Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-9q27g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-20T07:59:38.724Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - The Role of Working Memory in Problem Solving

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

David Z. Hambrick
Affiliation:
Michigan State University
Randall W. Engle
Affiliation:
Georgia Institute of Technology
Janet E. Davidson
Affiliation:
Lewis and Clark College, Portland
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

The combination of moment-to-moment awareness and instant retrieval of archived information constitutes what is called the working memory, perhaps the most significant achievement of human mental evolution.

(Goldman-Rakic, 1992, p. 111)

Working memory plays an essential role in complex cognition. Everyday cognitive tasks – such as reading a newspaper article, calculating the appropriate amount to tip in a restaurant, mentally rearranging furniture in one's living room to create space for a new sofa, and comparing and contrasting various attributes of different apartments to decide which to rent – often involve multiple steps with intermediate results that need to be kept in mind temporarily to accomplish the task at hand successfully.

(Shah & Miyake, 1999, p. 1)

More than 25 years ago, Baddeley and Hitch (1974) lamented, “Despite more than a decade of intensive research on the topic of short-term memory (STM), we still know virtually nothing about its role in normal information processing” (p. 47). The primary concern for Baddeley and Hitch was the presumed centrality of limited-capacity short-term memory in contemporary models of memory, including Atkinson and Shiffrin's (1968) “modal model.” For example, Baddeley and Hitch described a patient with brain-damage (K.F.) who exhibited grossly deficient performance on tests of short-term memory but normal performance on long-term learning tasks. Logically, this could not occur if information passes from short-term memory to long-term memory.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, P. L., & Kyllonen, P. C. (1991). Trainee characteristics. In J. E. Morrison (Ed.), Training for performance: Principles of applied human learning (pp. 193–229). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons
Adams, J. W., & Hitch, G. J. (1997). Working memory and children's mental addition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 67, 21–38CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allard, F., & Starkes, J. L. (1991). Motor-skill experts in sports, dance, and other domains. In K. A. Ericsson & J. Smith (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 126–152). New York: Cambridge University Press
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Anderson, J. R. (1985). Cognitive psychology and its implications. New York: Freeman
Anderson, J. R. (1993). Problem solving and learning. American Psychologist, 48, 35–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), Advances in the psychology of learning and motivation research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 89–195). New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Babcock, R. L. (1994). Analysis of adult age differences on the Raven's Progressive Matrices Test. Psychology and Aging, 9, 303–314CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Elridge, M., & Lewis, V. (1981). The role of subvocalisation in reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 33A, 439–454CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Baddeley, A. D., Vallar, G., & Wilson, B. (1987). Sentence comprehension and phonological memory: Some neuropsychological evidence. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance VII: The psychology of reading (pp. 509–529). Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum
Barfield, W. (1997). Skilled performance on software as a function of domain expertise and program organization. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 85, 1471-1480CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, S. L., Kraft, R. G., Glover, J. A., & Plake, B. S. (1984). Cognitive capacity differences among writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 820–834CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bors, D. A., & Forrin, B. (1995). Age, speed of information processing, recall, and fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 20, 229–248CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Britton, B. K., Stimson, M., Stennett, B., & Gülgöz, S. (1998). Learning from instruction text: Test of an individual-differences model. Journal of Education Psychology, 90, 476–491CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1943). The measurement of adult intelligence. Psychological Bulletin, 40, 153–193CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charness, N. (1981). Search in chess: Age and skill differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 7, 467–476Google Scholar
Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55–81CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cherry, C. E. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975–979CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., & Bunting, M. F. (2001). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 331–335CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Green, I. (1986). Individual difference in comprehending and producing words in context. Journal of Memory & Language, 25, 1–18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Merikle, P. M. (1996). Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 422–433CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donders (1868/1969). Over de snelheid van psychische processen. Onderzoekingen gedann in het Psysiologish Laboratorium der Utrechtsche Hoogeschool (W. G. Koster, Trans.). In W. G. Koster (Ed.), Attention and Performance II. Acta Psychologica, 30, 412–431. (Original work published in 1868.)
Duncker, K. (1945). On problem-solving. Psychological Monographs, 58 (Whole No. 270)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engle, R. W., Cantor, J., & Carullo, J. J. (1992). Individual differences in working memory and comprehension: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 18, 972–992Google ScholarPubMed
Engle, R. W., Carullo, J. J., & Collins, K. W. (1991). Individual differences in working memory for comprehension and following directions. Journal of Education Research, 84, 253–262CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J., & Tuholski, S. W. (1999). Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 102–134). New York: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. St. B., & Brooks, P. G. (1981). Competing with reasoning: A test of the working memory hypothesis. Current Psychological Research, 1, 139–147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feigenbaum, E. A. (1989). What hath Simon wrought? In D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky (Eds.), Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 165–180). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Frensch, P. A., & Sternberg, R. J. (1989a). Expertise and intelligent thinking: When is it worse to know better? In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 5, pp. 157–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Frensch, P. A., & Sternberg, R. J. (1989b). Skill-related differences in game playing. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms (pp. 343–381). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Gilhooly, K. J., Logie, R. H., Wetherick, N. E., & Wynn, V. (1993). Working memory and strategies in syllogistic-reasoning tasks. Memory & Cognition, 21, 115–124CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilhooly, K. J., Wood, M., Kinnear, P. R., & Green, C. (1988). Skill in map reading and memory for maps. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 40, 87–107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glass, A. L., Eddy, J. K., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (1980). The verification of high and low imagery sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 6, 692–704Google Scholar
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1992). Working memory and the mind. Scientific American, 267, 110–117CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haenggi, D., & Perfetti, C. A. (1992). Individual differences in reprocessing of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 182–192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haenggi, D., & Perfetti, C. A. (1994). Processing components of college-level reading comprehension. Discourse Processes, 17, 83–104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halford, G. S., Bain, J. D., & Mayberry, M. T. (1984). Does a concurrent memory load interfere with reasoning? Current Psychological Research and Reviews, 3, 14–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hambrick, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2002). Effects of domain knowledge, working memory capacity, and age on cognitive performance. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 339–387CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasher, L., Quig, M. B., & May, C. P. (1997). Inhibitory control over no-longer-relevant information: Adult age differences. Memory and Cognition, 25, 286–295CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 22, pp. 193–225). San Diego, CA: Academic PressCrossRef
Hasher, L., Zacks, & May, C. P. (1999). Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age. In D. Gopher & A. Koriat (Eds.), Attention and performance XVII. Cognitive regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and application (pp. 653–675). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley
Heglin, H. J. (1956). Problem solving set in different age groups. Journal of Gerontology, 38, 71–76Google Scholar
Horn, J. L., & Masunaga, H. (2000). New directions for research into aging and intelligence: The development of expertise. In T. J. Perfect & E. A. Maylor (Eds.), Models of cognitive aging (pp. 125–159). Oxford: Oxford University Press
Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., & Dixon, R. A. (1990). Ability correlates of memory performance in adulthood and aging. Psychology and Aging, 5, 356–368CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall
Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R. A., & Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled attention view of working memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 169–183CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Kintsch, W., Healy, A. F., Hegarty, M., Pennington, B. F., & Salthouse, T. A. (1999). Models of working memory: Eight questions and some general issues. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 412–441). Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Kintsch, W., & Dijk, T. A. (1978). Towards a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363–394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klauer, K. C., Stegmaier, R., & Meiser, T. (1997). Working memory involvement in propositional and spatial reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning, 3, 9–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyllonen, P. C. (1996). Is working memory capacity Spearman's g? In I. Dennis & P. Tapsfield (Eds.), Human abilities: Their nature and measurement (pp. 49–75). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc
Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity? Intelligence, 14, 389–433CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuo-spatial working memory. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum
Luchins, A. S. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 54 (Whole No. 248)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masson, M. E., & Miller, Jo A. (1983). Working memory and individual differences in comprehension and memory of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 314–318CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meinz, E. J., & Salthouse, T. A. (1998). The effects of age and experience on memory for visually presented music. Journals of Gerontology, 53B, 60–69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, K. M. (1957). Einstellung rigidity, intelligence and teaching methods. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 27, 127–134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minsky, M. L., & Papert, S. (1974). Artificial intelligence. Eugene, OR: State System of Higher Education
Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11, 56–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ormrod, J. E., & Cochran, K. F. (1988). Relationship of verbal ability and working memory to spelling achievement and learning to spell. Reading Research & Instruction, 28, 33–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robbins, T. W., Anderson, E. J., Barker, D. R., Bradley, A. C., Fearnyhough, C., Henson, R., Hudson, S. R., & Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Working memory in chess. Memory & Cognition, 24, 83–93CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, J. C., Keyes, B. J., & Fuller, B. J. (1976). Solution shift in the elderly. Journal of Gerontology, 31, 670–675CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosen, V. M., & Engle, R. W. (1997). The role of working memory capacity in retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 211–227CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosen, V. M., & Engle, R. W. (1998). Working memory capacity and suppression. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 418–436CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1992). Mechanisms of age-cognition relations in adulthood. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shah, P., & Miyake, A. (1996). The separability of working memory resources for spatial thinking and language processing: An individual differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 4–27CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shah, P., & Miyake, A. (1999). Models of working memory: An introduction. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 1–27). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Stine, E. A. L., & Wingfield, A. (1990). How much do working memory deficits contribute to age differences in discourse memory? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology Special Issue: Cognitive Gerontology, 2, 289–304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toms, M., Morris, N., & Ward, D. (1993). Working memory and conditional reasoning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology Special Issue: The cognitive psychology of reasoning, 46A, 679–699CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory & Language, 28, 127–154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voss, J. F., Greene, T. R., Post, T. A., & Penner, B. C. (1983). Problem-solving skill in the social sciences. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research theory (Vol. 17, pp. 165–213). New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Waters, G., Caplan, D., & Hildebrandt, N. (1987). Working memory and written sentence comprehension. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance 12: The psychology of reading. Hove, England: Lawrence Erlbaum
Welsh, M. C., Satterlee-Cartmell, T., & Stine, M. (1999). Towers of Hanoi and London: Contribution of working memory and inhibition to performance. Brain & Cognition, 41, 231–242CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittmann, W. W., & Süβ, H. (1999). Investigating the paths between working memory, intelligence, and complex problem-solving performances via Brunswik Symmetry. In P. L. Ackerman, P. C. Kyllonen, & R. D. Roberts (Eds.), Learning and individual differences (pp. 77–110). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
Zacks, R. T., Hasher, L., & Li, K. Z. H. (2000). Human memory. In T. A. Salthouse & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 293–357). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×