Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-7drxs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T06:13:33.415Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 December 2017

Nigel Duffield
Affiliation:
Konan University, Japan
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Reflections on Psycholinguistic Theories
Raiding the Inarticulate
, pp. 347 - 375
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abe, J. (2014). A Movement Theory of Anaphora. Studies in Generative Grammar, vol. 120. Boston, MA, and Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abney, S. (1987). The English noun-phrase in its sentential aspect. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Abrahamsson, N. (2012). Age of onset and native-like L2 ultimate attainment of morphosyntactic and phonetic intuition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 34: 187214.Google Scholar
Adger, D. (2013). Constructions are not explanations. Lingbuzz 001675. http://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/001675Google Scholar
Aitchison, J. (2003). Psycholinguistic perspectives on language change. In Joseph, B. and Janda, R., eds, The Handbook of Historical Linguistics, pp. 736–43. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Akhtar, N. and Tomasello, M. (1997). Young children’s productivity with word order and verb morphology. Developmental Psychology 33 (6): 952–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alario, F.-X., Costa, A., Ferreira, S. V. and Pickering, M. J. (2006). Architectures, representations and processes of language production. Language and Cognitive Processes 21: 777–89.Google Scholar
Allen, J. and Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). The emergence of grammaticality in connectionist networks. In MacWhinney, B, ed., The Emergence of Language, pp. 115–52. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Altmann, G. T. M. (1989). Parsing and interpretation: An introduction. Language and Cognitive Processes 4: SI1SI19.Google Scholar
Altmann, G. T. M. (1998). Ambiguity in sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Science 2 (4): 146–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alves, M. (1999). What’s so Chinese about Vietnamese? In Thurgood, G., ed., Papers from the Ninth Annual Meeting of the South East Asian Linguistics Society 1999, pp. 221–41. Arizona: Arizona State University.Google Scholar
Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., Rowland, C. F., Chang, F. and Bidgood, A. (2013). The retreat from overgeneralization in child language acquisition: Word learning, morphology, and verb argument structure. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 4 (1): 4762.Google Scholar
Andersen, H. (1978). Perceptual and conceptual factors in abductive innovations. In Fisiak, J., ed., Recent Developments in Historical Phonology, pp. 122. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Andersson, L.-G. (1998). Some languages are harder than others. In Bauer, L. and Trudgill, P., eds, Language Myths, pp. 50–7. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Andics, A., Gábor, A., Gácsi, M., Faragó, T., Szabó, D. and Miklósi, Á. (2016). Neural mechanisms for lexical processing in dogs. Science 353 (6303): 1030–32.Google Scholar
Andreou, G. and Katsarou, D. (2013). Language learning in children with Down Syndrome: Receptive and expressive morphosyntactic abilities. Procedia: Social and Behavioral Sciences 93: 921–4.Google Scholar
Aoun, J., Hornstein, N., Lightfoot, D. and Weinberg, A. (1987). Two types of locality. Linguistic Inquiry 18: 537–78.Google Scholar
Aoun, J. and Li, Y.-H. A. (1989). Scope and constituency. Linguistic Inquiry 20: 141–72.Google Scholar
Aoun, J. and Li, Y.-H. A. (1993). The Syntax of Scope. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Aronoff, M. (1976). Word Formation in Generative Grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Authier, J.-M. (1992). Iterated CPs and embedded topicalization. Linguistic Inquiry 23: 329–36.Google Scholar
Axel, K. and Kitziak, T. (2007). Contributing to the extraction/parenthesis debate: Judgement data and historical data. In Featherston, S. and Sternefeld, W., eds, Roots: Linguistics in Search of its Evidential Base, pp. 2952. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Bacon, R. (Nolan, E., ed.) (1902). Grammatica Graeca [Greek Grammar]. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bailey, C.-J. N. and Maroldt, K. (1977). The French lineage of English. In Meisel, J., ed., Pidgins – Creoles – Languages in Contact, pp. 2153. Tübingen: Günther Narr.Google Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (1987). Object permanence in 3½-and 4½-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology 23: 655–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (2002). The acquisition of physical knowledge in infancy: A summary in eight lessons. In Goswami, U., ed., Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development, pp. 4683. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M. and Bian, L. (2016). Psychological reasoning in infancy. Annual Review of Psychology 67: 159–86.Google Scholar
Baker, M. (2001). The Atoms of Language: The Mind’s Hidden Rules of Grammar. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Baker, M. (2008). The macroparameter in a microparametric world. In Biberauer, T., ed., The Limits of Syntactic Variation, pp. 351–73. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Baker, M., Johnson, K. and Roberts, I. (1989). Passive arguments raised. Linguistic Inquiry 20: 219–51.Google Scholar
Baltin, M. (1987). Do antecedent-contained deletions exist? Linguistic Inquiry 18: 579–95.Google Scholar
Barlow, M. and Kemmer, S. (2000). Usage-Based Models of Language. Stanford, CA: Center for the Study of Language and Information.Google Scholar
Barton, E. (1986). Interacting models: Constituent structures and constituent utterances. In Farley, A. M., Farley, P. T. and McCullough, K.-E., eds, CLS 22: Papers from the Parasession on Pragmatics and Grammatical Theory, pp. 140–51. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Basbøll, H. (2005). The Phonology of Danish. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E. and Goodman, J. C. (1997). On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisition, aphasia and realtime processing. Language and Cognitive Processes 12 (5/6): 507–84.Google Scholar
Bauer, H. H. (1994). Scientific Literacy and the Myth of the Scientific Method. Urbana-Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Behme, C. (2013). Remarks on recursive misrepresentations by Legate et al. (2013). Lingbuzz 001840. http://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/001840Google Scholar
Behme, C. (2014). A ‘Galilean’ science of language. Journal of Linguistics 50: 671704.Google Scholar
Berent, I., Brem, A.-K., Zhao, X., Seligson, E., Pan, H., Epstein, J., Stern, E., Galaburda, A. M. and Pascual-Leone, A. (2015). Role of the motor system in language knowledge. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112: 1983–8.Google Scholar
Berk, S. (2003). Why why is different. In Beachley, B., Brown, A. and Conlin, F., eds, Boston University Conference on Language Development, vol. 27, pp. 127–37. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Berko, J. (1958). The child’s learning of English morphology. Word 14: 150–77.Google Scholar
Berko, J. and Brown, R. (1960). Word association and the acquisition of grammar. Child Development 31: 114.Google Scholar
van Berkum, J. J. A. (1996). The Psycholinguistics of Grammatical Gender. Nijmegen: Nijmegen University Press.Google Scholar
Bernstein, B. (2004). Class, Codes and Control, vol. 2: Applied Studies Towards a Sociology of Language. Abingdon, UK, and New York: Routledge. Originally published 1973.Google Scholar
Bever, T. G. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In Hays, J. R, ed., The Development of Language, pp. 279362. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bever, T. G. and McElree, B. (1988). Empty categories access their antecedents during comprehension. Linguistic Inquiry 19: 3443.Google Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R. (1990). The logical problem of foreign language learning. Linguistic Analysis 20: 349.Google Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R., Felix, S. and Ioup, G. (1988). The accessibility of Universal Grammar in adult language learning. Second Language Research 4: 132.Google Scholar
Bloomfield, L. (1933). Language. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Bobaljik, J. (2012). Universals of Comparative Morphology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bobaljik, J. (2015). Distributed morphology. Manuscript, University of Connecticut.Google Scholar
Bobaljik, J. and Wurmbrand, S. (2008). Case in GB/minimalism. In Malchukov, A. and Spencer, A., eds, Handbook of Case, pp. 4458. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bock, K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology 18: 355–87.Google Scholar
Boeckx, C. (2006). Linguistic Minimalism: Origins, Concepts, Methods and Aims. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Boeckx, C. (2008). Bare Syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Boeckx, C. (2009). Language in Cognition: Uncovering Mental Structures and the Rule Behind Them. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J., Okanoya, K. and Scharff, C. (2010). Twitter evolution: Converging mechanisms in birdsong and human speech. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11: 747–59.Google Scholar
Bölte, J. and Connine, C. M. (2004). Grammatical gender in spoken word recognition in German. Perception & Psychophysics 66: 1018–32.Google Scholar
Bonet, E. and Harbour, D. (2012). Contextual allomorphy. In Trommer, J., ed., The Morphology and Phonology of Exponence, pp. 195235. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bonneville-Roussy, A., Rentfrow, P. J., Xu, M. K. and Potter, J. (2013). Music through the ages: Trends in musical engagement and preferences from adolescence through middle adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 105: 703–17.Google Scholar
Borer, H. (1988). On the morphological parallelism between compounds and constructs. Yearbook of Morphology 1: 4566.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought?: Mandarin and English speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology 43: 122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, L., Schmidt, L. A. and Phillips, W. (2003). Sex, syntax, and semantics. In Gentner, D. and Goldin-Meadow, S., eds, Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Cognition, pp. 6179. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Borsche, T. (1990). Wilhelm Von Humboldt. Munich: C. H. Beck.Google Scholar
Bouton, L. F. (1970). Antecedent-contained pro-forms. In Campbell, M. A., Lindholm, J., Davison, A., Fisher, W., Furbee, L., Lovins, J., Maxwell, E., Reighard, J. and Straight, S., eds, Proceedings of Sixth Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, pp. 154–67. Chicago: University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1982). Reorganizational processes in lexical and syntactic development. In Wanner, E. and Gleitman, L., eds, Language Acquisition: The State of the Art, pp. 319–46. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1988). The ‘no negative evidence’ problem: How do children avoid constructing an overly general grammar? In Hawkins, J. A., ed., Explaining Language Universals, pp. 73104. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. and Choi, S. (2001). Shaping meanings for language: Universal and language-specific in the acquisition of spatial semantic categories. In Bowerman, M. and Levinson, S., eds, Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development, pp. 475511. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bransford, J. D. and Franks, J. J. (1971). The abstraction of linguistic ideas. Cognitive Psychology 2: 231350.Google Scholar
Bresnan, J. (1971). Sentence stress and syntactic transformations. Language 47 (2): 257–81.Google Scholar
Brooks, P. J., Tomasello, M., Dodson, K. and Lewis, L. B. (1999). Young children’s overgeneralizations with fixed transitivity verbs. Child Development 70: 1325–37.Google Scholar
Brown, C. (1997). Acquisition of segmental structure: Consequences for speech perception and second language acquisition. PhD thesis, Department of Linguistics, McGill University.Google Scholar
Brown, C. (2000). The interrelation between speech perception and phonological acquisition from infant to adult. In Archibald, J., ed., Second Language Acquisition and Linguistic Theory, pp. 463. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Brown, H. D. (2007). Principles of Language Learning and Teaching. New York: Longman.Google Scholar
Bruening, B. and Tran, T. (2006). Wh-questions in Vietnamese. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 15: 319–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryson, B. (2005). A Short History of Nearly Everything. New York: Broadway Books/Random House.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. (2010). Language, Usage and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. and Hopper, P. J., eds (2001a). Frequency and the Emergence of Linguistic Structure. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. and Hopper, P. J. (2001b). Introduction to frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure. In Bybee, J. L. and Hopper, P. J., eds, Frequency and the Emergence of Linguistic Structure, pp. 124. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. and Thompson, S. A. (1997). Three frequency effects in syntax. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of Berkeley Linguistics Society: General Session and Parasession on Pragmatics and Grammatical Structure, pp. 378–88. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Byrne, D. (2010). Song lyrics are overrated. CNN Online.Google Scholar
Cahill, L. and Gazdar, G. (1999). German noun inflection. Journal of Linguistics 35: 142.Google Scholar
Cahill, T. (1995). How the Irish Saved Civilization: The Untold Story of Ireland’s Heroic Role from the Fall of Rome to the Rise of Medieval Europe. New York and London: Anchor Books/Doubleday.Google Scholar
Cai, Z., Sturt, P. and Pickering, M. (2011/2012). The effect of non-adopted analyses on sentence processing. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 27: 1286–311.Google Scholar
Callahan, S. M., Shapiro, L. P. and Love, T. (2010). Parallelism effects and verb activation: The sustained reactivation hypothesis. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 39: 101–18.Google Scholar
Campbell, J. (2013). Thou Art That: Transforming Religious Metaphor. Novato, CA: New World Library.Google Scholar
Campbell, L. (1998). Historical Linguistics: An Introduction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, R. N. (2006). Language development: Pre-scientific studies. In Brown, K., ed., Encyclopedia of Languages and Linguistics, pp. 391–4. Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Cappelen, H. and Lepore, E. (2005). A tall tale: In defense of semantic minimalism and speech act pluralism. In Preyer, G. and Peter, G., eds, Contextualism in Philosophy: Knowledge, Meaning, and Truth, pp. 197220. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carnie, A. (2011). Syntax: A Generative Introduction, second edition. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Cedergren, H. J., Levac, L. and Perreault, H. (1992). Durational effects of prosodic structure in spontaneous spoken French. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 91 (4): 2387.Google Scholar
Cedergren, H. J., and Perreault, H. (1994). Speech rate and syllable timing in spontaneous speech. In Proceedings of ICSLP ’94, Yokohama, vol. 3, 1087–90. Tokyo: Acoustical Society of Japan.Google Scholar
Chien, Y.-C. and Wexler, K. (1990). Children’s knowledge of locality conditions in binding as evidence for the modularity of syntax and pragmatics. Language Acquisition 1: 225–95.Google Scholar
Chierchia, G. (1995). Dynamics of Meaning: Anaphora, Presupposition and the Theory of Grammar. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Choi, S. ([2009] 2015). Language specific spatial semantics and cognition: Developmental patterns in English and Korean. In Lee, C., Simpson, G. B. and Kim, Y., eds, The Handbook of East Asian Psycholinguistics, pp. 107–26. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of B. F. Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. Language 35: 2658.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1970). Remarks on nominalization. In Jacobs, R. and Rosenbaum, P., eds, Readings in Transformational Grammar, pp. 184221. Waltham, MA: Ginn and Co.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1973). Conditions on transformations. In Anderson, S. R. and Kiparsky, P., eds, A Festschrift for Morris Halle, pp. 232–85. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1977). Essays on Form and Interpretation. Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on Government and Binding: The Pisa Lectures, vol. 9. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1985). Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin, and Use. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1988). Language and Problems of Knowledge: The Managua Lectures. Current Studies in Linguistics, vol. 16. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1995). The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2001). Derivation by phrase. In Kenstowicz, M., ed., Ken Hale: A Life in Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2005). Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry 36: 122.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2008). On phases. In Freidin, R., Otero, C. and Zubizarreta, M. L., eds, Foundational Issues in Linguistic Theory: Essays in Honor of Jean-Roger Vergnaud, pp. 133–66. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2010). Poverty of Stimulus: Some Unfinished Business. Paris: CNRS Images.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N., Belletti, A. and Rizzi, L. (2002). On Nature and Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Chorost, M. (2014). Your brain on metaphors. Chronicle of Higher Education. http://chronicle.com/article/Your-Brain-on-Metaphors/148495/Google Scholar
Christiansen, M. H. and Chater, N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31: 489509.Google Scholar
Christodoulou, C. and Wexler, K. (2016). The morphosyntactic development of case in Down Syndrome. Lingua 184: 2552.Google Scholar
Chung, S. and McCloskey, J. (1987). Government, barriers and small clauses in Modern Irish. Linguistic Inquiry 18: 173237.Google Scholar
Cinque, G. (1999). Adverbs and Functional Heads: A Cross-Linguistic Perspective. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cinque, G. (2002). Mapping Functional Structure. In Cinque, G., ed., Functional Structure in the IP and DP, pp. 311. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cinque, G. (2005). Deriving Greenberg’s Universal 20 and its exceptions. Linguistic Inquiry 36: 315–32.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. and Featherston, S. (1999). Antecedent priming at trace positions: Evidence from German scrambling. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 28: 415–37.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. and Felser, C. (2006). Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics 27: 342.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. and Muysken, P. (1986). The availability of Universal Grammar to adult and child learners: A study of the acquisition of German word order. Second Language Research 2: 93119.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1993). The Lexicon in Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cocker, E. (1736). Cocker’s Arithmetick, Perused and Published by J. Hawkins, 48th edition. London: John Hawkins.Google Scholar
Cohen, D. (2008). Equations from God: Pure Mathematics and Victorian Faith. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Collins, C. (1991). Why and how come. In MIT Working Papers in Linguistics, pp. 3145. Cambridge, MA: MIT Working Papers in Linguistics.Google Scholar
Collins, J. (2011). The Unity of Linguistic Meaning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M. (2013). How can functional neuroimaging inform cognitive theories? Perspectives on Psychological Science 8: 98103.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R. and Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review 108: 204–56.Google Scholar
Comrie, B. (1981). Language Universals and Linguistic Typology, second edition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Conroy, A. and Lidz, J. (2007). Production/comprehension asymmetry in children’s why questions. In Belikova, A. et al., eds, Proceedings of the 2nd Conference on Language Acquisition North America (Galana), pp. 7383. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Conroy, A. and Thornton, R. (2005). Children’s knowledge of Principle C in discourse. In Otsu, Y., ed., Proceedings of the Sixth Tokyo Conference on Psycholinguistics, pp. 6994. Tokyo: Hituzi Syobu.Google Scholar
Conway Morris, S. (2003). Life’s Solution: Inevitable Humans in a Lonely Universe. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cook, V. (1991). The poverty-of-the-stimulus argument and multicompetence. Second Language Research 7: 103–17.Google Scholar
Cook, V. (1995). Multicompetence and effects of age. In Singleton, D. and Lengyl, Z., eds, The Age Factor in Second Language Acquisition. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Cooper, J. (1991). Drug-Related Problems in Geriatric Nursing Home Patients. New York: Pharmaceutical Products Press.Google Scholar
Cooper, M. and Chalfant, H. (1984). Subway Art. New York: Henry Holt and Company.Google Scholar
Corbett, G. G. (1991). Gender, Cambridge Textbooks in Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Corbett, G. and Fraser, N. (1993). Network morphology: A DATR account of Russian nominal inflection. Journal of Linguistics 29: 113–42.Google Scholar
Cornips, L. and Corrigan, K., eds (2005). Syntax and Variation: Reconciling the Biological and the Social. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Craik, K. J. W. (1967). The Nature of Explanation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (Original edition, 1943.)Google Scholar
Crain, S. (1991). Language acquisition in the absence of experience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14: 597612.Google Scholar
Crain, S. (1993). Language acquisition in the absence of experience. In Bloom, P., ed., Language Acquisition: Core Readings, pp. 364409. New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf.Google Scholar
Crain, S. and McKee, C. (1985). The acquisition of structural restrictions on anaphora. In Berman, S., Choe, J.-W. and McDonough, J., eds, Proceedings of NELS 15, pp. 94110. Amherst, MA: GLSA Publications.Google Scholar
Crain, S. and Nakayama, M. (1987). Structure dependence in grammar formation. Language 63: 522–43.Google Scholar
Crain, S. and Pietroski, P. (2001). Nature, nurture and Universal Grammar. Linguistics and Philosophy 24: 139–86.Google Scholar
Crain, S. and Thornton, R. (1998). Investigations in Universal Grammar: A Guide to Experiments on the Acquisition of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Crisma, P. (1990). Functional categories inside the noun phrase: A study on the distribution of nominal modifiers. Tesi di Laurea, University of Venice.Google Scholar
Croft, W. (2001). Radical Construction Grammar: Syntactic Theory in Typological Perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Culicover, P. (1993). Evidence against ECP accounts of the that-trace effect. Linguistic Inquiry 24: 557–61.Google Scholar
Culicover, P. (1998). The minimalist impulse. In Culicover, P. W. and McNally, L., eds, The Limits of Syntax, pp. 4477. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Culicover, P. (1999). Syntactic Nuts: Hard Cases, Syntactic Theory and Language Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Culicover, P. and Jackendoff, R. (2005). Simpler Syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cutler, A., ed. (2005). Twenty-First Century Psycholinguistics: Four Cornerstones. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cutler, A. (2015). Native Listening: Language Experience and the Recognition of Spoken Words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cutler, A., Dahan, D. and Van Donselaar, W. (1997). Prosody in the comprehension of spoken language: A literature review. Language and Speech 40: 141201.Google Scholar
Cutler, A. and Norris, D. (1988). The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 14: 113–21.Google Scholar
Cutler, A. and Otake, T. (1994). Mora or phoneme? Further evidence for language-specific listening. Journal of Memory and Language 33: 824844.Google Scholar
Dabrowska, E. (2001). Learning a morphological system without a default: the Polish genitive. Journal of Child Language 28: 545–74.Google Scholar
Dabrowska, E. and Street, J. (2006). Individual differences in language attainment: Comprehension of passive sentences by native and non-native English speakers. Language Sciences 28: 604–15.Google Scholar
Dahan, D., Swingley, D., Tanenhaus, M. and Magnuson, J. S. (2000). Linguistic gender and spoken-word recognition in French. Journal of Memory and Language 42: 465–80.Google Scholar
Darwin, E. (1794). Zoonomia, or the Laws of Organic Life, vol. II (third edition 1801). London: J. Johnson. Cited in Ridley (1999: 12).Google Scholar
de Carvalho, A., Lidz, J., Tieu, L., Bleam, T. and Christophe, A. (2016). English-speaking preschoolers can use phrasal prosody for syntactic parsing. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 139 (6): EL216–EL22.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. D. and Pyers, J. E. (2002). Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding. Cognitive Development 17: 1037–60.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. and Cohen, L. (1991). Two mental calculation systems: A case study of severe acalculia with preserved approximation. Neuropsychologia 29: 1045–54.Google Scholar
Deprez, V. and Pierce, A. (1993). Negation and functional projections in early grammar. Linguistic Inquiry 24: 2567.Google Scholar
Desai, R. H., Binder, J. R., Conant, L. L., Mano, Q. R. and Seidenberg, M. S. (2011). The neural career of sensory-motor metaphors. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23: 2376–86.Google Scholar
Devlin, K. (1998). The Language of Mathematics: Making the Invisible Visible. New York: Henry Holt and Company.Google Scholar
Dixon, J. A., Mahoney, B. and Cocks, R. (2002). Accents of guilt? Effects of regional accent, ‘race’ and crime type on attributions of guilt. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 21: 162−8.Google Scholar
Dobler, T. (2013). Ever the twain shall meet? Croatian Journal of Philosophy XIII (38): 293311.Google Scholar
Doherty, C. (1994). The syntax of subject contact relatives. In Beals, K., ed., Proceedings of the Twenty-Ninth Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, pp. 5565. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Doherty, C. (2013). Clauses without ‘That’: The Case for Bare Sentential Complementation in English. Abingdon, UK, and New York: Routledge. Originally published in 2000 by Garland Publishing.Google Scholar
Dromi, E. (1987). Early Lexical Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Duffield, N. (1993). On Case-checking and NPI licensing in Hiberno-English. Rivista di Linguistica 5: 215–44.Google Scholar
Duffield, N. (1996). On structural invariance and lexical diversity in VSO languages: Arguments from Irish noun-phrases. In Borsley, R. and Roberts, I., eds, The Syntax of the Celtic Languages, pp. 314–40. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Duffield, N. (2003). Measures of competent gradience. In van Hout, R., Hulk, A., Kuiken, F. and Towell, R., eds, The Lexicon–Syntax Interface in Second Language Acquisition, pp. 97127. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Duffield, N. (2004). Implications of competent gradience. Moderne Sprachen 45: 95117.Google Scholar
Duffield, N. (2010). Roll up for the mystery tour! Lingua 120: 2673–5.Google Scholar
Duffield, N. (2013). On polarity emphasis, assertion and mood in English and Vietnamese. Lingua 137: 248–70.Google Scholar
Duffield, N. (2014). Shake can well. Lingbuzz 002119. http://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/002119Google Scholar
Duffield, N. (2015). On what projects. Lingbuzz 002429. http://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/002429Google Scholar
Duffield, N. (2016). Seeing names, hearing faces: A cross-modal investigation of perceptual narrowing in second language learners. Paper presented at PacSLRF 2016, Chuo University, Tokyo.Google Scholar
Duffield, N. (2017a). On what projects in Vietnamese. Journal of East Asian Lingusitics (in press).Google Scholar
Duffield, N. (2017b). Which Other Race Effect? Cross-linguistic and cross-modal asymmetries in perceptual narrowing. Manuscript, Konan University.Google Scholar
Duffield, N., Matsuo, A. and Roberts, L. (2010). Factoring out the parallelism effect in VP-ellipsis: English vs. Dutch contrasts. Second Language Research 25 (4), 427–67.Google Scholar
Durham, M. (2011). Right dislocation in Northern England: Frequency and use – perception meets reality. English World-Wide 32 (3): 257–79.Google Scholar
Durrell, M. (2011). Hammer’s German Grammar and Usage, fifth edition. Abingdon, UK, and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Eco, U. (1993). La Ricerca Della Lingua Perfetta. Rome and Bari: Editori Laterza.Google Scholar
Edwards, H. T. and Kirkpatrick, A. G. (1999). Metalinguistic awareness in children: A developmental progression. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 28: 313–29.Google Scholar
Einstein, A. (1934). On the method of theoretical physics: The Herbert Spencer lecture. Philosophy of Science 1 (2): 163–9.Google Scholar
Eisenbeiss, S. (2000). The acquisition of the determiner phrase in German child language. In Friedemann, M.-A. and Rizzi, L., eds, The Acquisition of Syntax. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Eisenbeiss, S. (2002). Merkmalsgesteuerter Grammatikerwerb: Eine Untersuchung zum Erwerb der Struktur und Flexion von Nominalphrasen. PhD dissertation, Linguistics Department, Heinrich Heine-Universität, Düsseldorf.Google Scholar
Ellegård, A. (1953). The Auxiliary Do: The Establishment and Regulation of Its Use in English. Gothenburg Studies in English, vols 2, 3. Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell.Google Scholar
Ellis, A. W. and Young, A. W. (1988). Human Cognitive Neuropsychology. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Elman, J. L. (1993). Learning and development in neural networks: The importance of starting small. Cognition 48: 7199.Google Scholar
Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D. and Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Embick, D. and Marantz, A. (2005). Cognitive neuroscience and the English past tense: Comments on the paper by Ullman et al. Brain and Language 93: 243–7.Google Scholar
Embick, D. and Noyer, R. (2007). Distributed morphology and the syntax–morphology interface. In Ramchand, G. and Reiss, C., eds, Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Interfaces, pp. 289324. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Embick, D. and Poeppel, D. (2015). Towards a computational(ist) neurobiology of language: Correlational, integrated, and explanatory neurolinguistics. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 30: 357–66.Google Scholar
Eppler, E. D. and Ozón, G. (2013). English Words and Sentences: An Introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Epstein, S. D., Flynn, S. and Martohardjono, G. (1996). Second language acquisition: Theoretical and experimental issues in contemporary research. Brain and Behavioral Sciences 19: 677714.Google Scholar
Epstein, S. D. and Seely, T. D. (2006). Derivations in Minimalism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Erteschik-Shir, N. (1997). The Dynamics of Focus Structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Etcoff, N. (1999). Survival of the Prettiest: The Science of Beauty. London: Abacus Books.Google Scholar
Evans, G. (1982). The Varieties of Reference. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Evans, N. and Levinson, S. (2009). The myth of Language Universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32: 429–92.Google Scholar
Everett, D. L. (2005). Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã. Current Anthropology 46: 621–46.Google Scholar
Everett, D. L. (2009). Pirahã culture and grammar: A response to some criticisms. Language 85: 405–42.Google Scholar
Everett, D. L. (2013). Language: The Cultural Tool. London: Profile Books.Google Scholar
Eysenck, M. (1984). A Handbook of Cognitive Psychology. London and Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fabri, R. (1993). Kongruenz und die Grammatik des Maltesischen [Agreement and the Grammar of Maltese]. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer Verlag.Google Scholar
Fassi Fehri, A. (1999). Arabic modifying adjectives and DP structures. Studia Linguistica 53: 105–54.Google Scholar
Fay, D. and Cutler, A. (1977). Malapropisms and the structure of the mental lexicon. Linguistic Inquiry 8: 505–20.Google Scholar
Fearon, J. D. (2003). Ethnic and cultural diversity by country. Journal of Economic Growth 8: 195222.Google Scholar
Featherston, S. (2001). Empty Categories in Sentence Processing. Linguistik Aktuell/Linguistics Today, vol. 43. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Featherston, S. (2007). Data in generative grammar: The stick and the carrot. Theoretical Linguistics 33: 269318.Google Scholar
Ferreira, F., Ferraro, V. and Bailey, K. G. D. (2002). Good-enough representations in language comprehension. Current Directions in Psychological Science 11: 11–5.Google Scholar
Ferreira, F. and Patson, N. D. (2007). The ‘good enough’ approach to language comprehension. Language and Linguistics Compass 1: 7183.Google Scholar
Ferreira, V. S. (2006). How are speakers’ linguistic choices affected by ambiguity? In Meyer, A. S., Krott, A. and Wheeldon, L. R., eds, Automaticity and Control in Language Processing, pp. 6392. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Ferreira, V. S. and Bock, K. (2006). The functions of structural priming. Language and Cognitive Processes 21: 1011–29.Google Scholar
Ferreira, V. S. and Dell, G. S. (2000). The effect of ambiguity and lexical availability on syntactic and lexical production. Cognitive Psychology 40: 296340.Google Scholar
Ferreira, V. S., Slevc, L. R. and Rogers, E. S. (2005). How do speakers avoid ambiguous linguistic expressions? Cognition 96: 263–94.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. J., Kay, P. and O’Connor, M. C. (1988). Regularity and Idiomaticity in grammatical constructions: The case of let alone. Language 64: 501–38.Google Scholar
Fisher, S. E. and Scharff, C. (2009). FOXP2 as a molecular window into speech and language. Trends in Genetics 25: 166–77.Google Scholar
Fisher, S. E., Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K. E., Monaco, A. P. and Pembrey, M. E. (1998). Localisation of a gene implicated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nat Genet 18: 168–70.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., Takagi, N. and Mann, V. (1996). Lexical familiarity and English-language experience affects Japanese adults’ perception of /ɹ/ and /L/. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 99: 1161–73.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., Yeni-Komshian, G. H. and Liu, S. (1999). Age constraints on second language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language 41: 78104.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1975). The Language of Thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1981). The present status of the innateness controversy. In RePresentations: Philosophical Essays on the Foundations of Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1998). Concepts: Where Cognitive Science Went Wrong. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A., Bever, T. G. and Garrett, M. F. (1974). The Psychology of Language. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. and Garrett, M. F. (1967). Some syntactic determinants of sentential complexity. Perception & Psychophysics 2: 289–96.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. D. (1998). Learning to parse? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 27: 285319.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. D. (2002). Prosodic disambiguation in silent reading. In Hirotani, M., ed., Proceedings of NELS 32, vol. 1, pp. 113–32. Amherst, MA: GLSA Publications.Google Scholar
Foley, W. A. and Van Valin, R. D. (1984). Functional Syntax and Universal Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Frank, S. L., Bod, R. and Christiansen, M. H. (2012). How hierarchical is language use? Proceedings of the Royal Society B 279: 4522–31.Google Scholar
Fraser, N. and Corbett, G. (1997). Defaults in Arapesh. Lingua 103: 2557.Google Scholar
Frazier, L. (1990). Exploring the architecture of the language processing System. In Altmann, G. T. M., ed., Cognitive Models of Speech Processing, pp. 409–33. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Frazier, L. and Flores d’Arcais, G. (1989). Filler driven parsing: A study of gap filling in Dutch. Journal of Memory and Language 28: 331–44.Google Scholar
Frazier, L., Taft, L., Roeper, T., Clifton, C. and Ehrlich, K. (1984). Parallel structure: A source of facilitation in sentence comprehension. Memory and Cognition 12: 421–30.Google Scholar
Freeborn, D. (2005). From Old English to Standard English: A Course Book in Language Variations across Time. London: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Frege, G. ([1892] 1952). On sense and reference. In Geach, P. and Black, M., eds, Translations from the Philosophical Writings of Gottlob Frege. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6: 7884.Google Scholar
Fromkin, V. and Rodman, R. (1993). An Introduction to Language, fifth edition. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Fruchter, J., Stockall, L. and Marantz, A. (2013). MEG masked priming evidence for form-based decomposition of irregular verbs. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7: 798.Google Scholar
Fujita, K. (1996). Double objects, causatives and derivational economy. Linguistic Inquiry 27: 146–73.Google Scholar
Gabrielli, L. H., Cardenas, J., Poitras, C. B. and Lipson, M. (2009). Cloaking at optical frequencies. arXiv:0904.3508v1 [physics.optics].Google Scholar
Gardner, R. C. (2010). Motivation and Second Language Acquisition. New York: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Gass, S. M. and Selinker, L. (2001). Second Language Acquisition: An Introductory Course. Abingdon, UK, and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Gattuso, J. (2005). The Lotus Still Blooms: Sacred Buddhist Teachings for the Western Mind. New York: Jeremy P. Tarcher/Penguin USA.Google Scholar
Gauvenet, H., Hassan, M., Gross, H. and Mason, B. (1963). Bonjour Line: An Audio-Visual French Course for Children Starting at Primary Level. London: G.G. Harrap.Google Scholar
van Gelderen, E. (2000). The absence of verb-movement and the role of C: Some negative constructions in Shakespeare. Studia Linguistica 54: 412–23.Google Scholar
Gentner, D. and Stevens, A. L., eds (1983). Mental Models. New York and London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Geuder, W. and Weisgerber, M. (2006). Manner and causation in movement verbs. In Ebert, C. and Endriss, C., eds, Proceedings of Sinn und Bedeutung 10, pp. 125–38. ZAS Papers in Linguistics. Berlin: Zentrum für Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft, Sprachtypologie und Universalienforschung.Google Scholar
Gibson, E., Piantadosi, S. T. and Fedorenko, E. (2013). Quantitative methods in syntax/semantics research: A response to Sprouse and Almeida (2013). Language and Cognitive Processes 28: 229–40.Google Scholar
Gladwell, M. (2013). David and Goliath: Underdogs, Misfits and the Art of Battling Giants. New York: Little, Brown and Company.Google Scholar
Gleitman, H. and Gleitman, L. R. (1979). Language use and language judgment. In Fillmore, C. J., Kempler, D. and Wang, W. S., eds, Individual Differences in Language Ability and Language Behavior, pp. 103–26. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Goddard, C. and Wierzbicka, A. (2002). Semantic primes and Universal Grammar. In Goddard, C and Wierzbicka, A, eds, Meaning and Universal Grammar: Theory and Empirical Findings, pp. 4185. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Goethe, J. W. von ([1821] 1907). Maximen und Reflexionen, Aphorismen und Aufzeichnungen: Nach den Handschriften des Goethe-und-Schiller-Archivs, vol. 3: Aus Kunst und Altertum. Edited by Hecker, M. Weimar: Verlag der Goethe- Gesellschaft.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Construction Grammar: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (2006). Constructions at Work: The Nature of Generalization in Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gombert, J. E. (1992). Metalinguistic Development. Translated by T. Pownall. New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf.Google Scholar
Goodluck, H. and Zweig, E. (2013). Introduction: Formal vs. processing explanations of linguistic phenomena. Language and Cognitive Processes 28: 18.Google Scholar
Gopnik, M. (1990). Feature-blind grammar and dysphasia. Nature 344: 715.Google Scholar
Gopnik, M. and Crago, M. B. (1991). Familial aggregation of a developmental language disorder. Cognition 39: 150.Google Scholar
Goto, H. (1971). Auditory perception by normal Japanese adults of the sounds ‘L’ and ‘R’. Neuropsychologia 9: 317–23.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1987). An Urchin in the Storm. London: W. W. Norton and Company.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (2002). The Structure of Evolutionary Theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. and Eldredge, N. (1977). Punctuated equilibria: The tempo and mode of evolution reconsidered. Paleobiology 3: 115–51.Google Scholar
Greenberg, J. H. (1963). Some universals of grammar with particular reference to the order of meaningful elements. In Greenberg, J. H, ed., Universals of Language, pp. 5890. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Greenleaf, A., Lassas, M. and Uhlmann, G. (2003). Anisotropic conductivities that cannot be detected by EIT. Physiol. Meas. 24: 413–19.Google Scholar
Gregg, K. R. (1984). Krashen’s monitor and Occam’s razor. Applied Linguistics 5: 79100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P. ([1975] 1989). Logic and conversation. In Cole, P. and Morgan, J. L, eds, Syntax and Semantics III: Speech Acts, pp. 4158. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Guasti, M.-T. (2004). Language Acquisition: The Growth of Grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gussenhoven, C. (1984). On the Grammar and Semantics of Sentence Accents. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Hackl, M., Koster-Hale, J. and Varvoutis, J. (2012). Quantification and ACD: Evidence from real time sentence processing. Journal of Semantics 29: 145206.Google Scholar
Haden, E. F. (1955). The uvular r in French. Language 31: 504–10.Google Scholar
Haeberli, E. and Ihsane, T. (2016). Revisiting the loss of verb movement in the history of English. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 34: 497542.Google Scholar
Haiman, J. and Thompson, S. A., eds (1988). Clause Combining in Grammar and Discourse. Typological Studies in Language, vol. 18. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1970). A Course in Spoken English: Intonation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (2005). On Grammar. London: Bloomsbury Academic.Google Scholar
Hambrich, D. Z., Macnamara, B.N., Campitelli, G., Ullén, F., and Mosing, M. A. (2016). Beyond born vs. made: a new look at expertise. In B. H. Hall, ed., Psychology of Learning and Motivation, vol. 64, pp. 1–55. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Han, C.-H. (2000). The evolution of do-support in English imperatives. In Pintzuk, S., Tsoulas, G and Warner, A. R., eds, Diachronic Syntax: Models and Mechanisms, pp. 275–95. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Han, C.-H. and Kroch, A. S. (2000). The rise of do-support in English: Implications for clause-structure. In Hirotani, M., Coetzee, A., Hall, N. and Kim, J.-Y., eds, Proceedings of NELS 30, pp. 311–26. Amherst, MA: GLSA Publications.Google Scholar
Hankamer, J. (1971). Deletion in coordinate structures. PhD dissertation, Yale University.Google Scholar
Hankamer, J. (1979). Deletion in Coordinate Structures. New York: Garland.Google Scholar
Hankamer, J. and Sag, I. (1976). Deep and surface anaphora. Linguistic Inquiry 7: 391428.Google Scholar
Hansell, M. (2007). Built by Animals: The Natural History of Animal Architecture. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Harbour, D. (2015). Untitled (segment). More than Words: Morphology and the Universality of language. Symposium at NYU Abu Dhabi Institute. www.youtube.com/watch?v=39Gpbm9qgzsGoogle Scholar
Hardt, D. (1993). Verb phrase ellipsis: Form, meaning and processing. PhD dissertation, Computer and Information Science, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Harley, H. (1995). Subjects, events, and licensing. PhD dissertation, Department of Linguistics and Philosophy, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Harmon, D. and Loh, J. (2010). The index of linguistic diversity: A new quantitative measure of trends in the status of the world’s languages. Language Documentation & Conservation 4: 97151.Google Scholar
Harris, M. and Coltheart, M. (1986). Language Processing in Adults and Children. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Hartshorne, J. K. and Ullman, M. T. (2006). Why do girls say ‘holded’ more than boys? Developmental Science 9: 2132.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, M. (2000). Why can’t we talk to each other? Lingua 110: 235–55.Google Scholar
Hawkins, J. A. (1983). Word Order Universals. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hawkins, J. A. (1995). A Performance Theory of Order and Constituency. Cambridge Studies in Linguistics, vol. 73. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, J. A. (2001). Why are categories adjacent? Journal of Linguistics 37: 134.Google Scholar
Hawkins, P. R. (2004). Hesitation phenomena and pausing. In Bernstein, B., ed., Class, Codes and Control, vol. 2: Applied Studies Towards a Sociology of Language. Taylor & Francis (originally published 1973).Google Scholar
Hayes, B. (1982). Extrametricality and English stress. Linguistic Inquiry 13: 227–76.Google Scholar
Hegarty, M. (2005). A Feature-Based Syntax of Functional Categories. Berlin: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. and Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33: 61135.Google Scholar
Henry, A. (1995). Dialect Variation and Parameter-Setting: A Study of Belfast English and Standard English. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Heycock, C. (2013). The syntax of predication. In M. den Dikken, ed., The Cambridge Handbook of Generative Syntax, pp. 322–52. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hill, T. (2015). Geometry and Faith: A Supplement to the Ninth Bridgewater Treatise – Scholar’s Choice Edition. BiblioLife.Google Scholar
Hiramatsu, K. and Lillo-Martin, D. (1998). Children who judge ungrammatical what they produce. Paper presented at Boston University Conference on Child Language Development.Google Scholar
Hoffman, A. (2016). Dogs know when you’re praising them: That doesn’t mean they understand human speech. The Smithsonian, 2 September 2016. www .smithsonianmag.com/science-nature/scientists-say-dogs-understand-human- speech-how-can-we-be-sure-180960336/Google Scholar
Hofmeister, P., Casasanto, L. S. and Sag, I. A. (2013a). Islands in the grammar? Standards of evidence. In Sprouse, J. and Hornstein, N., eds, Experimental Syntax and the Islands Debate, pp. 4263. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hofmeister, P., Jaeger, T. F., Arnon, I., Sag, I. A. and Snider, N. (2013b). The source ambiguity problem: Distinguishing the effects of grammar and processing on acceptability judgments. Language and Cognitive Processes 28: 4887.Google Scholar
Hogenboom, M. (2014). Spectacular real virgin births. BBC Online. www.bbc.com/earth/story/20141219-spectacular-real-virgin-birthsGoogle Scholar
Hogg, R. and Denison, D., eds (2006). A History of the English Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hopper, P. J. (1987). Emergent grammar. In Proceedings of the 13th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Hornstein, N. (2012). Why this blog? Faculty of Language. http://facultyoflanguage.blogspot.com/2012_09_01_archive.htmlGoogle Scholar
Hovdhaugen, E. (1990). Una et eadem: Some observations on Roger Bacon’s Greek grammar. In Bursill-Hall, G. L., Ebbesen, S. and Koerner, E. F. K., eds, De Ortu Grammaticae: Studies in Medieval Grammar and Linguistic Theory in Memory of Jan Pinborg. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Huang, C.-T. J. (1984). On the distribution and reference of empty pronouns. Linguistic Inquiry 15: 531–44.Google Scholar
Huddleston, R., Pullum, G. et al. (2002). The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hurford, J. R., Heasley, B. and Smith, M. B. (2007). Semantics: A Coursebook, second edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ibrahim, M. H. (1973). Grammatical Gender: Its Origin and Development. The Hague and Paris: Mouton.Google Scholar
Imai, M., Kita, S., Nagumo, M. and Okada, H. (2008). Sound symbolism facilitates early verb learning. Cognition 109: 5465.Google Scholar
Inagaki, S. (2001). Motion verbs with goal PPs in the L2 acquisition of English and Japanese. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 23: 153–70.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (1992). Babe Ruth homered his way into the hearts of America. In Stowell, T. and Wehrli, E, eds, Syntax and the Lexicon, pp. 155–78. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Jacobson, P. and Gibson, E. (2014). Processing of ACD gives no evidence of QR. In Proceedings of SALT 24, pp. 156–76.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. ([1959] 2000). On linguistic aspects of translation. In Venuti, L., ed., The Translation Studies Reader, pp. 138–43. London and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
James, A. (2000). States and sovereignty. In Salmon, T., ed., Issues in International Relations, pp. 124. Abingdon, UK, and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Jawaid, B. and Ahmed, T. (2009). Hindi to Urdu conversion: Beyond simple transliteration. In Proceedings of the Conference on Language & Technology 2009. Lahore, Pakistan: National University of Computer and Emerging Sciences.Google Scholar
Jespersen, O. (1949). A Modern English Grammar on Historical Principles: I-VII. Heidelberg: Carl Winter.Google Scholar
Johnson, J. S. and Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology 21: 6099.Google Scholar
Johnson, K. (1991). Object positions. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 9: 577636.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental Models: Towards a Cognitive Science of Language, Inference and Consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. and Stevenson, R. (1970). Memory for syntax. Nature 227 (5256): 412.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. and Slobin, D. I. (1979). The development of locative expressions in English, Italian, Serbo-Croatian, and Turkish. Journal of Child Language 16: 531–47.Google Scholar
Jones, S. (1993). The Language of the Genes: Biology, History and the Evolutionary Future. London: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Joos, M. (1957). Readings in Linguistics: The Development of Descriptive Linguistics in America since 1925. Washington, DC: American Council of Learned Societies.Google Scholar
Juffs, A. and Harrington, M. (1995). Parsing effects in second language sentence processing: Subject and object asymmetries in wh-extraction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 17: 483516.Google Scholar
Juffs, A. and Harrington, M. (1996). Garden path sentences and error data in second language sentence processing. Language Learning 46: 283326.Google Scholar
Juffs, A. and Rodríguez, G. A. (2015). Second Language Sentence Processing. New York and London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Jung, C. G. ([1954] 1981). Collected Works of C. G. Jung, vol. 17: Development of Personality. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Kabuzono, H. (2006). Phonetic and phonological organization of speech. In Nakayama, M., Mazuka, R. and Shirai, Y., eds, The Handbook of East Asian Psycholinguistics, pp. 191200. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kacinik, N. A. (2014). Sticking your neck out and burying the hatchet: What idioms reveal about embodied simulation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8: 689.Google Scholar
Kay, J., Lesser, R. and Coltheart, M. (1992). Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia (Palpa). Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kayne, R. (1994). The Antisymmetry of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kayne, R. and Pollock, J.-Y. (2008). Toward an analysis of French hyper-complex inversion. In Brugè, L., Cardinaletti, A., Giusti, G., Munaro, N. and Poletto, C., eds, Functional Heads. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Keenan, E. (2003). An historical explanation of some binding theoretic facts in English. In Moore, J. and Polinsky, M., eds, The Nature of Explanation in Linguistic Theory, pp. 152–89. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Kellerman, E. and van Hoof, A. (2003). Manual accents. International Review of Applied Linguistics 41: 251–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerswill, P. (1996). Children, adolescents and language change. Language Variation and Change 8: 177202.Google Scholar
Kiefer, F. (1980). Yes-no questions as wh-questions. In Searle, J. R., Kiefer, F. and Bierwisch, M., eds, Speech Act Theory and Pragmatics, pp. 97120. Dordrecht, Boston and London: D. Reidel.Google Scholar
Kilbury, J. (2001). German noun inflection revisited. Journal of Linguistics 37: 339–53.Google Scholar
Kimberley, A. (2015). On the supposed incompatability of truth-conditional semantics and semantic underdetermination. Conference presentation. https://eastanglia.academia.edu/AdamKimberleyGoogle Scholar
Kiparsky, P. (2013). Towards a null theory of the passive. Lingua 125: 733.Google Scholar
Kita, S. and Özyürek, A. (2003). What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal?: Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language 48: 1632.Google Scholar
Klein, W. and Perdue, C. (1997). The basic variety (Or: Couldn’t natural language be much simpler?). Second Language Research 14: 301–47.Google Scholar
Klima, E. (1964). Negation in English. In Fodor, J. and Katz, J., eds, The Structure of Language. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Kluender, R. and Kutas, M. (1993). Subjacency as a processing phenomenon. Language and Cognitive Processes 8: 573640.Google Scholar
Knoeferle, P. and Crocker, M. W. (2009). Constituent order and semantic parallelism in online comprehension: Eye-tracking evidence from German The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 62: 2338–71.Google Scholar
Krashen, S. (1977). Some issues relating to the monitor model. In Brown, H. D., Yorio, C. A. and Crymes, R. H., eds, On TESOL ’77: Teaching and Learning English as a Second Language: Trends in Research and Practice. Washington, DC: TESOL.Google Scholar
Krauss, M. (1992). The world’s languages in crisis. Language 68: 410.Google Scholar
Krifka, M., Pelletier, F. J., Carlson, G. N., ter Meulen, A., Chierchia, G. and Link, G. (1995). Genericity: An introduction. In Carlson, G. N. and Pelletier, F. J., eds, The Generic Book, pp. 1124. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kripke, S. (1980). Naming and Necessity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kroch, A. S. (1989). Reflexes of grammar in patterns of language Use. Journal of Language Variation and Change 1: 199244.Google Scholar
Kroch, A. S. (2003). Syntactic Change. In Baltin, M. and Collins, C., eds, Handbook of Contemporary Syntactic Theory. Malden, MA, and Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. (2004). Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5: 831–43.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F.-M. and Liu, H.-M. (2003). Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100: 9096–101.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K. N. and Lindblom, B. (1992). Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science 255: 606–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, W. (1981). Resolving the Neogrammarian controversy. Language 57: 267308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, W. (1994). Principles of Linguistic Change, three volumes. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Labov, W. and Labov, T. (1978). Learning the syntax of questions. In Campbell, R. and Smith, P., eds, Recent Advances in the Psychology of Language III, pp. 144. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Ladd, D. R. ([1996] 2008). Intonational Phonology, second edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Laenzlinger, C. (2005). French adjective ordering: Perspectives on DP-internal movement types. Lingua 115: 645–89.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. ([1965] 1970). On the nature of syntactic irregularity. Dissertation, Indiana University. Published as Irregularity in Syntax (1970), New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1986). Frame semantic control of the co-ordinate structure constraint. In Farley, A. M., Farley, P. T. and McCullough, K.-E., eds, CLS 22: Papers from the Parasession on Pragmatics and Grammatical Theory, pp. 152–67. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, Fire and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal About the Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. (2003). Metaphors We Live By. London: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Land, F. (1974). The Language of Mathematics. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. (1991). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D. (2016). Missing the trees for the forest: Morphology in second language acquisition. Second Language 15: 528.Google Scholar
Larson, B. (2015). Right node raising and non-grammaticality. Lingbuzz 002421. http://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/002421Google Scholar
Larson, R. K. (1988). On the double object construction. Linguistic Inquiry 19: 335–91.Google Scholar
Leopold, W. (1949). Speech Development of a Bilingual Child, vol. 4. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Lev-Ari, S. and Keysar, B. (2010). Why don’t we believe non-native speakers? The influence of accent on credibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46: 1093–6.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levin, B. and Rappaport Hovav, M. (2007). Reflections on the complementarity of manner and result (handout of a talk on 21 November 2007, Berlin). Manuscript, Stanford University. http://web.stanford.edu/~bclevin/pubs.htmlGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C. and Evans, N. (2010). Time for a sea-change in linguistics. Lingua 120: 2733–58.Google Scholar
Lewis, B. A., Shriberg, L. D., Freebairn, L. A., Hansen, A. J., Stein, C. M., Taylor, H. J. and Iyengar, S. K. (2006). The genetic bases of speech sound disorders: Evidence from spoken and written language. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 49: 1249–312.Google Scholar
Lewis, C. S. ([1960] 2013). Studies in Words, third edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. (2015). Review of The Science of Language: Interviews with James McGilvray (Chomsky). Modern Language Review 110: 222–4.Google Scholar
Lieven, E., Tomasello, M., Behrens, H. and Speares, J. (2003). Early syntactic creativity: A usage-based approach. Journal of Child Language 30: 333370.Google Scholar
Lightfoot, D. (1991). How to Set Parameters: Arguments from Language Change. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lightfoot, D. (1992). Why UG needs a learning theory. In Jones, C., eds, Historical Linguistics, pp. 191213. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Long, M. H. (1996). The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition. In Ritchie, W. C. and Bhatia, T. K., eds, Handbook of Second Language Acquisition, pp. 413–68. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Loschky, L. (2008). Comprehensible input and second language acquisition: What is the relationship? Studies in Second Language Acquisition 16: 303–23.Google Scholar
Losonsky, M., ed. (1999). Humboldt: On Language. Cambridge Texts in the History of Philosophy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lyons, J. ([1965] 1996). On competence and performance and related notions. In Brown, G., Malmkjaer, K. and Williams, J., eds, Performance and Competence in Second Language Acquisition, pp. 932. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
MacDonald, M.-E. C., Pearlmutter, N. J. and Seidenberg, M. A. (1994). Syntactic ambiguity resolution as lexical ambiguity resolution. In Clifton, C. Jr, Frazier, L. and Rayner, K., eds, Perspectives on Sentence Processing, pp. 123–54. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). Connectionism and language learning. In Barlow, M. and Kemmer, S., eds, Usage-Based Models of Language, pp. 121–50. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. and Bates, E. (1989). The Cross-Linguistic Study of Sentence Processing. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. and Chang, F. (1995). Connectionism and language learning. In Nelson, C. A., ed., Basic and Applied Perspectives on Learning, Acquisition and Development. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. and Leinbach, J. (1991). Implementations are not conceptualizations: Revising the verb learning model. Cognition 40: 121–57.Google Scholar
Mameli, M. and Bateson, P. (2006). Innateness and the sciences. Biology and Philosophy 21: 155–88.Google Scholar
Marcus, G. F. (2014). The trouble with brain science. New York Times. www.nytimes.com/2014/07/12/opinion/the-trouble-with-brain-science.htmlGoogle Scholar
Marcus, G. F., Brinkmann, U., Clahsen, H., Wiese, R. and Pinker, S. (1995). German inflection: The exception that proves the rule. Cognitive Psychology 29: 189256.Google Scholar
Marcus, G. F., Pinker, S., Ullman, M., Hollander, M., Rosen, T. J. and Xu, F. (1992). Over-regularization in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 57.Google Scholar
Marcus, R. B. (1961). Modalities and intensional languages. Synthese 13: 303–22.Google Scholar
Margolis, E. and Laurence, S. (2011). Learning matters: The role of learning in concept acquisition. Mind & Language 26: 507639.Google Scholar
Marr, D., Ullman, S. F. and Poggio, T. A., eds ([1982] 2010). Vision. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W., Tyler, L. K. and Seidenberg, M. (1978). Sentence processing and the clause-boundary. In Levelt, W. J. M. and Flores d’Arcais, G., eds, Studies in the Perception of Language, pp. 219–46. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W., Tyler, L. K., Waksler, R. and Older, L. (1994). Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon. Psychological Review 101: 333.Google Scholar
Martens, M. A., Wilson, S. J. and Reutens, D. C. (2008). Research review: Williams Syndrome: A critical review of the cognitive, behavioral, and neuroanatomical phenotype. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 49: 576608.Google Scholar
Martin, A., Peperkamp, S. and Dupoux, E. (2014). Learning phonemes with a proto-lexicon. Cognitive Science 37: 103–24.Google Scholar
Matsuo, A. (1998). A comparative study of tense and ellipsis. PhD dissertation, Department of Linguistics, University of Connecticut.Google Scholar
Max Planck Society (2016). Language is in the genes. https://www.mpg.de/10751617/simon-fisher-language-researchGoogle Scholar
May, R. (1977). The grammar of quantification. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
May, R. (1985). Logical Form: Its Structure and Derivation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
McCloskey, J. (1997). A global silencing. The Poetry Ireland Review 52: 41–6.Google Scholar
McCloskey, J. (2001). The morphosyntax of WH-extraction in Irish. Journal of Linguistics 37: 67100.Google Scholar
McDaniel, D., McKee, C. and Cairns, H. S. (1996). Methods for Assessing Children’s Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
McGilvray, J. ([1999] 2014). Chomsky: Language, Mind and Politics. Malden, MA, and Cambridge: Polity Press.Google Scholar
McLaurin, M. A. (2009). Marines of Montford Point: America’s First Black Marines. Charlotte, NC: University of North Carolina Press.Google Scholar
McLuhan, M. (1964). Understanding Media: The Extensions of Man. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Menand, L. (2004). Bad comma. New Yorker. www.newyorker.com/magazine/2004/06/28/bad-commaGoogle Scholar
Mendívil Giró, J. L. (2012). The myth of language diversity. In Boeckx, C., Horno Chéliz, M. and Mendívil Giró, J. L, eds, Language, from a Biological Point of View: Current Issues in Biolinguistics, pp. 85–134. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Merchant, J. (2001). The Syntax of Silence: Sluicing, Islands, and the Theory of Ellipsis. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Merchant, J. (2005). Fragments and ellipsis. Linguistics and Philosophy 27: 661738.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. and Chomsky, N. (1963). Finitary models of language users. In Luce, R. D., Bush, R. R. and Galanter, E., eds, Handbook of Mathematical Psychology, pp. 419–91. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. and McKean, K. O. (1964). A chronometric study of some relations between sentences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 16: 297308.Google Scholar
Millward, C. M. (1989). A Biography of the English Language. Fort Worth, TX: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Milroy, J. (1998). Children can’t speak or write properly anymore. In Trudgill, P. and Bauer, L., eds, Language Myths, pp. 5865. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Milsark, G. L. (1977). Toward an explanation of certain peculiarities of the existential construction in English. Linguistic Analysis 3: 129.Google Scholar
Miyawaki, K., Strange, W., Verbugge, R., Liberman, A., Jenkins, J. and Fujimura, O. (1975). An effect of linguistic experience: The discrimination of [r] and [l] by native speakers of Japanese and English. Perception & Psychophysics 18: 331–40.Google Scholar
Monsell, S. (1987). On the relation between lexical input and output pathways for speech. In Allport, A., MacKay, D., Prinz, W. and Scheerer, E., eds, Language Perception and Production, pp. 271311. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Moore, T. and Carling, C. (1982). Understanding language: Towards a post-Chomskyan linguistics. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Morgan, J. (1973). Sentence fragments and the notion ‘sentence’. In Kachru, B. B., Lees, R. B., Malkiel, Y., Pietrangeli, A. and Saporta, S., eds, Issues in Linguistics: Papers in Honor of Henry and Renée Kahane, pp. 719–52. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Morgan-Short, K. and Tanner, D. (2014). Event-related potentials. In Jegerski, J. and van Patten, B., eds, Research Methods in Second Language Psycholinguistics, pp. 127–52. New York and Abingdon, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Morris, J. (2012). Baby Talk. Harlequin (ebook).doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.006.Google Scholar
Müller, S. (2015). Deriving island constraints with Searle and Grice. Studia Linguistics 69 (1): 157.Google Scholar
Munafò, M. R., Yalcin, B., Willis-Owen, S. A. and Flint, J. (2007). Association of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene and approach-related personality traits: Meta-analysis and new data. Biological Psychiatry 63: 197206.Google Scholar
Neusner, J., ed. (1995). Judaism in Late Antiquity, part 1: The Literary and Archaeological Sources. Leiden: E. J. Brill.doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.006.Google Scholar
Nevins, A., Pesetsky, D. and Rodriguez, C. (2009). Piraha exceptionality: A reassessment. Language 85: 355404.Google Scholar
Newman, A. J. (2014). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (FMRI). In Jegerski, J. and van Patten, B, eds, Research Methods in Second Language Psycholinguistics, pp. 153–84. New York and Abingdon, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Newman, S. (1946). On the stress system of English. Word 2: 171–87.Google Scholar
Newmeyer, F. (1983). Grammatical Theory: Its Limits and Possibilities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Newmeyer, F. (1999). Language Form and Language Function. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Nichol, J. and Swinney, D. (1989). The role of structure in coreference assignment during sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 18: 520.Google Scholar
Ogura, M. (1993). The development of periphrastic do in English: A case of lexical diffusion in syntax. Diachronica 10: 5185.Google Scholar
O’Grady, W. (2003). The radical middle: Nativism without Universal Grammar. In Doughty, C. J. and Long, M. H., eds, The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition, pp. 4362. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Otake, T., Hatano, G., Cutler, A. and Mehler, J. (1993). Mora or syllable? Speech segmentation in Japanese. Journal of Memory and Language 32: 258–78.Google Scholar
Oxford, University of (2016). Charles F. Hockett. www.revolvy.com/main/index .php?s=Charles F. Hockett&uid=1575Google Scholar
Palmer, D. C. (2006). On Chomsky’s appraisal of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. The Behavior Analyst 29: 253–67.Google Scholar
Paradis, M. (2009). Declarative and Procedural Determinants of Second Languages. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Parker, D. and Phillips, C. (2016). Negative polarity illusions and the format of hierarchical encodings in memory. Cognition 157: 321–39.Google Scholar
Partee, B. (1991). Topic, focus and quantification. In Moore, S and Wyner, A. Z, eds, Proceedings of SALT 1, pp. 159–87. Linguistic Society of America.Google Scholar
Patsiurko, N., Campbell, J. L. and Hall, J. A. (2012). Measuring cultural diversity: Ethnic, linguistic and religious fractionalization in the OECD. Ethnic and Racial Studies 35: 195217.Google Scholar
Patterson, K. and Shewell, C. (1987). Speak and spell: Dissociations and word-class effects. In Coltheart, M., Sartori, G. and Job, R., eds, The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Language, pp. 273–94. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Paul, H. (1880). Prinzipien der Sprachgeschichte (second edition 1886; third edition 1898). Halle: Max Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Payne, T. E. (1997). Describing Morphosyntax: A Guide for Field Linguists. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Peacocke, C. (1986). Explanation in computational psychology: Language, perception and Level 1.5. Mind & Language 1: 101–23.Google Scholar
Pepper, J. (1981). John Pepper’s Ulster–English Dictionary, 1995 edition. Belfast: Appletree Press.Google Scholar
Perez, F. (2016). Nearly 20 years in the making, dictionary awakens Mutsun language. BenitoLink. http://goo.gl/3yJgyNGoogle Scholar
Perovic, A. (2006). Syntactic deficit in Down Syndrome: More evidence for the modular organisation of language. Lingua 116: 1616–30.Google Scholar
Pesetsky, D. M. (1995). Zero Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pesetsky, D. M. (2015). Complementizer-trace effects (encyclopaedia article). Lingbuzz 002385. http://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/002385Google Scholar
Pesetsky, D. M. and Torrego, E. (2001). T-to-C movement: Causes and consequences. In Kenstowicz, M., ed., Ken Hale: A Life in Language, pp. 355426. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Philip, W. and de Villiers, J. G. (1992). Monotonicity and the acquisition of weak wh islands. In Clark, E, ed., Proceedings of the 24th Annual Child Language Research Forum. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Phillips, C. (1996). Order and structure. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Phillips, C. (2013). Some arguments and non-arguments for reductionist accounts of syntactic phenomena. Language and Cognitive Processes 28: 156–87.Google Scholar
Phillips, C. and Lewis, S. (2013). Derivational order in syntax: Evidence and architectural consequences. Studies in Linguistics 6: 1147.Google Scholar
Phillips, C., Wagers, M. and Lau, E. (2011). Grammatical illusions and selective fallibility in real-time language comprehension. In Runner, J., ed., Experiments at the Interfaces, pp. 153–86. Syntax and Semantics, vol. 37. Bingley, UK: Emerald.Google Scholar
Pickering, M. and Barry, G. (1991). Sentence processing without empty categories. Language and Cognitive Processes 6 (3): 229–63.Google Scholar
Pienemann, M. (1998). Language Processing and Second Language Development: Processability Theory. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Piller, I. (2002). Passing for native: Identity and success in second language learning. Journal of Sociolinguistics 6: 179206.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1998). Words and rules. Lingua 106: 219–42.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2007). The Stuff of Thought: Language as a Window into Human Nature. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. and Prince, A. (1988). On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition 28: 73193.Google Scholar
Poeppel, D. (2014). The temporal structure of perceptual experience. Presentation at Genetics and Neurobiology of Language Conference, Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory.www.youtube.com/watch?v=lIqO4wz3VCsGoogle Scholar
Postal, P. M. (1974). On Raising. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Potter, M. C. and Lombardi, L. (1990). Regeneration in short-term recall of sentences. Journal of Memory and Language 29: 633–54.Google Scholar
Pullum, G. (2010). Creation myths of generative grammar and the mathematics of Syntactic Structures. In Ebert, C., Jäger, G. and Michaelis, J., eds, The Mathematics of Language, pp. 238–54. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Pullum, G. and Scholz, B. (2002). Empirical assessment of stimulus poverty arguments. The Linguistic Review 19: 950.Google Scholar
Quine, W. v. O. ([1960] 2013). Word and Object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G. and Svartvik, J. (1985). A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Radick, G. (2016). The unmaking of a modern synthesis: Noam Chomsky, Charles Hockett and the politics of behaviorism, 1955–1965. Isis 107: 4973.Google Scholar
Rampton, B. (1995). Crossing: Language and Ethnicity among Adolescents. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Rampton, B. (1999). Styling the other: Introduction. Journal of Sociolinguistics 3: 421–7.Google Scholar
Read, C. 2016. The Lost Boys. London: The Save the Children Fund.Google Scholar
Recanati, F. (2004). What is said and the semantics/pragmatics distinction. In Bianchi, C., ed., The Semantics/Pragmatics Distinction, pp. 4564. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Reimer, M. and Michaelson, E. (2016). Reference. The Stanford Encylopedia of Philosophy. http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2016/entries/reference/Google Scholar
Rice, M. L., Smolik, F., Perpich, D., Thompson, T., Rytting, N. and Blossom, M. (2010). Mean length of utterance levels in 6-month intervals for children 3 to 9 years with and without language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 53: 333–49.Google Scholar
Richardson, H. (2016). Grammar schools: What are they and why are they controversial? BBC Online. www.bbc.com/news/education-34538222Google Scholar
Riddoch, M. J. and Humphreys, G. W. (1987). A case of integrative visual agnosia. Brain 110: 1431-62.Google Scholar
Ridley, M. (1999). Genome: The Autobiography of a Species in 23 Chapters. London: Fourth Estate.Google Scholar
Riney, T. J., Takagi, N., Ota, K. and Uchida, Y. (2007). The intermediate degree of VOT in Japanese initial voiceless stops. Journal of Phonetics 35: 439–43.Google Scholar
Ritter, E. (1987). NSO noun-phrases in a VSO language. In McDonough, J. and Plunkett, B., eds, Proceedings of NELS 17. Amherst, MA: GLSA Publications.Google Scholar
Ritter, E. (1988). A head-movement approach to construct-state noun phrases. Linguistics 26: 909–29.Google Scholar
Rivenc, P. (2003). Apprentissage d’une Langue Étrangère/Seconde, vol. 3: La Méthodologie. Brussels: De Boeck Supérieur.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (1997). The fine structure of the left periphery. In Haegeman, L., ed., Elements of Grammar, pp. 281337. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (2002). Locality and the left periphery. In Belletti, A. and Rizzi, L., eds, The Structure of CP and IP: The Cartography of Syntactic Structures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. and Roberts, I. (1989). Complex inversion in French. Probus 1: 130.Google Scholar
Roberts, I. (1985). Agreement parameters and the development of English modal auxiliaries. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 3: 2158.Google Scholar
Roberts, I. (1993). Verbs and Diachronic Syntax. Studies in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, vol. 28. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Roberts, I. (2007). Diachronic Syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Roberts, I. and Roussou, A. (2003). Syntactic Change: A Minimalist Approach to Grammaticalization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Roberts, L., Matsuo, A. and Duffield, N. (2013). Processing VP-ellipsis and VP-anaphora with structurally parallel and non-parallel antecedents: An eye-tracking study. Language and Cognitive Processes 28: 2947.Google Scholar
Robins, R. H. (1989). General Linguistics, fourth edition. Longman Linguistics Library. London and New York: Longman.Google Scholar
Robinson, I. (1975). The New Grammarians’ Funeral: A Critique of Noam Chomsky’s Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Robson, D. (2014). The mind-bending effects of feeling two hearts. BBC Online. www.bbc.com/future/story/20141205-the-man-with-two-heartsGoogle Scholar
Roeper, T. (1982). Review of Linguistic Theory and Psycholinguistic Reality (1981), edited by Halle, Bresnan and Miller. Language 58: 467–8.Google Scholar
Roland, D., Elman, J. L. and Ferreira, V. S. (2006). Why is that? Structural prediction and ambiguity resolution in a very large corpus of English sentences. Cognition 98: 245–72.Google Scholar
Roseberry, S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Parish-Morris, J. and Golinkoff, R. M. (2009). Live action: Can young children learn verbs from video? Child Development 80: 1360–75.Google Scholar
Ross, J. R. (Haj) (1967). Constraints on variables in syntax. PhD dissertation, Department of Linguistics and Philosophy, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Published as Infinite Syntax! (1986), Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Ross, J. R. (Haj) (1982). Pronoun deleting processes in German. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, San Diego.Google Scholar
Ross, J. R. (Haj) (2015). Why to syntax. Manuscript, University of North Texas.Google Scholar
Ruigendijk, M. E., Baauw, S., Zuckerman, S., Vasic, N., de Lange, J. and Avrutin, S. (2011). A cross-linguistic study on the interpretation of pronouns by children and agrammatic speakers: Evidence from Dutch, Spanish and Italian. In Gibson, E and Pearlmutter, N. J, eds, The Processing and Acquisition of Reference. Cambridge, MA, and London: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Russell, B. ([1911] 1917). Knowledge by acquaintance and knowledge by description. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society 11: 108128. Republished in Mysticism and Logic (1917), London: George Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
Russell, B. (1912). The Problems of Philosophy. London: Williams and Norgate.Google Scholar
Sabbagh, J. (2007). Ordering and linearizing rightward movement. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 25: 349401.Google Scholar
Saberi, K. and Perrott, D. R. (1999). Cognitive restoration of reversed speech. Nature 398 (6730): 760760.Google Scholar
Sabourin, L. (2003). Grammatical gender and second language processing: An ERP study. PhD dissertation, Department of Linguistics, Rijksuniversiteit Groningen.Google Scholar
Sachs, J. D. S. (1967). Recognition memory for syntactic and semantic aspects of connected discourse. Perception & Psychophysics 2: 437–42.Google Scholar
Sachs, O. (2007). Musicophilia: Tales of Music and the Brain. New York: First Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Sag, I. (1976). Deletion and logical form. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Salzmann, M., Häusler, J., Bader, M. and Bayer, J. (2013). That-trace effects without traces. An experimental investigation. In Keine, S., ed., Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Meeting of the North East Linguistic Society, pp. 149–62. Amherst, MA: GLSA Publications.Google Scholar
Sampson, G. (1980). Schools of Linguistics: Competition and Evolution. London: Hutchison.Google Scholar
Sampson, G. (2014). Minds in uniform: How generative grammar regiments culture, and why it shouldn’t. In Sampson, G. and Babarczy, A., eds, Grammar without Grammaticality: Growth and Limits of Grammatical Precision. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Sampson, G. (2015). Two ideas of creativity. Lingbuzz 002619. http://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/002619Google Scholar
Sanford, A. J. and Sturt, P. (2012). Depth of processing in language comprehension: Not noticing the evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6: 382–6.Google Scholar
Sapir, E. ([1907] 2008). The problem of an international auxiliary language. In Swiggers, P., ed., The Collected Works of Edward Sapir, vol. 1. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Sapir, E. (1921). Language: An Introduction to the Study of Speech. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company.Google Scholar
Saxton, M. (2010). Child Language: Acquisition and Development. London: SAGE Publications.Google Scholar
Schlinger, H. D. (2008). The long good-bye: Why B. F. Skinner’s Verbal Behavior is alive and well on the 50th anniversary of its publication. The Psychological Record 58: 329–37.Google Scholar
Schmid, M. (2013). Language Attrition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schmid, M. (2016). The best age to learn a second language. Independent. www.independent.co.uk/news/education/the-best-age-to-learn-a-second-language-a6860886.htmlGoogle Scholar
Schütze, C. T. (1996). The Empirical Base of Linguistics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. and Sprouse, R. (1996). L2 cognitive states and the Full Transfer/Full Access model. Second Language Research 12: 4072.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. and Vikner, S. (1996). The verb always leaves IP in V2 clauses. In Belletti, A. and Rizzi, L., eds, Parameters and Functional Heads, pp. 1163. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1975). Indirect speech acts. In Cole, P. and Morgan, J. L., eds, Syntax and Semantics, pp. 5982. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Segel, E. and Boroditsky, L. (2011). Grammar in art. Frontiers in Psychology 1. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00244Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S. and MacDonald, M. C. (1999). A probabilistic constraints approach to language acquisition and processing. Cognitive Science 23: 569–88.Google Scholar
Seliger, H. W. and Vago, R. M., eds (1991). First Language Attrition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sick, B. (2004). Der Dativ ist dem Genitiv sein Tod: Ein Wegweiser durch den Irrgarten der Deutschen Sprache. Cologne: Kiepenheuer and Witsch.Google Scholar
Sidtis, D. V. L. and Bridges, K. A. (2013). Formulaic language in Alzheimer’s disease. Aphasiology 27: 799810.Google Scholar
Sidtis, D. V. L., Kougentakis, K. M., Cameron, K., Falconer, C. and Sidtis, J. J. (2012). ‘Down with _______’: The linguistic schema as intermediary between formulaic and novel expressions. Yearbook of Phraseology 3: 87108.Google Scholar
Siegal, M. (2008). Marvelous Minds: The Discovery of What Children Know. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (2003). Language and thought online: Cognitive consequences of linguistic relativity. In Gentner, D and Goldin-Meadow, S, eds, Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought, pp. 157–92. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Smith, N. V. (1973). The Acquisition of Phonology: A Case Study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Snedeker, J. and Trueswell, J. (2003). Using prosody to avoid ambiguity: Effects of speaker awareness and referential context. Journal of Memory and Language 48: 103–30.Google Scholar
Soames, S. (1985). Semantics and psychology. In Katz, J., ed., The Philosophy of Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sobin, N. (1987). The variable status of comp-trace phenomena. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 5: 3360.Google Scholar
Sobin, N. (2002). The comp-trace effect, the adverb effect and minimal CP. Journal of Linguistics 38: 527–60.Google Scholar
Solan, L. (1983). Pronominal Reference: Child Language and the Theory of Grammar. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Sorace, A. (2000). Gradients in auxiliary selection with intransitive verbs. Language 76: 859–90.Google Scholar
Sorace, A. (2004). Native language attrition and developmental instability at the syntax–discourse interface: Data, interpretations and methods. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 7 (2): 143–5.Google Scholar
Sorensen, R. (2012). Veridical idealizations. In Frappier, M., Meynell, L. and Brown, J. R., eds, Thought Experiments in Science, Philosophy and the Arts, pp. 3052. New York and Oxford: Routledge.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. and Hespos, S. (2002). Conceptual development in infancy: The case of containment. In Stein, N., Bauer, P. and Rabinowitz, M., eds, Representation, Memory and Development: Essays in Honor of Jean Mandler, pp. 223–46. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. and Kinzler, K. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science 10: 8696.Google Scholar
Spinelli, E., Meunier, F. and Seigneuric, A. (2006). Spoken word recognition with gender-marked context. The Mental Lexicon 1: 277–97.Google Scholar
Spivey, M. J., McRae, K. and Joanisse, M. F., eds (2012). The Cambridge Handbook of Psycholinguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sportiche, D. (1995). French predicate clitics and clause structure. In Cardinaletti, A. and Guasti, M. T., eds, Small Clauses. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Reprinted in Sportiche (1998).Google Scholar
Sportiche, D. (1998). Partitions and Atoms of Clause Structure: Subjects, Agreement, Case and Clitics. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Sproat, R. and Shih, C. (1991). The crosslinguistic distribution of adjective ordering restrictions. In Georgopoulos, C. and Ishihara, R., eds, Interdisciplinary Approaches to Language: Essays in Honor of S.-Y. Kuroda, pp. 565–92. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Sprouse, J. and Almeida, D. (2012). Assessing the reliability of textbook data in syntax: Adger’s Core Syntax. Journal of Linguistics 48 (3): 609652.Google Scholar
Sprouse, J., Wagers, M. and Phillips, C. (2012). A test of the relation between working-memory capacity and syntactic island effects. Language 88: 82123.Google Scholar
Steiner, G. (1961). The retreat from the word. Kenyon Review 23: 187216.Google Scholar
Steiner, G. (1976). After Babel: Aspects of Language and Translation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Steiner, G. (1978). On Difficulty, and Other Essays. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Steiner, G. (1989). Real Presences: A Secondary City. London: University of Chicago Press/Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
Steinhauer, K. and Drury, J. E. (2012). On the early left-anterior negativity (ELAN) in syntax studies. Brain and Language 120: 135–62.Google Scholar
Stemberger, J. P. and MacWhinney, B. (1988). Are inflected forms stored in the lexicon. In Hammond, M. and Noonan, M., eds, Theoretical Morphology: Approaches in Modern Linguistics, pp. 101–16. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Stern, C. and Stern, W. (1907). Die Kindersprache [Children’s Language]. Reprinted by Kessinger Publishing LLC (2010).Google Scholar
Stone, T. and Davies, M. K. (2012). Theoretical issues in cognitive psychology. In Braisby, N. and Gellatly, A., eds, Cognitive Psychology, pp. 639–79. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stowe, L. (1986). Evidence for online gap location. Language and cognitive processes 1: 227–45.Google Scholar
Street, J. and Dabrowska, E. (2014). Lexically specific knowledge and individual differences in adult native speakers’ processing of the English passive. Applied Psycholinguistics 35: 97118.Google Scholar
Sussman, R. S. and Sedivy, J. C. (2003). The time-course of processing syntactic dependencies: Evidence from eye movements. Language and Cognitive Processes 18: 143–61.Google Scholar
Sutton, M., Lukyanenko, C. and Lidz, J. (2011). The onset of Principle C at 30 months: The role of vocabulary, syntactic development, and processing efficiency. In Danis, N., Mesh, K. and Sung, H., eds, Proceedings of the 35th Boston University Conference on Language Development, pp. 577–98. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Swain, M. (1985). Communicative competence: Some roles of comprehensible input and comprehensible output in its development. In Gass, S. and Madden, C., eds, Input in Second Language Acquisition, pp. 235–53. Rowley, MA: Newberry House.Google Scholar
Sweet, H. (1899). The Practical Study of Languages. London: Dent.Google Scholar
Syed, M. (2011). Bounce: The Myth of Talent and the Power of Practice. London: Fourth Estate.Google Scholar
Szabolcsi, A. and Zwarts, F. (1993). Weak islands and an algebraic semantics for scope taking. Natural Language Semantics 1 (3): 235–84.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (1985). Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In Shopen, T., ed., Language Typology and Syntactic Description, vol. 3: Grammatical Categories and the Lexicon, pp. 57149. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (2000). Toward a Cognitive Semantics: Typology and Process in Concept Structuring, vol. 2. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tchernichovski, O. and Marcus, G. (2014). Vocal learning beyond imitation: Mechanisms of adaptive vocal development in songbirds and human infants. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 28: 42–7.Google Scholar
Teodorescu, A. (2006). Adjective ordering restrictions revisited. In Baumer, D., Montero, D. and Scanlon, S., eds, Proceedings of the 25th West Coast Conference on Linguistics, pp. 399407. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Theakston, A. L. (2004). The role of entrenchment in children’s and adults’ performance on grammaticality judgment tasks. Cognitive Development 19: 1534.Google Scholar
Thomas, E. (2010). Sociophonetics: An Introduction. London: Palgrave.Google Scholar
Thompson, E., Palacios, A. and Varela, F. J. (2002). Ways of coloring: Comparative color vision as a case study for cognitive science. In Noë, A. and Thompson, E., eds, Vision and Mind: Selected Readings in the Philosophy of Perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thornton, R. (1994). Why continuity? In Brugos, A., Micciulla, L. and Smith, C., eds, Proceedings of the 28th Boston University Conference on Language Development, pp. 620–32. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Tillyard, E. M. (2011). The Elizabethan World Picture. London: Random House. (Original edition 1959.)Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1992). First Verbs: A Case Study of Early Grammatical Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Travis, L. (1984). Parameters and effects of word order variation. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Travis, L. (1991). Parameters of phrase structure and verb-second phenomena. In Freidin, R, ed., Principles and Parameters in Comparative Grammar, pp. 339364. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Trudgill, P. (2001). Received Pronunciation: Sociolinguistic aspects. Studia Anglica Posnaniensia 36: 313.Google Scholar
Trudgill, P. (2008). The historical sociolinguistics of elite accent change: On why RP is not disappearing. Studia Anglica Posnaniensia 44: 311.Google Scholar
Truss, L. (2003). Eats, Shoots and Leaves. London: Profile Books.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T. (2001a). The declarative/procedural model of the lexicon and grammar. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 30: 3769.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T. (2001b). The neural basis of lexicon and grammar in first and second language: The declarative/procedural model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 4: 105–22.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition 931: 231–70.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T. and Pierpont, E. I. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex 41: 399433.Google Scholar
van Urk, C. and Richards, N. (2013). Two components of long-distance extraction: Successive cyclicity in Dinka. Lingbuzz 001766. http://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/001766Google Scholar
Van Valin, R. D. and La Polla, J. R. (1997). Syntax: Structure, Meaning and Function. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Vardon-Smith, G. M. (2014). Safety and security: Staying safe in a humanitarian conflict. In Ryan, J. et al., eds, Conflict and Catastrophe Medicine: A Practical Guide, pp. 341360. London: Springer.Google Scholar
Varga, E. (2005). Lexical V-to-I raising in Late Modern English. GG@G (Generative Grammar in Geneva) 4: 261–85.Google Scholar
Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K., Alcock, K., Fletcher, P. and Passingham, R. (1995). Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large family with a genetically transmitted speech and language disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 92: 930–3.Google Scholar
Vidal, J. (2014). Why we are losing a world of languages. The Guardian. www.theguardian.com/environment/2014/jun/08/why-we-are-losing- a-world-of-languagesGoogle Scholar
Vitevich, M. S. (1997). The neighborhood characteristics of malapropisms. Language and Speech 40: 211–28.Google Scholar
de Vries, M. (2009). On multidominance and linearization. Biolinguistics 3: 344403.Google Scholar
Vulanović, R. (2010). The rise and fall of periphrastic do in affirmative declaratives. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics 12: 131.Google Scholar
Walker, J. (2012). Variation in Linguistic Systems. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Wang, W. S.-Y. (1969). Competing changes as a cause of residue. Language 45: 925.Google Scholar
Warner, A. R. (2005). Why do dove: Evidence for register variation in Early Modern English negatives. Language Variation and Change 17: 257–80.Google Scholar
Warner, N., Butler, L. and Geary, Q. (2016). Mutsun–English English–Mutsun Dictionary: mutsun-inkiS inkiS-mutsun riica pappel. Language Documentation & Conservation Special Publication no. 11. Honolulu: University of Hawai‘i Press. http://hdl.handle.net/10125/24679Google Scholar
Warren, P. (2013). Introducing Psycholinguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wasow, T. (2015). Ambiguity avoidance is overrated. In Winckler, S., ed., Ambiguity: Language and Communication, pp. 2947. Berlin: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Weijerman, M. E. and de Winter, J. P. (2010). Clinical practice: The care of children with Down Syndrome. European Journal of Pediatrics 169: 1445–52.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D. and Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development 72: 655–84.Google Scholar
Werker, J. and Lalonde, C. E. (1988). Cross-language speech perception: Initial capabilities and developmental change. Developmental Psychology 24: 672–83.Google Scholar
Werker, J. and Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development 7: 4963.Google Scholar
Wexler, K. (1994). Optional infinitives, head movement and the economy of derivations. In Lightfoot, D. and Hornstein, N., eds, Verb Movement, pp. 305–50. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wexler, K. (1998). Very early parameter-setting and the unique checking constraint: A new explanation of the optional infinitive stage. Lingua 106: 2379.Google Scholar
Wexler, P. (1990). The Schizoid Nature of Modern Hebrew: A Slavic Language in Search of a Semitic Past. Mediterranean Language and Culture Monograph Series, vol. 4. Wiesbaden: Otto Harrassowitz.Google Scholar
Whaley, L. J. (1998). Introduction to Typology: The Unity and Diversity of Languages. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.Google Scholar
White, L. (1987). Against comprehensible input: The input hypothesis and the development of second-language competence. Applied Linguistics 8: 95110.Google Scholar
White, L. (1996). Universal Grammar and second language acquisition: Current trends and new directions. In Ritchie, W. and Bhatia, T., eds, Handbook of Language Acquisition. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
White, L. (2003). Second Language Acquisition and Universal Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wiggins, G. (2013). Computer models of music cognition. In Rebuschat, P., ed., Language and Music as Cognitive Systems, pp. 169–88. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wilshire, C. E. (2008). Cognitive neuropsychological approaches to word production in aphasia: Beyond boxes and arrows. Aphasiology 22: 1019–53.Google Scholar
Winer, G. A., Cottrell, J. E., Gregg, V., Fournier, J. S. and Bica, L. A. (2002). Fundamentally misunderstanding visual perception: Adults’ belief in visual emissions. American Psychologist 57: 417–24.Google Scholar
Winograd, T. (1983). Language as a Cognitive Process, vol. I: Syntax. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
de Winter, J. C. F. and Dodou, D. (2010). Five-point Likert items: T-Test versus Mann–Whitney–Wilcoxon. Practical Assessment, Research and Evaluation 15 (11): 17.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1874). Grundzüge der Physiologischen Psychologie [Principles of Physiological Psychology]. Leipzig: Engelmann.Google Scholar
Xia, L., Murray, A., Zheng, D., Liu, F., Ye, X. and Ning, G. (2012). Cardiovascular system modeling (editorial). Computational and Mathematical Methods in Medicine 2012: 583172.Google Scholar
Yngve, V. H. (1960). A model and a hypothesis for language structure. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 104: 444–66.Google Scholar
Yngve, V. H. (1986). Linguistics as a Science. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Zhang, Y., Kuhl, P. K., Imada, T., Kotani, M. and Mohkura, Y. (2005). Effects of language experience: Neural commitment to language-specific auditory patterns Neuroimage 26: 703–20.Google Scholar
Zubizarreta, M. L. (1998). Prosody, Focus and Word Order. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Zukav, G. (2001). The Dancing Wu Li Masters: An Overview of the New Physics. New York: Perennial Classics.Google Scholar
Zwicky, A. M. (1978/79). Classical malapropisms. Language Sciences 1: 339–49.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Nigel Duffield
  • Book: Reflections on Psycholinguistic Theories
  • Online publication: 28 December 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108264969.031
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Nigel Duffield
  • Book: Reflections on Psycholinguistic Theories
  • Online publication: 28 December 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108264969.031
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Nigel Duffield
  • Book: Reflections on Psycholinguistic Theories
  • Online publication: 28 December 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108264969.031
Available formats
×