Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-nmvwc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-17T04:19:47.047Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

17 - Mediation in Cognitive Socialization

The Influence of Socioeconomic Status

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Pedro R. Portes
Affiliation:
College of Education, University of Louisville, Louisville, Kentucky
Jennifer A. Vadeboncoeur
Affiliation:
School of Education, The University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Queensland, Australia
Alex Kozulin
Affiliation:
International Center for the Enhancement of Learning Potential, Jerusalem
Boris Gindis
Affiliation:
Touro College, New York
Vladimir S. Ageyev
Affiliation:
State University of New York, Buffalo
Suzanne M. Miller
Affiliation:
State University of New York, Buffalo
Get access

Summary

Social activities and adult–child interactions form the basis for cognitive socialization. The extent to which everyday activities differ by socioeconomic status (SES), and the extent to which they are employed differently, becomes a contentious topic when different outcomes in social and academic competence are linked. Socialization results in attitudes, values, and cognitive and linguistic skills that children use as they grow and ultimately become means or tools for development. Children develop competencies through various patterns of adult–child and other social interactions. Often what is seen as important and valuable for socialization varies across communities. Only some of the literature on adult–child interaction addresses elements of SES differentiated activity settings – for example, family structures, scripts, values, and task demands – that are historically embedded. This chapter focuses on a cultural–historical analysis of research linking SES to variations in adult–child interaction.

Although the scope of the chapter limits the extent to which these topics may be explored, a sample of research is examined and framed within the structure provided by Cultural–Historical Activity Theory (CHAT), along with examples of adult–child interaction research approached from a CHAT perspective. CHAT is characterized by a developmental and social analysis of human action that is mediated generally by different cultural tools. To understand differences in individual development, variations in the latter must be considered across activity settings, as well as the way a person responds in terms of shared values, expectations, and practices.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anyon, J. (1980). Social class and the hidden curriculum of work. Journal of Education, 162(1), 67–92Google Scholar
Apple, M. (1989). American realities: Poverty, economy, and education. In L. Weis, E. Farrar, & H. Petrie (Eds.), Drop outs in schools: Issues, dilemmas and solutions (pp. 205–224). Albany, NY: SUNY Press
Baldwin, A. L., Kalhorn, J., & Breese, F. H. (1945). Patterns of parent behavior. Psychological Monographs, 58 (3, Whole No. 299)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernstein, B. (1962). Linguistic codes, hesitation phenomena and intelligence. Language and Speech, 5, 31–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bourdieu, P. (1987). Distinction: A social critique on the judgment of taste. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (1976). Schooling in capitalist AmeriCA: Educational reform and the contradictions of economic life. New York: Basic Books
Bryk, A., & Thum, Y. (1989). The effects of high school organization on dropping out: An exploratory investigation. American Educational Research Journal, 26, 353–383CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caldwell, B., & Bradley, R. (1984). Observation of the home environment. Little Rock: University of Arkansas
Carlson, E. A.& Sroufe, L. A., Collins, W. A.& Jimerson, S.& Weinfield, N.& Hennighausen, K., Egeland, B.& Hyson, D. M.& Anderson, F., & Meyer, S. E. (1999). Early environmental support and elementary school adjustment as predictors of school adjustment in middle adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Research, 14(1), 72–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Center for Research on Education, Diversity & Excellence (CREDE). (2002). Five Standards for Effective Pedagogy and Student Outcomes (Technical Report No. G1). Santa Cruz, CA: CREDE
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Cole, M.& Gay, J., & Glick, J. (1968). A cross-cultural investigation of information processing. International Journal of Psychology, 3, 93–102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Comer, J. P. (1990). Home, school and academic learning In J. Goodlad & P. Keating (Eds.), Access to knowledge: An agenda for our nation's schools (pp. 23–42). New York: College Entrance Examination Board
The Condition of Education. (1998). Washington, DC: United States Government Printing Office
Conroy, M., Hess, R., Azuma, H., & Kashiwagi, K. (1980). Maternal strategies for regulating children's behavior: Japanese and American families. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 11, 153–172CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diaz, R. M., Neal, C. J., & Amaya-Williams, M. (1991). Social origins of self-regulation. In L. Moll (Ed.), Vygotsky and education: Instructional implications and applications of sociohistorical psychology (pp. 127–154). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Dunham, R. M., Kidwell, J. S., & Portes, P. R. (1988). Effects of parent-adolescent interaction on the continuity of cognitive development from early childhood to early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 8(3), 97–130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunham, R. M., Kidwell, J. S., & Portes, P. R. (1995). Do the seeds of accelerative learning and teaching lie in a behavioral carrier wave?Journal of Accelerated Learning and Teaching, 20, 53–87Google Scholar
Fine, M. (1989). Framing drop outs: Notes on the politics of an urban high school. Albany, NY: SUNY Press
Gauvin, M., & Rogoff, B. (1989). Collaborative problem solving and children's planning skills. Developmental Psychology, 25, 139–151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1992). American parenting of language-learning children: Persisting differences in family-child interactions observed in natural home environments. Developmental Psychology, 28(6), 1096–1105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life, and work in communities and classrooms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Henderson, B. B. (1991, February). Describing parent-child interaction during exploration: Situation definitions and negotiations. Genetic, Social and General Psychology Monographs, 117(1), 77–89Google ScholarPubMed
Hess, R., & Shipman, V. (1965). Early experience and the socialization of cognitive modes in children. Child Development, 36, 377–388CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isabella, R., & Belsky, J. (1991). Interactional synchrony and the origins of infant-mother attachment: A replication study. Child Development, 60, 103-118Google Scholar
Jackson, P. W. (1956). Verbal solutions to parent-child problems. Child Development, 27, 339-349Google ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J., Moss, H. A., & Sigel, I. E. (1963). Psychological significance of styles of conceptualization. In J. E. Wright, & J. Kagan (Eds.), Basic cognitive processes in children. Monograph of the Society for Research in Child Development, 28(2)
Karpov, Y., & Bransford, J. D. (1995). L. S. Vygotsky: The doctrine of empirical and theoretical learning. Educational Psychologist, 30, 61–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohn, M. L. (1977). Class and conformity: A study of values (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Kozulin, A. (1990). Vygotsky's psychology: A biography of ideas. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Kucznski, L., & Kochanska, G. (1990). Development of children's noncompliance strategies from toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology, 26, 398–408CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laosa, L. M. (1981). Parent-child interaction: Theory, research and prospects. New York: Academic Press
Lazar, I., & Darlington, R. (1982). Lasting effects of early education: A report from the consortium for longitudinal studies. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 47(2–3), 1–151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lytton, H. (1990). Child and parent effects in boys' conduct disorders: A reinterpretation. Developmental Psychology, 26, 683–697CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mickelson, R. A. (1990). The attitude-achievement paradox among Black adolescents. Sociology of Education, 63, 44–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murdock, T. B. (1999). The social context of risk: Status and motivational predictors of alienation in middle school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(1), 62–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakes, J. (1985). Keeping track: How schools structure inequality. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press
Palincsar, A. M., & Brown, A. L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension-fostering and comprehension monitoring activities. Cognition and Instruction, 1, 117–175Google Scholar
Panofsky, C. P. (1999). Getting to the heart of the matter: Literacy as value commitments. In R. J. Telfer, (Ed.), American reading forum (vol. 19). Whitewater: University of Wisconsin
Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family processes. Eugene, OR: Castalia Press
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books
Portes, P. R. (1988). Maternal verbal regulation and intellectual development. Roeper Review, 11(2), 106–110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Portes, P. R. (1991). Assessing children's cognitive environments through parent-child interaction: Estimation of a general zone of proximal development in relation to scholastic achievement. Journal of Research in Education, 24(3), 30–38Google Scholar
Portes, P. R. (1996). Ethnicity in education and psychology. In D. Berliner & R. Calfee (Eds.), The handbook of educational psychology. New York: McMillan
Portes, P. R. (1999). Social and psychological factors in academic achievement of children of immigrants: A cultural history puzzle. American Educational Research Journal, 36, 489–507CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Portes, P. R., Cuentas, T. E., & Zady, M. F. (2000). Cognitive socialization across ethnocultural contexts: Literacy and cultural differences in intellectual performance and parent-child interaction. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 161(1), 79–98CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Portes, P. R., Dunham, R. M., & Williams, S. (1986). Preschool intervention, social class and parent-child interaction differences. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 147(2), 241–255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Portes, P. R., Smith, T. L., Zady, M. F., & Del Castillo, K. (1997). Extending the double stimulation method in cultural-historical research: Parent-child interaction and cognitive change. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 4(2), 108–123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Portes, P. R., Zady, M. F., & Dunham, R. M. (1998). The effects of parents' assistance on middle school students' problem solving and achievement. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 159(2), 163–178CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radziszewska, B., & Rogoff, B. (1991). Children's guided participation in planning imaginary errands with skilled adult or peer partners. Developmental Psychology, 27(3), 381–389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogoff, B., & Gardner, W. (1984). Adult guidance of cognitive development. In B. Rogoff & J. Lave (Eds.), Everyday cognition: Its development in social context (pp. 95–150). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Roopnarine, J. L., & Carter, D. B. (Eds.) (1992). Annual advances in applied developmental psychology (Vol. 5). Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Saxe, G. (1994). Studying cognitive developments in CHAT context: The development of a practice-based approach. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 1, 135–157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saxon, T. F., & Reilly, J. T. (1999). Joint attention and toddler characteristics: Race, sex, and socioeconomic status. Early Child Development and Care, 149, 59-69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scribner, S., & Cole, M. (1981). The psychology of literacy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Sigel, I. E. (1982). The relationship between parents' distancing strategies and the child's cognitive behavior. In L. M. Laosa, & I. E. Sigel (Eds.), Families as learning environments for children (pp. 47–86). New York: PlenumCrossRef
Sigel, I. E., Anderson, L. M., & Shapiro, H. (1966). Categorization behavior of lower- and middle-class negro preschool children: Differences in dealing with representation of familiar objects. Journal of Negro Education, 35(3), 218–229CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sigel, I. E., Stinson, E. T., & Flaugher, J. (1991). Socialization of representational competence in the family: The distancing paradigm. In L. Okagaki & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Directors of development: Influences on the development of children's thinking (pp. 121–144). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Sigel, I. E., Stinson, E. T., & Kim, M. (1993). Socialization of cognition: The distancing model. In K. W. Fischer & R. Wozniak (Eds.), Specific environments: Thinking in contexts. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books
Tharp, R. G., & Gallimore, R. (1988). Rousing minds to life: Teaching, learning, and schooling in social context. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Tzuriel, D. (1996). Mediated learning experience in free-play versus structured situations among preschool children of low-, medium-, and high-SES. Early Childhood Development and Care, 126, 57–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valsiner, J. (1984). Construction of the zone of proximal development in adult-child joint action: The socialization of meals. In B. Rogoff & J. Wertsch (Eds.), Children's learning in the zone of proximal development (pp. 65–76). San Francisco: Jossey-BassCrossRef
Valsiner, J. (1989). Human development and culture: The social nature of personality and its study. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books
Valsiner, J. (1998). The guided mind: A sociogenetic approach to personality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Weisner, T. S. (1984). Ecocultural niches of middle childhood: A cross cultural perspective. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Development during middle childhood: The years from six to twelve (pp. 335–369). Washington, DC: National Academy of the Sciences Press
Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the mind: A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Wertsch, J. V., Minick, N., & Arns, R. J. (1984). The creation of context in joint problem solving: A cross-cultural study. In B. Rogoff & J. Lave (Eds.), Everyday cognition: Its development in social context. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
White, K. R. (1982). The relation between socioeconomic status and academic achievement. Psychological Bulletin, 91(3), 461–481CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (1992). The development of achievement task values: A theoretical analysis. Developmental Review, 12, 265–310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, D. J., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem-solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89–100CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zinchenko, V. P. (2001). External and internal: Another comment on the issue. In S. Chaiklin (Ed.), The theory and practice of cultural-historical psychology (pp. 135–147). Aarhus, Denmark: Aarhus University Press

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×