Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-5wvtr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-20T18:53:25.745Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

21 - Individual Differences in Reasoning and the Algorithmic/Intentional Level Distinction in Cognitive Science

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Keith E. Stanovich
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Jonathan E. Adler
Affiliation:
Brooklyn College, City University of New York
Lance J. Rips
Affiliation:
Northwestern University, Illinois
Get access

Summary

When a layperson thinks of individual differences in reasoning they think of IQ tests. It is quite natural that this is their primary associate, because IQ tests are among the most publicized products of psychological research. This association is not entirely inaccurate either, because intelligence – as measured using IQ-like instruments – is correlated with performance on a host of reasoning tasks (Ackerman, Kyllonen, and Roberts 1999; Carroll 1993; Hunt 1999; Lohman 2000; Lubinski 2004; Rips and Conrad 1983; Sternberg 1977, 1985). Nonetheless, a major theme of this chapter will be that certain very important classes of individual differences in thinking are ignored if only intelligence-related variance is the primary focus. A number of these ignored classes of individual differences are those relating to rational thought.

In this chapter, I will argue that intelligence-related individual differences in thinking are largely the result of differences at the algorithmic level of cognitive control. Intelligence tests thus largely fail to tap processes at the intentional level of control. Because understanding rational behavior necessitates understanding processes operating at both levels, an exclusive focus on intelligence-related individual differences will tend to obscure important differences in human thinking.

This argument obviously depends strongly on differentiating the algorithmic from the intentional level of analysis. Therefore, in the next section I will outline the sources that I rely on for this conceptual distinction and how I will utilize the distinction to provide a framework for thinking about individual differences in reasoning.

Type
Chapter
Information
Reasoning
Studies of Human Inference and its Foundations
, pp. 414 - 436
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, P. L. (1994). Intelligence, attention, and learning: Maximal and typical performance. In Detterman, D. K. (Ed.), Current topics in human intelligence (Vol. 4) (pp. 1–27). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Ackerman, P. L. (1996). A theory of adult development: Process, personality, interests, and knowledge. Intelligence, 22, 227–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ackerman, P. L., & Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Intelligence, personality, and interests: Evidence for overlapping traits. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 219–245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ackerman, P. L., Kyllonen, P., and Roberts, R. (Eds.) (1999). The future of learning and individual differences research: Processes, traits, and content. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adler, J. E. (1984). Abstraction is uncooperative. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 14, 165–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adler, J. E. (1991). An optimist's pessimism: Conversation and conjunctions. In Eells, E. & Maruszewski, T. (Eds.), Probability and rationality: Studies on L. Jonathan Cohen's philosophy of science (pp. 251–282). Amsterdam: Editions Rodopi.Google Scholar
Adler, J. E. (1998). Rationality of belief. In Craig, E. (Ed.), Routledge encyclopedia of philosophy (pp. 86–90). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1990). The adaptive character of thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1991). Is human cognition adaptive?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14, 471–517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, M. (1992). Intelligence and development: A cognitive theory. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Anderson, M. (1998). Mental retardation, general intelligence, and modularity. Learning and Individual Differences, 10, 159–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atran, S. (1998). Folk biology and the anthropology of science: Cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 547–609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Austin, E. J., and Deary, I. J. (2002). Personality dispositions. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Why smart people can be so stupid (pp. 187–211). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556–559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A., Chincotta, D., and Adlam, A. (2001). Working memory and the control of action: Evidence from task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 641–657.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bara, B. G., Bucciarelli, M., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1995). Development of syllogistic reasoning. American Journal of Psychology, 108, 157–193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, J. (1985). Rationality and intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J. (1995). Myside bias in thinking about abortion. Thinking and Reasoning, 1, 221–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J., and Hershey, J. C. (1988). Outcome bias in decision evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 569–579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bermudez, J. L. (2001). Normativity and rationality in delusional psychiatric disorders. Mind & Language, 16, 457–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bratman, M. E., Israel, D. J., & Pollack, M. E. (1991). Plans and resource-bounded practical reasoning. In Cummins, J. & Pollock, J. (Eds.), Philosophy and AI: Essays at the interface (pp. 7–22). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brook, A., & Ross, D. (2002). Dennett's position in the intellectual world. In Brook, A. & Ross, D. (Eds.), Daniel Dennett (pp. 3–37). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (1991). Evolutionary personality psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 42, 459–491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J., & Jarvis, W. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 197–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., & Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological Review, 97, 404–431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1997). Psychometrics, intelligence, and public perception. Intelligence, 24, 25–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2002). The cognitive functions of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 657–726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carruthers, P., Stich, S., & Siegal, M. (Eds.). (2002). The cognitive basis of science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory for fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1998). Where is intelligence? Some answers from the triadic theory. In McArdle, J. J. & Woodcock, R. W. (Eds.), Human cognitive abilities in theory and practice (pp. 29–38). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cederblom, J. (1989). Willingness to reason and the identification of the self. In Maimon, E., Nodine, D., & Conner, O' (Eds.), Thinking, reasoning, and writing (pp. 147–159). New York: Longman.Google Scholar
Cherniak, C. (1986). Minimal rationality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, L. J. (1981). Can human irrationality be experimentally demonstrated?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 317–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, L. J. (1982). Are people programmed to commit fallacies? Further thoughts about the interpretation of experimental data on probability judgment. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 12, 251–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colom, R., Rebollo, I., Palacios, A., Juan-Espinosa, M., & Kyllonen, P. C. (2004). Working memory is (almost) perfectly predicted by g. Intelligence, 32, 277–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., Bunting, M. F., Therriault, D. J., & Minkoff, S. R. B. (2002). A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 30, 163–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, A. R. A., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7, 547–552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Copeland, D. E., & Radvansky, G. A. (2004). Working memory and syllogistic reasoning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 1437–1457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 163–228). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). Consider the source: The evolution of adaptations for decoupling and metarepresentation. In Sperber, D. (Ed.), Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective (pp. 53–115). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO personality inventory. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Cronbach, L. J. (1949). Essentials of psychological testing. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Currie, G., & Ravenscroft, I. (2002). Recreative minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniel, M. H. (2000). Interpretation of intelligence test scores. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 477–491). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, M. (2000). Interaction without reduction: The relationship between personal and sub-personal levels of description. Mind & Society, 1, 87–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J. (2000). Looking down on human intelligence: From psychometrics to the brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denes-Raj, V., & Epstein, S. (1994). Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: When people behave against their better judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 819–829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennett, D. C. (1978). Brainstorms: Philosophical essays on mind and psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1984). Elbow room: The varieties of free will worth wanting. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1987). The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1996). Kinds of minds: Toward an understanding of consciousness. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
DeShon, R. P., Smith, M. R., Chan, D., & Schmitt, N. (1998). Can racial differences in cognitive test performance be reduced by presenting problems in a social context?Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 438–451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dias, M., Roazzi, A., & Harris, P. L. (2005). Reasoning from unfamiliar premises: A study with unschooled adults. Psychological Science, 16, 550–554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dienes, Z., & Perner, J. (1999). A theory of implicit and explicit knowledge. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 735–808.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dulany, D. E., & Hilton, D. J. (1991). Conversational implicature, conscious representation, and the conjunction fallacy. Social Cognition, 9, 85–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, J., Emslie, H., Williams, P., Johnson, R., & Freer, C. (1996). Intelligence and the frontal lobe: The organization of goal-directed behavior. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 257–303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elton, M. (2003). Daniel Dennett: Reconciling science and our self-conception. Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, S., & Meier, P. (1989). Constructive thinking: A broad coping variable with specific components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 332–350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2002). The influence of prior belief on scientific thinking. In Carruthers, P., Stich, S., & Siegal, M. (Eds.), The cognitive basis of science (pp. 193–210). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2003). In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 454–459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2005). Deductive reasoning. In Holyoak, K. J. & Morrison, R. G. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 169–184). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T., Barston, J., & Pollard, P. (1983). On the conflict between logic and belief in syllogistic reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 11, 295–306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. St. B. T., & Feeney, A. (2004). The role of prior belief in reasoning. In Leighton, J. P. & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of reasoning (pp. 78–102). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T., Newstead, S., Allen, J., & Pollard, P. (1994). Debiasing by instruction: The case of belief bias. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 6, 263–285.Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T., & Over, D. E. (1996). Rationality and reasoning. Hove, England: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T., & Over, D. E. (1999). Explicit representations in hypothetical thinking. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 763–764.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T., & Over, D. E. (2004). If. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T., & Perry, T. (1995). Belief bias in children's reasoning. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 14, 103–115.Google Scholar
Barrett, Feldman L., Tugade, M. M., & Engle, R. W. (2004). Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 553–573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fetherstonhaugh, D., Slovic, P., Johnson, S. M., & Friedrich, J. (1997). Insensitivity to the value of human life: A study of psychophysical numbing. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 14, 282–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fong, G. T., Krantz, D. H., & Nisbett, R. E. (1986). The effects of statistical training on thinking about everyday problems. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 253–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frankfurt, H. (1971). Freedom of the will and the concept of a person. Journal of Philosophy, 68, 5–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuster, J. M. (1990). Prefrontal cortex and the bridging of temporal gaps in the perception-action cycle. In Diamond, A. (Ed.), The development and neural bases of higher cognitive functions (pp. 318–336). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google ScholarPubMed
Geary, D. C. (2005). The origin of the mind: Evolution of brain, cognition, and general intelligence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A., & Faust, M. E. (1991). The mechanism of suppression: A component of general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 245–262.Google ScholarPubMed
Gigerenzer, G. (1996). On narrow norms and vague heuristics: A reply to Kahneman and Tversky (1996). Psychological Review, 103, 592–596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilhooly, K. J. (2004). Working memory and reasoning. In Leighton, J. P. & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of reasoning (pp. 49–77). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gilovich, T. (1991). How we know what isn't so. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Girotto, V. (2004). Task understanding. In Leighton, J. P. & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of reasoning (pp. 103–125). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 1–55.Google ScholarPubMed
Goel, V., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Explaining modulation of reasoning by belief. Cognition, 87, B11–B22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1992). Working memory and the mind. Scientific American, 267, 111–117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gould, S. J. (1991). Bully for the Brontosaurus. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Gray, J. R., Chabris, C. F., & Braver, T. S. (2003). Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 316–322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffin, D., & Tversky, A. (1992). The weighing of evidence and the determinants of confidence. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 411–435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hambrich, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2003). The role of working memory in problem solving. In Davidson, J. E. & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), The psychology of problem solving (pp. 176–206). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Handley, S. J., Capon, A., Beveridge, M., Dennis, I., & Evans, J. St. B. T. (2004). Working memory, inhibitory control and the development of children's reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning, 10, 175–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hasher, L., Zacks, R. T., & May, C. P. (1999). Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age. In Gopher, D. & Koriat, A. (Eds.), Attention & Performance ⁩VII, Cognitive Regulation of Performance: Interaction of Theory and Application (pp. 653–675). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hilton, D. J. (1995). The social context of reasoning: Conversational inference and rational judgment. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 248–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horgan, T., & Tienson, J. (1993). Levels of description in nonclassical cognitive science. In Hookway, C. & Peterson, D. (Eds.), Philosophy and cognitive science (pp. 159–188). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Horn, J. L., & Cattell, R. B. (1967). Age differences in fluid and crystallized intelligence. Acta Psychologica, 26, 1–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horn, J. L., & Noll, J. (1997). Human cognitive capabilities: Gf-Gc theory. In Flanagan, D., Genshaft, J., & Harrison, P. (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (pp. 53–91). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hunt, E. (1987). The next word on verbal ability. In Vernon, P. A. (Ed.), Speed of information-processing and intelligence (pp. 347–392). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Hunt, E. (1999). Intelligence and human resources: Past, present, and future. In Ackerman, P., Kyllonen, P., & Richards, R. (Eds.), Learning and individual differences: Process, trait, and content determinants (pp. 3–28). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (1996). How language helps us think. Pragmatics and Cognition, 4, 1–34.Google Scholar
Jepson, C., Krantz, D., & Nisbett, R. (1983). Inductive reasoning: Competence or skill?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 494–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1999). Deductive reasoning. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 109–135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2005). Mental models and thought. In Holyoak, K. J. & Morrison, R. G. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 185–208). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., & Kahneman, D. (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 49–81). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2005). A model of heuristic judgment. In Holyoak, K. J. & Morrison, R. G. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of thinking andreasoning (pp. 267–293). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93, 136–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1996). On the reality of cognitive illusions. Psychological Review, 103, 582–591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (Eds.). (2000). Choices, values, and frames. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kane, M. J. (2003). The intelligent brain in conflict. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 375–377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M., Conway, A., & Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled-attention view of WM capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 169–183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 637–671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 47–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., Tuholski, S. W., Wilhelm, O., Payne, T., & Engle, R. W. (2004). The generality of working memory capacity: A latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 189–217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kardash, C. M., & Scholes, R. J. (1996). Effects of pre-existing beliefs, epistemological beliefs, and need for cognition on interpretation of controversial issues. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 260–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirkpatrick, L., & Epstein, S. (1992). Cognitive-experiential self-theory and subjective probability: Evidence for two conceptual systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 534–544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klaczynski, P. A. (1997). Bias in adolescents' everyday reasoning and its relationship with intellectual ability, personal theories, and self-serving motivation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 273–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klaczynski, P. A., & Gordon, D. H. (1996). Self-serving influences on adolescents' evaluations of belief-relevant evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 62, 317–339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klaczynski, P. A., Gordon, D. H., & Fauth, J. (1997). Goal-oriented critical reasoning and individual differences in critical reasoning biases. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 470–485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klaczynski, P. A., & Lavallee, K. L. (2005). Domain-specific identity, epistemic regulation, and intellectual ability as predictors of belief-based reasoning: A dual-process perspective. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klaczynski, P. A., & Robinson, B. (2000). Personal theories, intellectual ability, and epistemological beliefs: Adult age differences in everyday reasoning tasks. Psychology and Aging, 15, 400–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kokis, J, Macpherson, R., Toplak, M., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2002). Heuristic and analytic processing: Age trends and associations with cognitive ability and cognitive styles. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83, 26–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kruglanski, A. W., & Webster, D. M. (1996). Motivated closing the mind: “Seizing” and “freezing”. Psychological Review, 103, 263–283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhberger, A. (2002). The rationality of risky decisions: A changing message. Theory & Psychology, 12, 427–452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “Theory of Mind.”Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. (1995). Chapters of psychology. In Solso, R. L. & Massaro, D. W. (Eds.), The science of the mind: 2001 and beyond (pp. 184–202). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lillard, A. (2001). Pretend play as twin Earth: A social-cognitive analysis. Developmental Review, 21, 495–531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lohman, D. F. (2000). Complex information processing and intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 285–340). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopes, L. (1991). The rhetoric of irrationality. Theory & Psychology, 1, 65–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubinski, D. (2004). Introduction to the special section on cognitive abilities: 100 years after Spearman's (1904) “General Intelligence, Objectively Determined and Measured.”Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 96–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manktelow, K. I. (2004). Reasoning and rationality: The pure and the practical. In Manktelow, K. I. & Chung, M. C. (Eds.), Psychology of reasoning: Theoretical and historical perspectives (pp. 157–177). Hove, England: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Margolis, H. (1987). Patterns, thinking, and cognition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Markovits, H., & Doyon, C. (2004). Information processing and reasoning with premises that are empirically false: Interference, working memory, and processing speed. Memory & Cognition, 32, 592–601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markovits, H., & Nantel, G. (1989). The belief-bias effect in the production and evaluation of logical conclusions. Memory & Cognition, 17, 11–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Matthews, G., & Deary, I. J. (1998). Personality traits. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Matthews, G., Zeidner, M., & Roberts, R. D. (2002). Emotional intelligence: Science & myth. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science, 306, 503–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509–516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrew, K. S. (1997). Analysis of major intelligence batteries according to a proposed comprehensive Gf-Gc framework. In Flanagan, D., Genshaft, J., & Harrison, P. (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (pp. 151–180). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
McGrew, K. S., & Woodcock, R. W. (2001). Technical Manual. Woodcock-Johnson III. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.Google Scholar
Mellers, B., Hertwig, R., & Kahneman, D. (2001). Do frequency representations eliminate conjunction effects? An exercise in adversarial collaboration. Psychological Science, 12, 269–275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mirolli, M. (2002). A naturalistic perspective on intentionality: Interview with Daniel Dennett. Mind & Society, 3, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mithen, S. (1996). The prehistory of mind: The cognitive origins of art and science. London: Thames and Hudson.Google Scholar
Moshman, D. (1994). Reasoning, metareasoning, and the promotion of rationality. In Demetriou, A. & Efklides, A. (Eds.), Intelligence, mind, and reasoning: Structure and development (pp. 135–150). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Moutier, S., Angeard, N., & Houde, O. (2002). Deductive reasoning and matching-bias inhibition training: Evidence from a debiasing paradigm. Thinking and Reasoning, 8, 205–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muller, U., Zelazo, P. D., Hood, S., Leone, T., & Rohrer, L. (2004). Interference control in a new rule use task: Age-related changes, labeling, and attention. Child Development, 75, 1594–1609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newell, A. (1982). The knowledge level. Artificial Intelligence, 18, 87–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newell, A. (1990). Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Newstead, S. E., Handley, S. J., Harley, C., Wright, H., & Farrelly, D. (2004). Individual differences in deductive reasoning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 33–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nichols, S., & Stich, S. P. (2003). Mindreading: An integrated account of pretence, self-awareness, and understanding other minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2, 175–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nickerson, R. S. (2004). Teaching reasoning. In Leighton, J. P. & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of reasoning (pp. 410–442). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nisbett, L., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Norris, S. P. (1992). Testing for the disposition to think critically. Informal Logic, 14, 157–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakhill, J., Johnson-Laird, P. N., & Garnham, A. (1989). Believability and syllogistic reasoning. Cognition, 31, 117–140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (1995). Theories of reasoning and the computational explanation of everyday inference. Thinking and Reasoning, 1, 121–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osman, M. (2004). An evaluation of dual-process theories of reasoning. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 988–1010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Over, D. E. (2002). The rationality of evolutionary psychology. In Bermudez, J. L. & Millar, A. (Eds.), Reason and nature: Essays in the theory of rationality (pp. 187–207). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999a). The interaction of three facets of concrete thinking in a game of chance. Thinking and Reasoning, 5, 303–325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999b). The relation of rational and experiential information processing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 972–987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, A. M., & Fischhoff, B. (2005). Decision-making competence: External validation through an individual differences approach. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 18, 1–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paul, R. W. (1984). Critical thinking: Fundamental to education for a free society. Educational Leadership, 42(1), 4–14.Google Scholar
Paul, R. W. (1987). Critical thinking and the critical person. In Perkins, D. N., Lockhead, J., & Bishop, J. (Eds.), Thinking: The second international conference (pp. 373–403). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Perkins, D. N. (1995). Outsmarting IQ: The emerging science of learnable intelligence. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Politzer, G., & Macchi, L. (2000). Reasoning and pragmatics. Mind & Society, 1, 73–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Politzer, G., & Noveck, I. A. (1991). Are conjunction rule violations the result of conversational rule violations?Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 20, 83–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollock, J. L. (1995). Cognitive carpentry: A blueprint for how to build a person. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., & Bering, J. M. (2002). The mentality of apes revisited. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(4), 115–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., & Giambrone, S. (2001). Reasoning about beliefs: A human specialization?Child Development, 72, 691–695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pylyshyn, Z. (1984). Computation and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Reber, A. S. (1992). The cognitive unconscious: An evolutionary perspective. Consciousness and Cognition, 1, 93–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Reber, A. S., Walkenfeld, F. F., & Hernstadt, R. (1991). Implicit and explicit learning: Individual differences and IQ. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 888–896.Google Scholar
Rescher, N. (1988). Rationality: A philosophical inquiry into the nature and rationale of reason. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rips, L. J. (1994). The psychology of proof. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rips, L. J., & Conrad, F. G. (1983). Individual differences in deduction. Cognition and Brain Theory, 6, 259–285.Google Scholar
Roese, N. (1997). Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 131–148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rokeach, M. (1960). The open and closed mind. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Ross, R. (2000). Introduction: The Dennettian stance. In Ross, R., Brook, A., & Thompson, D. (Eds.), Dennett's philosophy: A comprehensive assessment (pp. 1–26). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
, W., Kelley, C., Ho, C., & Stanovich, K. E. (2005). Thinking about personal theories: Individual differences in the coordination of theory and evidence. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1149–1161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, W., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (1999). The domain specificity and generality of belief bias: Searching for a generalizable critical thinking skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 497–510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saks, M., & Kidd, R. (1980–1981). Human information processing and adjudication: Trial by heuristics. Law and Society Review, 15, 123–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A., Atkinson, T. M., & Berish, D. E. (2003). Executive functioning as a potential mediator of age-related cognitive decline in normal adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 566–594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samuels, R., & Stich, S. P. (2004). Rationality and psychology. In Mele, A. R. & Rawling, P. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of rationality (pp. 279–300). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Samuels, R., Stich, S. P., & Bishop, M. (2002). Ending the rationality wars: How to make disputes about human rationality disappear. In Elio, R. (Ed.), Common sense, reasoning and rationality (pp. 236–268). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheffler, I. (1991). In praise of the cognitive emotions. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Schoenfeld, A. H. (1983). Beyond the purely cognitive: Belief systems, social cognitions, and metacognitions as driving forces in intellectual performance. Cognitive Science, 7, 329–363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schommer, M. (1990). Effects of beliefs about the nature of knowledge on comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 498–504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schommer-Aikins, M. (2004). Explaining the epistemological belief system: Introducing the embedded systemic model and coordinated research approach. Educational Psychologist, 39, 19–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shafir, E., & LeBoeuf, R. A. (2002). Rationality. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 491–517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shafir, E., & LeBoeuf, R. A. (2005). Decision making. In Holyoak, K. J. & Morrison, R. G. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 243–265). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Simoneau, M., & Markovits, H. (2003). Reasoning with premises that are not empirically true: Evidence for the role of inhibition and retrieval. Developmental Psychology, 39, 964–975.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinatra, G. M., & Pintrich, P. R. (Eds.). (2003). Intentional conceptual change. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sloman, A. (1993). The mind as a control system. In Hookway, C. & Peterson, D. (Eds.), Philosophy and cognitive science (pp. 69–110). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloman, S. A. (2002). Two systems of reasoning. In Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., & Kahneman, D. (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 379–396). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2002). The affect heuristic. In Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., & Kahneman, D. (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 397–420). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. (1994). The modularity of thought and the epidemiology of representations. In Hirschfeld, L. A. & Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 39–67). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. (2000). Metarepresentations in evolutionary perspective. In Sperber, D. (Ed.), Metarepresentations: A Multidisciplinary Perspective (pp. 117–137). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1989). Implicit philosophies of mind: The dualism scale and its relationships with religiosity and belief in extrasensory perception. Journal of Psychology, 123, 5–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1999). Who is rational? Studies of individual differences in reasoning. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2002). Rationality, intelligence, and levels of analysis in cognitive science: Is dysrationalia possible? In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Why smart people can be so stupid (pp. 124–158). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2003). The fundamental computational biases of human cognition: Heuristics that (sometimes) impair decision making and problem solving. In Davidson, J. E. & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), The psychology of problem solving (pp. 291–342). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2004). The robot's rebellion: Finding meaning in the age of Darwin. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2008). What IQ tests miss: The cognitive science of rational and irrational thinking. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1997). Reasoning independently of prior belief and individual differences in actively open-minded thinking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 342–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998a). Cognitive ability and variation in selection task performance. Thinking and Reasoning, 4, 193–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998b). Individual differences in framing and conjunction effects. Thinking and Reasoning, 4, 289–317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998c). Individual differences in rational thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 161–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1999). Discrepancies between normative and descriptive models of decision making and the understanding/acceptance principle. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 349–385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645–726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, E. (1996). Without good reason: The rationality debate in philosophy and cognitive science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (1990). The representational theory of mind: An introduction. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2001). The evolution of agency and other essays. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2003). Thought in a hostile world: The evolution of human cognition. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1977). Intelligence, information processing, and analogical reasoning. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Thinking styles. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.). (2000). Handbook of intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2003). Wisdom, intelligence, and creativity synthesized. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (Eds.). (2002). The general factor of intelligence: How general is it?Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Stich, S. P. (1990). The fragmentation of reason. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sub, H.-M., Oberauer, K., Wittmann, W. W., Wilhelm, O., & Schulze, R. (2002). Working-memory capacity explains reasoning ability – and a little bit more. Intelligence, 30, 261–288.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., & Whiten, A. (2001). Mental evolution and development: Evidence for secondary representation in children, great apes, and other animals. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 629–650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taub, G. E., & McGrew, K. S. (2004). A confirmatory factor analysis of Cattell-Horn-Carroll theory and cross-age invariance of the Woodcock-Johnson tests of cognitive abilities III. School Psychology Quarterly, 19, 72–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tetlock, P. E., & Mellers, B. A. (2002). The great rationality debate. Psychological Science, 13, 94–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thurstone, L. L. (1927). The nature of intelligence. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company.Google Scholar
Tobacyk, J., & Milford, G. (1983). Belief in paranormal phenomena. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1029–1037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Toplak, M., & Stanovich, K. E. (2002). The domain specificity and generality of disjunctive reasoning: Searching for a generalizable critical thinking skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 197–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tor, A., & Bazerman, M. H. (2003). Focusing failures in competitive environments: Explaining decision errors in the Monty Hall game, the Acquiring a Company problem, and multiparty ultimatums. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 16, 353–374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90, 293–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valentine, E. R. (1975). Performance on two reasoning tasks in relation to intelligence, divergence and interference proneness: Content and context effects in reasoning. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 45, 198–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verschueren, N., Schaeken, W., & Ydewalle, D' G. (2005). Everyday conditional reasoning: A working memory-dependent tradeoff between counterexample and likelihood use. Memory & Cognition, 33, 107–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vinter, A., & Detable, C. (2003). Implicit learning in children and adolescents with mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 108, 94–107.2.0.CO;2>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vinter, A., & Perruchet, P. (2000). Implicit learning in children is not related to age: Evidence from drawing behavior. Child Development, 71, 1223–1240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K. (2002). Smart people doing dumb things: The case of managerial incompetence. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Why smart people can be so stupid (pp. 42–63). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Practical intelligence in real-world pursuits: The role of tacit knowledge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 436–458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, G., & Glaser, E. M. (1980). Watson-Glaser critical thinking appraisal. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2003). Is probability matching smart? Associations between probabilistic choices and cognitive ability. Memory & Cognition, 31, 243–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A. (2001). Meta-representation and secondary representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamagishi, K. (1997). When a 12.86% mortality is more dangerous than 24.14%: Implications for risk communication. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 495–506.3.0.CO;2-J>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zacks, R. T., Hasher, L., & Sanft, H. (1982). Automatic encoding of event frequency: Further findings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8, 106–116.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×