Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-jbqgn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-22T13:54:02.674Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Developmental Changes in Question-Asking

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2020

Lucas Payne Butler
Affiliation:
University of Maryland, College Park
Samuel Ronfard
Affiliation:
University of Toronto Mississauga
Kathleen H. Corriveau
Affiliation:
Boston University
Get access

Summary

Asking questions is a powerful learning tool that children take full advantage of, as they are well-known to be prolific and determined question-askers. But do children ask good questions? In this chapter, we review and discuss qualitative and quantitative studies to trace the developmental trajectory of children’s question–asking strategies, focusing on their effectiveness and adaptiveness. Previous research has so far established three milestones: children’s question–asking abilities evolve from being able to identify effective questions, but not being able to spontaneously generate them at the age of five, to beginning to generate effective questions from scratch at age seven, to implementing efficient and adaptive question–asking strategies by the age of ten, echoing adult–level patterns of performance. We discuss how the cognitive and environmental factors driving these developmental changes still remain unclear, and how taking a multidisciplinary approach might be necessary to fill these gaps. We argue that the results from research on question-asking have the potential to inform educational policies, and to help design targeted training interventions and educational curricula that exploit the early emergence of these skills and support their further development, providing children with a toolbox of strategies and concepts they can use to effectively navigate the world.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Questioning Child
Insights from Psychology and Education
, pp. 118 - 143
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Begus, K., Southgate, V., and Gliga, T. (2015). Neural mechanisms of infant learning: Differences in frontal theta activity during object exploration modulate subsequent object recognition. Biology Letters, 11, 20150041. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2015.0041Google Scholar
Berretty, P. M., Todd, P. M., and Blythe, P. W. (1997). Categorization by elimination: A fast and frugal approach to categorization. Proceedings of the Nineteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 43–8). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Berretty, P. M., Todd, P. M., and Martignon, L. (1999). Categorization by elimination: Using few cues to choose. In Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., and the ABC Research Group (eds.) Evolution and cognition: Simple heuristics that make us smart (pp. 235–54). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Best, J. R., and Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81, 1641–60. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.2010.01499.xGoogle Scholar
Birch, S. A., Vauthier, S. A., and Bloom, P. (2008). Three-and four-year-olds spontaneously use others’ past performance to guide their learning. Cognition, 107, 1018–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.008Google Scholar
Bridgers, S., Gweon, H., Bretzke, M., and Ruggeri, A. (2018). How you learned matters: The process by which others learn informs young children’s decisions about whom to ask for help. Proceedings of the 40th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Callanan, M. A., and Oakes, L. M. (1992). Preschoolers’ questions and parents’ explanations: Causal thinking in everyday activity. Cognitive Development, 7, 213–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885–2014(92)90012–gGoogle Scholar
Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chase, C. C., and Klahr, D. (2017). Invention versus direct instruction: For some content, it’s a tie. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 26, 582–96. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-017–9700–6Google Scholar
Chen, Z., and Klahr, D. (1999). All other things being equal: Acquisition and transfer of the control of variables strategy. Child Development, 70, 1098–120. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467–8624.00081Google Scholar
Chin, J., Payne, B. R., Fu, W.-T., Morrow, D. G., and Stine-Morrow, E. A. (2015). Information foraging across the life span: Search and switch in unknown patches. Topics in Cognitive Science, 7, 428–50. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12147CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chouinard, M. M. (2007). Children’s questions: A mechanism for cognitive development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 72, i129.Google Scholar
Coenen, A., Ruggeri, A., Bramley, N. R., and Gureckis, T. M. (2019). Testing one or multiple: How beliefs about sparsity affect causal experimentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition. Advance online publication. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000680Google Scholar
Corriveau, K. H., Fusaro, M., and Harris, P. L. (2009). Going with the flow: Preschoolers prefer nondissenters as informants. Psychological Science, 20, 372–7. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–9280.2009.02291.xGoogle Scholar
Courage, M. L. (1989). Children’s inquiry strategies in referential communication and in the game of twenty questions. Child Development, 60, 877–86. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131029Google Scholar
Davenport, J. L., Yaron, D., Klahr, D., and Koedinger, K. (2008). When do diagrams enhance learning? A framework for designing relevant representations. In Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on the Learning Sciences (pp. 191–8). International Society of the Learning Sciences.Google Scholar
Davidson, D. (1991a). Children’s decision-making examined with an information-board procedure. Cognitive Development, 6, 7790. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885–2014(91)90007–zGoogle Scholar
Davidson, D. (1991b). Developmental differences in children’s search of predecisional information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 52, 239–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022–0965(91)90061–vGoogle Scholar
Davidson, D. (1996). The effects of decision characteristics on children’s selective search of predecisional information. Acta Psychologica, 92, 263–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001–6918(95)00014–3Google Scholar
Dean, D., and Kuhn, D. (2007). Direct instruction vs. discovery: The long view. Science Education, 91, 3674. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.20194Google Scholar
Denison, S., and Xu, F. (2010a). Integrating physical constraints in statistical inference by 11-month-old infants. Cognitive Science, 34, 885908. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551–6709.2010.01111.xGoogle Scholar
Denison, S., and Xu, F. (2010b). Twelve- to 14-month-old infants can predict single-event probability with large set sizes. Developmental Science, 13, 798803. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–7687.2009.00943.xGoogle Scholar
Denison, S., and Xu, F. (2014). The origins of probabilistic inference in human infants. Cognition, 130, 335–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.12.001Google Scholar
Denison, S., Reed, C., and Xu, F. (2013). The emergence of probabilistic reasoning in very young infants: Evidence from 4.5- and 6-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 49, 243–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028278Google Scholar
Denney, D. (1972). Modeling and eliciting effects upon conceptual strategies. Child Development, 43, 810–23. https://doi.org/10.2307/1127633Google Scholar
Denney, D., Denney, N., and Ziobrowski, M. (1973). Alterations in the information-processing strategies of young children following observation of adult models. Developmental Psychology, 8, 202–8. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0034144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denney, N., and Turner, M. (1979). Facilitating cognitive performance in children: A comparison of strategy modeling and strategy modeling with overt self-verbalization. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 28, 119–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022–0965(79)90106–1Google Scholar
Durham, R. E., Farkas, G., Scheffner Hammer, C., Tomblin, J. B., and Catts, H. W. (2007). Kindergarten oral language skill: A key variable in the intergenerational transmission of socioeconomic status. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility, 25, 294305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rssm.2007.03.001Google Scholar
Eimas, P. D. (1970). Information processing in problem solving as a function of developmental level and stimulus saliency. Developmental Psychology, 2, 224–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0028746Google Scholar
Frazier, B. N., Gelman, S. A., and Wellman, H. M. (2009). Preschoolers’ search for explanatory information within adult–child conversation. Child Development, 80, 1592–611. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.2009.01356.xGoogle Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., and the ABC Research Group (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., and Wellman, H. M. (1994). The theory theory. In Hirschfeld, L. A. and Gelman, S. A. (eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 257–93). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gottfried, A. W., Gottfried, A. E., Bathurst, K., Guerin, D. W., and Parramore, M. M. (2003). Socioeconomic status in children’s development and family environment: Infancy through adolescence. In Bornstein, M. H. and Bradley, R. H. (eds.), Socioeconomic status, parenting and child development (pp. 189207). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Green, L., and Mehr, D. R. (1997). What alters physicians’ decisions to admit to the coronary care unit? Journal of Family Practice, 45, 219–26.Google Scholar
Greif, M. L., Kemler Nelson, D. G., Keil, F. C., and Gutierrez, F. (2006). What do children want to know about animals and artifacts? Domain-specific requests for information. Psychological Science, 17, 455–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–9280.2006.01727.xGoogle Scholar
Gruber, M. J., Watrous, A. J., Ekstrom, A. D., Ranganath, C., and Otten, L. J. (2013). Expected reward modulates encoding-related theta activity before an event. Neuroimage, 64, 6874. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.07.064Google Scholar
Guderian, S., Schott, B., Richardson-Klavehn, A., and Düzel, E. (2009). Medial temporal theta state before an event predicts episodic encoding success in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 5365–70. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0900289106Google Scholar
Gweon, H., Tenenbaum, J. B., and Schulz, L. E. (2010). Infants consider both the sample and the sampling process in inductive generalization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 9066–71. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1003095107Google Scholar
Hamilton, G. C., Sanders, A., Strange, G. S., and Trott, A. T. (2003). Emergency medicine: An approach to clinical problem solving (2nd ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Saunders.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. (2012). Trusting what you’re told: How children learn from others. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L., and Corriveau, K. H. (2011). Young children’s selective trust in informants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 1179–87. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:osobl/9780199608966.003.0025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herwig, J. E. (1982). Effects of age, stimuli, and category recognition factors in children’s inquiry behavior. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 196206. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022–0965(82)90015–7Google Scholar
Hickling, A. K., and Wellman, H. M. (2001). The emergence of children’s causal explanations and theories: Evidence from everyday conversation. Developmental Psychology, 37, 668–83. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012–1649.37.5.668Google Scholar
Hirsch-Pasek, K., Zosh, J. M., Michnick Golinkoff, R., et al. (2015). Putting education in “educational” apps: Lessons from the science of learning. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 16, 334. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100615569721Google Scholar
Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. V., and van der Molen, M. W. (2006). Age-related change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2017–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.010Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., and Beatty, J. (1966). Pupil diameter and load on memory. Science, 154, 1583–5. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.154.3756.1583Google Scholar
Klahr, D., Fay, A. L., and Dunbar, K. (1993). Heuristics for scientific experimentation: A developmental study. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 111–46. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1993.1003Google Scholar
Klahr, D., Zimmerman, C., and Jirout, J. (2011). Educational interventions to advance children’s scientific thinking. Science, 333, 971–75. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1204528Google Scholar
Koenig, M. A., and Harris, P. L. (2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development, 76, 1261–77. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.2005.00849.xGoogle Scholar
Koriat, A., and Ackerman, R. (2010). Choice latency as a cue for children’s subjective confidence in the correctness of their answers. Developmental Science, 13, 441–53. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–7687.2009.00907.xGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D., and Angelev, J. (1976). An experimental study of the development of formal operational thought. Child Development, 47, 697706. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128184Google Scholar
Kuhn, D., and Brannock, J. (1977). Development of the isolation of variables scheme in experimental and “natural experiment” contexts. Developmental Psychology, 13, 914. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012–1649.13.1.9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D., and Phelps, E. (1982). The development of problem-solving strategies. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 17, 144.Google Scholar
Kuhn, D., Garcia-Mila, M., Zohar, A., et al. (1995). Strategies of knowledge acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60, i157.Google Scholar
Kurkul, K. E., and Corriveau, K. H. (2018). Question, explanation, follow-up: A mechanism for learning from others? Child Development, 89, 208–94. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12726Google Scholar
Kushnir, T., and Gopnik, A. (2005). Young children infer causal strength from probabilities and interventions. Psychological Science, 16, 678–83. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–9280.2005.01595.xGoogle Scholar
Legare, C. H., Mills, C. M., Souza, A. L., Plummer, L. E., and Yasskin, R. (2013). The use of questions as problem-solving strategies during early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 6376. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.002Google Scholar
Liaw, F. R., and Brooks-Gunn, J. (1994). Cumulative familial risks and low-birthweight children’s cognitive and behavioral development. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 23, 360–72. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp2304_2Google Scholar
Lindley, D. V. (1956). On a measure of the information provided by an experiment. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 27, 9861005. https://doi.org/10.1214/aoms/1177728069Google Scholar
Lyons, K., and Ghetti, S. (2011). The development of uncertainty monitoring in early childhood. Child Development, 82, 1778–87. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.2011.01649.xGoogle Scholar
Lyons, K., and Ghetti, S. (2013). I don’t want to pick! Introspection on uncertainty supports early strategic behavior. Child Development, 84, 726–36. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12004Google Scholar
Martignon, L., Katsikopoulos, K. V., and Woike, J. K. (2008). Categorization with limited resources: A family of simple heuristics. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 52, 352–61. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199744282.003.0014Google Scholar
Mata, R., von Helversen, B., and Rieskamp, J. (2011). When easy comes hard: The development of adaptive strategy selection. Child Development, 82, 687700. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.2010.01535.xGoogle Scholar
Matlen, B. J., and Klahr, D. (2012). Sequential effects of high and low instructional guidance on children’s acquisition of experimentation skills: Is it all in the timing? Instructional Science, 41, 621–34. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-012–9248–zGoogle Scholar
Mills, C. M., Legare, C. H., Bills, M., and Mejias, C. (2010). Preschoolers use questions as a tool to acquire knowledge from different sources. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11, 533–60. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2010.516419Google Scholar
Mills, C. M., Legare, C. H., Grant, M. G., and Landrum, A. R. (2011). Determining who to question, what to ask, and how much information to ask for: The development of inquiry in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 539–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.06.003Google Scholar
Mosher, F., and Hornsby, J. (1966). On asking questions. In Bruner, J., Oliver, R., and Greenfield, P. (eds.), Studies in cognitive growth. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
National Research Council (2012). A framework for K-12 science education: Practices, crosscutting concepts, and core ideas. Washington, DC: The National Academies.Google Scholar
Nelson, J. D. (2005). Finding useful questions: On Bayesian diagnosticity, probability, impact, and information gain. Psychological Review, 112, 979–99. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.112.4.979Google Scholar
Nelson, J. D., McKenzie, C. R., Cottrell, G. W., and Sejnowski, T. J. (2010). Experience matters: Information acquisition optimizes probability gain. Psychological Science, 21, 960–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610372637Google Scholar
Nelson, J. D., Divjak, B., Gudmundsdottir, G., Martignon, L. F., and Meder, B. (2014). Children’s sequential information search is sensitive to environmental probabilities. Cognition, 130, 7480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.09.007Google Scholar
Noble, K. G., Norman, M. F., and Farah, M. J. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of SES in kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 8, 7887. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–7687.2005.00394.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oaksford, M., and Chater, N. (1994). A rational analysis of the selection task as optimal data selection. Psychological Review, 101, 608–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.101.4.608Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1954). Language and thought from a genetic perspective. Acta Psychologica, 10, 5160.Google Scholar
Romine, C. B., and Reynolds, C. R. (2005). A model of the development of frontal lobe function: Findings from a meta-analysis. Applied Neuropsychology, 12, 190201. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an1204_2Google Scholar
Rowe, M. (2008). Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35, 185205. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000907008343Google Scholar
Ruggeri, A., and Feufel, M. (2015). How basic-level objects facilitate question-asking in a categorization task. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 113. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00918Google Scholar
Ruggeri, A., and Lombrozo, T. (2015). Children adapt their questions to achieve efficient search. Cognition, 143, 203–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.004Google Scholar
Ruggeri, A., Lombrozo, T., Griffiths, T. L., and Xu, F. (2016). Sources of developmental change in the efficiency of information search. Developmental Psychology, 52, 2159–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000240Google Scholar
Ruggeri, A., Sim, Z. L., and Xu, F. (2017). “Why is Toma late to school again?” Preschoolers identify the most informative questions. Developmental Psychology, 53, 1620–32. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000340Google Scholar
Ruggeri, A., Markant, D., Gureckis, T. D., and Xu, F. (2019a). Memory enhancements from active control of learning emerge across development. Cognition, 186, 8294.Google Scholar
Ruggeri, A., Xu, F., and Lombrozo, T. (2019b). Effects of explanation on children’s question asking. Cognition, 191, 103966.Google Scholar
Schneider, M., and Siegler, R. S. (2010) Representations of the magnitudes of fractions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1227–38. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018170Google Scholar
Schwichow, M., Croker, C., Zimmerman, C., Höffler, T., and Härtig., H. (2016). Teaching the control-of-variables strategy: A meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 39, 3763. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.12.001Google Scholar
Siler, S. A., Klahr, D., and Price, N. (2012). Investigating the mechanisms of learning from a constrained preparation for future learning activity. Instructional Science, 41, 191216. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-012–9224–7Google Scholar
Smith, J., Brooks-Gunn, J., and Klebanov, P. (1997). Consequences of living in poverty for young children’s cognitive and verbal ability and early school achievement. In Duncan, G. and Brooks-Gunn, J. (eds.), Consequences of growing up poor (pp. 132–89). New York: Russell Sage.Google Scholar
Steyvers, M., Tenenbaum, J. B., Wagenmakers, E.-J., and Blum, B. (2003). Inferring causal networks from observations and interventions. Cognitive Science, 27, 453–89. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2703_6Google Scholar
Sylva, K. (1994). School influences on children’s development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 135–70. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469–7610.1994.tb01135.xGoogle Scholar
Téglás, E., Girotto, V., Gonzalez, M., and Bonatti, L. L. (2007). Intuitions of probabilities shape expectations about the future at 12 months and beyond. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 19156–9. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0700271104Google ScholarPubMed
Téglás, E., Vul, E., Girotto, V., et al. (2011). Pure reasoning in 12-month-old infants as probabilistic inference. Science, 332, 1054–9. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1196404Google Scholar
Todd, P. M., Gigerenzer, G., and the ABC Research Group (2012). Ecological rationality: Intelligence in the world. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Walker, D., Greenwood, B., Hart, B., and Carta, J. (1994). Prediction of school outcomes based on early language production and socioeconomic factors. Child Development, 65, 606–21. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131404Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M. (2011). Reinvigorating explanations for the study of early cognitive development. Child Development Perspectives, 5, 33–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750–8606.2010.00154.xGoogle Scholar
Wierda, S. M., van Rijn, H., Taatgen, N. A., and Martens, S. (2012). Pupil dilation deconvolution reveals the dynamics of attention at high temporal resolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 8456–60. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1201858109Google Scholar
Xu, F., and Denison, S. (2009). Statistical inference and sensitivity to sampling in 11-month-old infants. Cognition, 112, 97104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.04.006Google Scholar
Xu, F., and Garcia, V. (2008). Intuitive statistics by 8-month-old infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 5012–15. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0704450105Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×