Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-swr86 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-19T17:32:34.495Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Why Transcortical Reflexes?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 September 2015

Mario Wiesendanger*
Affiliation:
Department of Physiology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada N6A
Dieter G. Rüegg
Affiliation:
Department of Physiology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada N6A
Gregory E. Lucier
Affiliation:
Department of Physiology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada N6A
*
Institut de Physiologie, Université de FribourgPérolles, CH-1700 Fribourg, Switzerland
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary:

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Experiments in humans and in monkeys have indicated that load perturbations, occurring during voluntary movements and postural activity, may be automatically compensated for. Overall muscle stiffness opposing load changes is determined by the viscoelastic properties of the muscle, by segmental reflex actions and finally by long-loop reflexes. Under certain circumstances, for instance when the subject or the experimental monkey is “prepared” to counteract perturbations which are unpredictable in time, the long-loop “reflexes” appear to be responsible for most of the corrective muscle tension. Experiments in anaesthetized monkeys revealed that signals from stretch afferents reach neurons of the motor cortex, possibly via a relay in the cortical area 3a. The latencies of these responses to well controlled muscle stretches were in the same range as motor cortical cell discharges recorded in alert monkeys subjected to load perturbations. Furthermore, these responses of cells in the motor cortex also had the appropriate timing to indicate a causal relationship with the long-latency electromyographic responses to load changes referred to above. These experimental results therefore strongly support the hypothesis, first proposed by Phillips (1969), of a transcortical servoloop adjusting motor cortical output according to the load conditions in which movements are performed.

The major advantage of transcortical regulations as opposed to segmental regulations, seems to be a powerful gain control acting at the cortical level; it was repeatedly shown that the long-loop reflexes are strongly modifiable and under voluntary control. It is suggested that an adaptive gain control at the cortical level is a prerequisite to preserve the complex capabilities of the motor cortex as the chief "executive" for skilled, preprogrammed movements. A loss of this adaptive gain control may be, at least partly, the cause of motor disorders such as rigidity in Parkinsonian patients, as reported by Tatton and Lee (1975). It is suggested that further investigations of the control of transcortical reflexes may aid in the understanding of the pathophysiology of motor disabilities.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Canadian Neurological Sciences Federation 1975

References

REFERENCES

Bell, C. (1826). On the nervous circle which connects the voluntary muscles with the brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 116, 163173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conrad, B., Matsunami, K., Meyerlohmann, J., Wiesendanger, M. and Brooks, V.B. (1974). Cortical load compensation during voluntary elbow movements. Brain Research, 71, 507514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conrad, B., Meyer-Lohmann, J., Matsunami, K. and Brooks, V.B. (1975). Precentral unit activity following torque pulse injections into elbow movements. Brain Research, (In press).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Euler, (1966). Proprioceptive control in respiration. In: (Granit, R., ed.). Muscular afferents and motor control, Nobel Symposium I. Almqvist and Wiksell, Stockholm, pp. 197207.Google Scholar
Evarts, E.V. (1973). Motor cortex reflexes associated with learned movement. Science, 179, 501503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evarts, E.V. and Tanji, J. (1974). Gating of motor cortex reflexes by prior instruction. Brain Research, 71, 479494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grillner, S. (1973). Muscle stiffness and motor control — forces in the ankle during locomotion and standing. In: (Gydikov, A.A., Tankov, N.T. and Kosavov, D.S., eds.) Motor Control. Plenum Press, New York, pp. 195215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammond, P.H. (1956). The influence of prior instruction to the subject on an apparently neuromuscular response. Journal of Physiology, 132, 1718P.Google Scholar
Hammond, P.H., Merton, P.A. and Sutton, G.G. (1956). Nervous gradation of muscular contraction. British Medical Bulletin, 12, 214218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucier, G.E., Rüegg, D.G. and Wiesendanger, M. (1975). Responses of neurons in motor cortex and in area 3a to controlled stretches of forelimb muscles in Cebus monkeys. Journal of Physiology. (In press.)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marin-Padilla, M. (1969). Origin of the pericellular baskets of the pyramidal cells of the human motor cortex: A Golgi study. Brain Research, 14, 633646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsden, C.D. (1973). Servo control, the stretch reflex and movement in man. In: (Desmedt, J.E., ed.). New developments in electromyography and clinical neurophysiology. Karger, Basel, Vol. 3, pp. 375382.Google Scholar
Marsden, C.D., Merton, P.A. and Morton, H.B. (1973). Latency measurements compatible with a cortical pathway for the stretch reflex in man. Journal of Physiology, 230, 5859P.Google ScholarPubMed
Matthews, P.B.C. (1972). Mammalian muscle receptors and their central actions. London: Edward Arnold Limited.Google Scholar
Melvill Jones, G. and Watt, D.G.D. (1971). Observations on the control of stepping and hopping movements in man. Journal of Physiology, 219, 709727.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milner-Brown, H.S., Stein, R.B. and Lee, R.G. (1975). Synchronization of human motor units: possible roles of exercise and supraspinal reflexes. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 38, 245254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, J.T., Wong, Y.C. and Kwan, H.C. (1975). Afferent-efferent linkages in motor cortex for single forelimb muscles. Journal of Neurophysiology, (In press).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, C.G. (1969). Motor apparatus of the baboon’s hand. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London; Biological Sciences, 173, 183198.Google ScholarPubMed
Phillips, C.G., Powell, T.P.S. and Wiesendanger, M. (1971). Projection from low-threshold muscle afferents of hand and forearm to area 3a of baboon’s cortex. Journal of Physiology, 217, 419446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sloper, J.J. (1973). An electron microscope study of the termination of afferent connections to the primate motor cortex. Journal of Neurocytology, 2, 361368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tatton, W.G., Forner, F.D., Gerstein, G.L., Chambers, W.W. and Liu, C.N. (1975). The effect of postcentral cortical lesions on motor responses to sudden upper limb displacements in monkeys. Brain Research, (In press).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tatton, W.G. and Lee, R.G. (1975). Motor responses to sudden upper limb displacements in primates, humans and Parkinsonian patients. Canada Physiology, 6, 60.Google Scholar
Wiesendanger, M. (1972). Pathophysiology of muscle tone. Neurology Series. Springer, Berlin - Heidelberg - New York, Vol. 9, pp. 1719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiesendanger, M. (1973). Input from muscle and cutaneous nerves of the hand and forearm to neurons of the precentral gyrus of baboons and monkeys. Journal of Physiology, 228, 203219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiesendanger, M.Séguin, J.J. and Künzle, H. (1973). The supplementary motor area — a control system for posture? In: (Stein, R.B., Pearson, K.B., Smith, R.S., and Redford, J.B., eds.). Control of posture and locomotion. Plenum Press, New York, pp. 331346.Google Scholar
Zander Olsen, P. and Diamantopoulos, E. (1967). Excitability of spinal neurons in normal subjects and patients with spasticity, Parkinsonian rigidity, and cerebellar hypotonia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 30, 325331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar