Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-8zxtt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-11T22:19:27.216Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Communication in spider monkeys: the function and mechanisms underlying the use of the whinny

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 May 2010

Gabriel Ramos-Fernández
Affiliation:
Instituto Politécnico Nacional, Santa Cruz Xoxocotlán, Oaxaca
Christina J. Campbell
Affiliation:
California State University, Northridge
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Several aspects of the biology of spider monkeys (Ateles spp.) make their communication system particularly interesting. First, as canopy-dwelling, frugivorous primates, spider monkeys must forage in a complex environment consisting of fruit patches of variable size that are spatially distributed over large areas (Klein, 1972; van Roosmalen and Klein, 1987). Such an environment places a high demand on those mechanisms by which individuals can efficiently coordinate their movements, exploit their food resources and avoid danger (Milton, 2000). Additionally, because of their fluid grouping and association patterns (Aureli and Schaffner, this volume), individual spider monkeys may spend long periods of time away from others in the group (Symington, 1990; Ramos-Fernández, 2005). This implies that at times, long-distance vocalizations, which can overcome spatial separation, or olfactory marks, which can overcome temporal separation, may be the only means by which spider monkeys can locate group members and, in general, maintain their social relationships (Ramos-Fernández, 2005; Aureli et al., in press).

This chapter reviews the studies carried out so far on spider monkey communication, placing more emphasis on the vocal mode, as it has received the most attention. The first section reviews the early, more descriptive studies of communication in spider monkeys and provides a set of definitions of the different vocal types used by different species. Among the different vocalizations used by spider monkeys, only the whinny has been studied in any detail.

Type
Chapter
Information
Spider Monkeys
The Biology, Behavior and Ecology of the Genus Ateles
, pp. 220 - 235
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aureli, F., Schaffner, C. M., Boesch, C.et al. (in press). Fission–fusion dynamics: new research frameworks. Current Anthropology.Google Scholar
Barrett, L., Henzi, P. and Dunbar, R. (2003). Primate cognition: from ‘what now?’ to ‘what if?’Trends Cogn. Sci., 7, 494–497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, C. J. (2000). Fur rubbing behavior in free-ranging black-handed spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) in Panama. Am. J. Primatol., 51, 205–208.3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, C. J. (2006). Copulation in free-ranging black-handed spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi). Am. J. Primatol., 68, 507–511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, C. R. (1935). Behavior of red spider monkeys in Panama. J. Mammal., 16, 171–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, C. A and Lefebvre, L. (1990). Manipulating foraging group size: spider monkey food calls at fruiting trees. Anim. Behav., 39, 891–896.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, C. A. and Weary, D. M. (1990). Variability in spider monkey's vocalizations may provide basis for individual recognition. Am. J. Primatol., 22, 279–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (2004). Social complexity and the information acquired during eavesdropping by primates and other animals. In Animal Communication Networks, ed. McGregor, P. K., Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 583–603.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M. and Palombit, R. (1996). The function and mechanisms underlying baboon “contact” barks. Anim. Behav., 52, 507–518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R. and Krebs, J. R. (1978). Animal signals: information or manipulation? In Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach, ed. Krebs, J. R. and Davies, N. B., Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 282–309.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, J. F. (1976). Communication mechanisms and social integration in the black spider monkey, Ateles fusciceps robustus, and related species. Smithson. Contrib. Zool., 213.Google Scholar
Enlder, J. A. (1993). Some general comments on the evolution and design of animal communication systems. Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B, 340, 215–225.Google Scholar
Evans, C. S. and Marler, P. (1984). Food calling and audience effects in male chickens, Gallus gallus: their relationships to food availability, courtship and social facilitation. Anim. Behav., 47, 1159–1170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guilford, T. and Dawkins, M. X. (1991). Receiver psychology and the evolution of animal signals. Anim. Behav., 42, 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M. D. (1996). The Evolution of Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Heinrich, B. and Marzluff, J. M. (1991). Do common ravens yell because they want to attract others?Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 28, 13–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herzel, H. (1993). Bifurcations and chaos in voice signals. App. Mechan. Rev., 46, 399–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herzel, H., Berry, D., Titze, I. and Steinecke, I. (1995). Nonlinear dynamics of the voice – signal analysis and biomechanical modeling. Chaos, 5, 30–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, L. L. (1972). The ecology and social organization of the spider monkey, Ateles belzebuth. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Laska, M., Bauer, V. and Salazar, Hernandez L. T. (2007). Self-annointing behavior in free-ranging spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) in Mexico. Primates, 48, 160–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masataka, N. (1986). Rudimentary representational vocal signalling of fellow group members in spider monkeys. Behaviour, 96, 49–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
May, B., Moody, D. B. and Stebbins, W. C. (1989). Categorical perception of conspecific communication sounds by Japanese macaques, Macaca fuscata. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 85, 837–847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGregor, P. K. (1993). Signalling in territorial systems: a context for individual identification, ranging and eavesdropping. Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B, 340, 237–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, P. K. and Dabelsteen, T. (1996). Communication networks. In Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds, ed. Kroodsma, D. E. and Miller, E. H., Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press, pp. 409–425.Google Scholar
McGregor, P. K. and Peake, T. M. (2000). Communication networks: social environments for receiving and signalling behaviour. Acta Ethologica, 2, 71–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milton, K. (2000). Quo vadis? Tactics of food search and group movement in primates and other animals. In On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel in Groups, ed. Boinski, S. and Garber, P. A., Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, pp. 375–417.Google Scholar
Naguib, M. and Wiley, R. H. (2001). Estimating the distance to a source of sound: mechanisms and adaptations for long-range communication. Anim. Behav., 62, 825–837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramos-Fernández, G. (2000). Patterns of association, feeding competition and vocal communication in spider monkeys, Ateles geoffroyi. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
Ramos-Fernández, G. (2005). Vocal communication in a fission–fusion society: do spider monkeys stay in touch with close associates?Int. J. Primatol., 26, 1077–1092.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramos-Fernández, G., Boyer, D. and Gómez, V. P. (2006). A complex social structure with fission–fusion properties can emerge from a simple foraging model. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 60, 536–549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Roosmalen, M. G. M. and Klein, L. L. (1987). The spider monkeys, genus Ateles. In Ecology and Behavior of Neotropical Primates, ed. Mittermeier, R. A. and Rylands, A. B., Washington DC: World Wide Fund for Nature, pp. 455–537.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M. and Cheney, D. L. (2003). Signalers and receivers in animal communication. Ann. Rev. Psychol., 54, 145–173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. and Marler, P. (1980). Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science, 210, 801–803.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, W. J. (1977). The Behavior of Communicating. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Symington, M. M. (1987). Ecological and social correlates of party size in the black spider monkey, Ateles paniscus chamek. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Princeton University, NJ.Google Scholar
Symington, M. M. (1990). Fission–fusion social organization in Ateles and Pan. Int. J. Primatol., 11, 47–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teixidor, P. and Byrne, R. W. (1997). Can spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) discriminate vocalizations of familiar individuals and strangers?Folia Primatol., 68, 254–264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teixidor, P. and Byrne, R. W. (1999). The ‘whinny’ of spider monkeys: individual recognition before situational meaning. Behaviour, 136, 279–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wahlberg, M., Ramos-Fernández, G., Ugarte, F., M⊘hl, B. and Rasch, M. (2002). Recording spider monkeys with a microphone array. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 112(5), Pt.2, 2400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walters, J. R. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1987). Conflict and cooperation. In Primate Societies, ed. Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W. and Struhsaker, T. T., Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, pp. 306–317.Google Scholar
Waser, P. M. (1977). Sound localization by monkeys: a field experiment. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 2, 427–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×