Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-vpsfw Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T01:13:03.206Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Habits and the Structure of Motivation in Everyday Life

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Wendy Wood
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Duke University
Jeffrey M. Quinn
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Duke University
Joseph P. Forgas
Affiliation:
University of New South Wales, Sydney
Kipling D. Williams
Affiliation:
University of New South Wales, Sydney
Simon M. Laham
Affiliation:
University of New South Wales, Sydney
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

Many of the motivational challenges in daily life recur at routine intervals. For example, most appetitive drives (e.g., hunger, sleep) are cyclic, and people act to address them at certain times of the day in certain circumstances. Many other goals, such as getting to work or achieving physical fitness, also involve periodic activities that occur in specific, recurring times and places. This regularity in motivated behavior organizes everyday experience into repeated patterns of goal-directed activities in particular circumstances. With repetition of behavior in stable contexts, actions become automatic in the sense that deliberation about behavior becomes unnecessary. These well-practiced behaviors represent habits, and in this chapter we consider the role of habitual behavior in motivational processes.

A role for habits in motivated behavior was outlined early in classic learning theories, which specified how habits emerge from previous motivated responding and how they structure subsequent motivated responding. For example, Hull (1943, 1950) believed that habits are stimulus— response (S—R) linkages that develop when a given response is reinforcing because it successfully reduces a drive state such as hunger or thirst. This reinforcement increases the association between the stimulus and the response and thereby increases habit strength. Thus, habits orient people to repeat activities that have in the past successfully met motivational needs.

Type
Chapter
Information
Social Motivation
Conscious and Unconscious Processes
, pp. 55 - 70
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000a). Habits as knowledge structures: Automaticity in goal-directed behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 53–63CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000b). The automatic activation of goal-directed behaviour: The case of travel habit. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 20, 75–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abelson, R. P. (1981). Psychological status of the script concept. American Psychologist, 36, 715–729CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ajzen, I. (2002). Residual effects of past on later behavior: Habituation and reasoned action perspectives. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 107–122CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Albarracin, D., Kumkale, G. T., & Johnson, B. T. (unpublished). Influences of population and methodological factors on reasoning in condom use: A meta-analysis
Bargh, J. A., & Barndollar, K. (1996). Automaticity in action: The unconscious as repository of chronic goals and motives. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 457–481). New York: Guilford Press
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54, 462–479CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230–244CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1994). Environmental control of goal-directed action: Automatic and strategic contingencies between situations and behavior. In W. D. Spaulding (Ed.), Integrative views of motivation, cognition, and emotion: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 41, pp. 71–124). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (2000). Ego depletion: A resource model of volition, self-regulation, and controlled processing. Social Cognition, 18, 130–150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandstätter, V., Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Implementations intentions and efficient action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 946–960CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, M. B. (2003). Implicit and explicit process in social judgment: Deep and high. In J. P. Forgas, K. D. Williams, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), Social judgments: Implicit and explicit processes (pp. 387–396). New York: Psychology Press
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2001). Optimism, pessimism, and self-regulation. In E. C. Chang (Ed.), Optimism and pessimism: Implications for theory, research, and practice (pp. 31–51). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dijksterhuis, A., Bargh, J. A., & Miedema, J. (2000). Of men and mackerels: Attention and automatic behavior. In H. Bless & J. P. Forgas (Eds.), Subjective experience in social cognition and behavior (pp. 36–51). Philadelphia: Psychology Press
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109–132CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, R. H. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, E., & Bibby, P. A. (2002). Predicting future blood donor returns: Past behavior, intentions, and observer effects. Health Psychology, 21, 513–518CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley
Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychologist, 43, 349–358CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guerrero-Witt, M., & Wood, W. (in progress). Thinking limits on goal success. Research
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, J. F., & Jacoby, L. L. (1996). Separating habit and recollection: Memory slips, process dissociations, and probability matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1323–1335Google ScholarPubMed
Heckhausen, H., & Beckmann, J. (1990). Intentional action and action slips. Psychological Review, 97, 36–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: An introduction to behavior theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts
Hull, C. L. (1950). Behavior postulates and corrolaries – 1949. Psychological Review, 57, 173–180CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelley, C. M., & Jacoby, L. L. (2000). Recollection and familiarity: Process-dissociation. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 215–228). New York: Oxford University Press
Lieberman, M. D., Gaunt, R., Gilbert, D. T., & Trope, Y. (2002). Reflexion and reflection: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to attributional inference. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 199–249). New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Mandler, G. (1975). Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley
McClelland, J. L. (2000). Connectionist models of memory. In E. Tulving & C. I. M. Fergus (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 583–596). London: Oxford University Press
McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: A multiprocess framework. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, S127–S144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ouellette, J. A., & Wood, W. (1998). Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 54–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 55–85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Quinn, J. M., & Wood, W. (under review). Habits across the lifespan. Manuscript
Reason, J. (1979). Actions not as planned: The price of automatisation. In G. Underwood & R. Stevens (Eds.), Aspects of consciousness, Vol. 1: Psychological Issues (pp. 67–89). London: Academic Press
Reason, J. (1984). Absent-mindedness and cognitive control. In J. E. Harris & P. E. Morris (Eds.), Everyday memory: Actions and absent-mindedness (pp. 113–132). London: Academic Press
Reason, J., & Lucas, D. (1984). Using cognitive diaries to investigate naturally occurring memory blocks. In J. E. Harris & P. E. Morris (Eds.), Everyday memory: Actions and absent-mindedness (pp. 53–70). London: Academic Press
Schooler, J. W. (2002). Re-representing consciousness: Dissociations between experience and meta-consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 339–344CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shah, J. Y., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). Priming against your will: How accessible alternatives affect goal pursuit. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 368–383CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 108–131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smolensky, P. (1988). On the proper treatment of connectionism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 1–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the association processes in animals. Psychological Review Monograph, 2 (Whole No. 8)Google Scholar
Thurber, J. (1942). My world – and welcome to it. New York: Harcourt, Brace
Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they're doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 3–15CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verplanken, B., & Aarts, H. (1999). Habit, attitude, and planned behavior: Is habit an empty construct or an interesting case of goal-directed automaticity? In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 101–134). Chichester, England: WileyCrossRef
Verplanken, B., Aarts, H., Knippenberg, A., & Moonen, A. (1998). Habit versus planned behaviour: A field experiment. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 111–128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verplanken, B., & Herabadi, A. (2001). Individual differences in impulse buying tendency: Feeling and no thinking. European Journal of Personality, 15, S71–S83CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, W., Quinn, J. M., & Kashy, D. (2002). Habits in everyday life: Thought, emotion, and action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1281–1297CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, W., Tam, L., & Guerrero-Witt, M. (under review). Changing contexts, disrupting habits. Manuscript

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×