged, and old subjects listened to sentences recorded with approximately zero, one or three seconds reverberation time and with half containing a meaningful pause. In a second study, a different group of participants listened to an undistorted recording of the test to provide some information on the standardization of the Kent State Speech Discrimination Test across age groups. All subjects, who were prescreened for audiological normal hearing, were tested for recognition of one key word in each sentence immediately following presentation. Reverberation of three seconds reduced intelligibility for all groups. Age differences which occurred at the longest reverberation time disappeared when a pause was inserted. However, only pauses before a key word were effective. Unmasking of reverberation was suggested as a possible factor in the pause position effect. Practical applications of results are also discussed.

Résumé

Le test de Kent State Speech Discrimination était utilisé pour mesurer le degré de compréhension d'une phrase après y avoir introduit de la réverberation avec pause ou sans pause. Un groupe de jeunes, d'adultes ainsi que de personnes agées, possédant tous une audition normale, ont participé a cette expérience. Il s'agissait pour eux d'écouter des phrases comprenant deux variable importantes, soient: une pause située au milieu de ces phrases de mêune réverbération de zéro, une ou trois secondes. Dans une seconde étude, un groupe différent de participants a écouté une bande enregistrée provenant du test de Kent State Speech Discrimination dans le but de fournir quelques informations supplémentaires afin de normaliser les résultats déjà obtenus à travers les différents groupes d'âges. Les sujets devaient reconnaître un mot clé dans chaque phrase immédiatement après leur présentation. Il a été démontré qu'une réverbération de trois secondes a réduit pour tous les groups d'âge l'intelligibilité des mots. Et, lorsqu'une pause est insérée dans le même genre de phrase, les résultats obtenus sont similaires pour tous. Il à noter que cette pause devient effective seulment lorsqu'elle est placée avant le mot clé, et no pas dans la dernière partie de la phrase. A la suite de ces résultats l'hypothèse suivante a été soulevée: si la pause précède le mot clé, permettant ainsi à la réverbération de disparaître, l'intelligibilité de celui-ci s'améliorerait. Des applications pratiques sont aussi discutés.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Canadian Association on Gerontology 1982

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

American National Standards Institute. Criteria for background noise in audiometrie rooms (S3.1 1960). New York: American National Standards Institute, 1960.Google Scholar
American National Standards Institute. Specifications for audiometers (S3.6 1969). New York: American National Standards Institute, 1969.Google Scholar
Berger, K.W.A speech discrimination test using multiple-choice key words in sentences. Journal of Auditory Research, 1969, 3, 247262.Google Scholar
Bergman, M.Hearing and Aging: Implications of recent research findings. Audiologv, 1971, 10, 164171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bergman, M., Blumenfeld, V.G., Cascardo, D., Leavitt, H. & Margulies, M.K.Age-related decrements in hearing for speech. Journal of Gerontology, 1976, 31, 533538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, S.S. & Geha-Mitzel, M. Coping with sensory losses in aging. In Ordy, J.M. & Brizzee, K.R. (Eds.), Sensory Systems and Communication in the Elderly. New York: Raven Press, 1979.Google Scholar
Korabie, E.W., Freeman, B.A. & Church, G.T.Intelligibility of time-expanded speech with normal hearing and elderly subjects. Audiology, 1978, 17, 159164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nabelek, A.K. Reverberation effects for normal and hearing-impaired listeners. In Hirsh, S.K., Eldredge, D.H., Hirsh, I.J. & Silverman, S.R. (Eds.), Hearing and Davis. St. Louis: Washington University Press, 1976.Google Scholar
Schon, T.D.The effects on speech intelligibility of time-compression and expansion on normal-hearing, hard-of-hearing, and aged males. Journal of Auditorv Research, 1970, 10, 263268.Google Scholar
Schubert, K.Sprachhörprüfmethoden. Stuttgart: Georgthieme Verlag, 1958.Google Scholar
Welford, A.T.Ageing and Human Skill. London: Oxford University Press, 1958.Google Scholar
Wilkes, A.L. Encoding processes and pausing behaviour. In Kennedy, A. & Wilkes, A.L., (Eds.), Studies in Long-Term Memory. London: Wiley Press, 1975.Google Scholar