Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T00:28:17.175Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - The neural correlates of self-awareness and self-recognition

from Part II - Cognitive and neurosciences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 December 2009

Julian Paul Keenan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Montclair State University, Upper Montclair, NJ, USA
Mark A. Wheeler
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
Michael Ewers
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
Tilo Kircher
Affiliation:
Eberhard-Karls-Universität Tübingen, Germany
Anthony David
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, London
Get access

Summary

Abstract

Attempts to elucidate the brain correlates of self-awareness have existed for centuries. However, only now is a clear picture emerging. Based on case studies and modern neuroimaging, it appears that the right hemisphere and the prefrontal cortex, probably the right prefrontal cortex, is dominant for self-related processing. Studies implicating the right hemisphere/prefrontal cortex have focused on self-recognition, self-face recognition, autobiographical episodic memory and autonoetic consciousness. We present this evidence and argue that the right hemisphere/prefrontal cortex may be dominant for the self.

Introduction

This chapter attempts to integrate a number of experimental findings pertaining to the neural correlates of the self and self-related processing. In this context, we have previously suggested that the brain be viewed as an Alexander Calder mobile, delicate and balanced, with a certain interdependence amongst the elements (Keenan, 2001). Calder's mobiles are fragile, with wire or string holding together elements typically made of metal in such a manner that perfect balance is achieved across the entire creation. These elements, which on some levels can be described as discrete units, are never wholly independent, and their actions shift as the environment changes. Destruction of a single unit often affects the entire brain, even if the change across distant regions is subtle. Further, different environmental conditions, analogous to different cognitive demands, often involve numerous elements, and the same element is often involved under numerous tasks.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerley, S. S., &Benton, A. L., (1947). Report of a case of bilateral frontal lobe defect. Research Publications: Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease, 27, 479–504Google Scholar
Breen, N., Caine, D., &Coltheart, M., (2001). Mirrored-self misidentification: two cases of focal onset dementia. Neurocase, 7, 239–54Google Scholar
Calabrese, P., Markowitsch, H. J., Durwen, H. F., et al. (1996). Right temporofrontal cortex as critical locus for the ecphory of old episodic memories. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 61, 304–10Google Scholar
Feinberg, T., &Shapiro, R., (1989). Misidentification-reduplication and the right hemisphere. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 2, 39–48Google Scholar
Feinberg, T. E., Haber, L. D., &Leeds, N. E., (1990). Verbal asomatognosia. Neurology, 40, 1391–4Google Scholar
Fink, G. R., Markowitsch, H. J., Reinkemeier, M. et al. (1996). Cerebral representation of one's own past: neural networks involved in autobiographical memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 4275–82Google Scholar
Freeman, W. & Watts, J. W. (1950). Psychosurgery, 2nd edn. Springfield, IL: Charles S. Thomas
Gallup, G. G. (1970). Chimpanzees: self-recognition. Science, 167, 86–7Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G. Jr (1977). Absence of self-recognition in a monkey (Macaca fascicularis) following prolonged exposure to a mirror. Developmental Psychobiology, 10, 281–4Google Scholar
Gardiner, J. M. &Java, R. I. (1990). Recollective experience in word and nonword recognition. Memory and Cognition, 18, 23–30Google Scholar
Howe, M. L. &Courage, M. L. (1997). The emergence and early development of autobiographical memory. Psychological Review, 104, 499–523Google Scholar
Ingvar, D. H. (1985). ‘Memory of the future’: an essay on the temporal organization of conscious awareness. Human Neurobiology, 4, 127–56Google Scholar
Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J. &Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 4302–11Google Scholar
Kanwisher, N., Tong, F. &Nakayama, K. (1998). The effect of face inversion on the human fusiform face area. Cognition, 68, B1–11Google Scholar
Kanwisher, N., Stanley, D. &Harris, A. (1999). The fusiform face area is selective for faces not animals. Neuroreport, 10, 183–7Google Scholar
Keenan, J. P. (2001). A thing done well: a reply to Dr Antti Revonsuo's ‘Can functional brain imaging discover consciousness in the brain?’ Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 31–3Google Scholar
Keenan, J. P., McCutcheon, N. B. &Pascual-Leone, A. (2001a). Functional magnetic resonance imaging and event related potentials suggest right prefrontal activation for self-related processing. Brain and Cognition, 47, 87–91Google Scholar
Keenan, J. P., Nelson, A., O'Connor, M. &Pascual-Leone, A. (2001b). Self-recognition and the right hemisphere. Nature, 409, 305Google Scholar
Kircher, T. T., Senior, C., Phillips, M. L. et al. (2001). Recognizing one's own face. Cognition, 78, B1–15Google Scholar
Kitchen, A., Denton, D. &Brent, L. (1996). Self-recognition and abstraction abilities in the common chimpanzee studied with distorting mirrors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 93, 7405–8Google Scholar
Levine, B., Black, S. E., Cabeza, R., et al. (1998). Episodic memory and the self in a case of isolated retrograde amnesia. Brain 121 1951–73Google Scholar
Lewis, M. & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social Cognition and Acquisition of Self. New York, NY: Plenum
Lewis, M., Sullivan, M. W., Stanger, C. &Weiss, M. (1989). Self development and self-conscious emotions. Child Development, 60, 146–56Google Scholar
Luria, A. R., Pribram, K. H. &Homskaya, E. D. (1964). An experimental analysis of the behavioral disturbance produced by a left frontal arachnoidal endothelioma. Neuropsychologia, 2, 257–80Google Scholar
Markowitsch, H. J. (1995). Which brain regions are critically involved in the retrieval of old episodic memory? Brain Research and Brain Research Reviews, 21, 117–27Google Scholar
Markowitsch, H. J., Calabrese, P., Haupts, M. et al. (1993). Searching for the anatomical basis of retrograde amnesia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 947–67Google Scholar
Markowitsch, H. J., Calabrese, P., Neufeld, H., Gehlen, W. &Durwen, H. F. (1999). Retrograde amnesia for world knowledge and preserved memory for autobiographic events. A case report. Cortex, 35, 243–52Google Scholar
Meador, K. J., Loring, D. W., Feinberg, T. E., Lee, G. P. &Nichols, M. E. (2000). Anosognosia and asomatognosia during intracarotid amobarbital inactivation. Neurology, 55, 816–20Google Scholar
Miller, P. J., Potts, R., Fung, H. &Hoogstra, L. (1990). Narrative practices and the social construction of self in childhood. American Ethnologist, 17, 292–311Google Scholar
Moniz, E. (1936). Tentatives Operatoires dans le Traitement de Certaines Psychoses. Paris: Mason
Patterson, F. (1984). Self-recognition by gorilla Gorilla gorilla. Gorilla, 7, 2–3Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Gallup, G. G. Jr, Eddy, T. J. &Bierschwale, D. T. (1997). Chimpanzees recognize themselves in mirrors. Animal Behaviour, 53, 1083–8Google Scholar
Preilowski, B. (1977). Self-recognition as a test of consciousness in left and right hemisphere of ‘split-brain’ patients. Acta Nervous Super (Praha), 19(suppl. 2), 343–4Google Scholar
Sergent, J. &Gross, C. (1992). Face recognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 2, 156–61Google Scholar
Sergent, J. &Poncet, M. (1990). From covert to overt recognition of faces in a prosopagnosic patient. Brain, 113, 989–1004Google Scholar
Shillito, D. J., Gallup, G. G. Jr, & Beck, B. B. (1999). Factors affecting mirror behaviour in western lowland gorillas, Gorilla gorilla. Animal Behaviour, 57, 999–1004Google Scholar
Spangenberg, K., Wagner, M. &Bachman, D. (1998). Neuropsychological analysis of a case of abrupt onset following a hypotensive crisis in a patient with vascular dementia. Neurocase, 4, 149–54Google Scholar
Sperry, R., Zaidel, E. &Zaidel, D. (1979). Self recognition and social awareness in the deconnected minor hemisphere. Neuropsychologia, 17, 153–66Google Scholar
Stuss, D. & Benson, F. (1986). The frontal lobes and control of cognition and memory. In The Frontal Lobes Revisited, ed. E. Perceman, pp. 141–54. Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Suarez, S. &Gallup, G. (1981). Self-recognition in chimpanzees and orangutans, but not gorillas. Journal of Human Evolution, 10, 175–88Google Scholar
Suarez, S. D. &Gallup, G. G. (1986). Social responding to mirrors in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): effects of changing mirror location. American Journal of Primatology, 11, 239–44Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1987). Multiple memory systems and consciousness. Human Neurobiology, 6, 67–80Google Scholar
Wheeleri, M. A., Stuss, D. T., &Tulving, E., (1997). Toward a theory of episodic memory: the frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 331–54Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×