Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-9pm4c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T09:10:02.824Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Finding common ground to promote dialogue and collaboration: using case material to jointly observe children's behavior

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Jane Holmes Bernstein
Affiliation:
Senior Associate in Psychology/Neuropsychology Children's Hospital Boston
Kurt W. Fischer
Affiliation:
Harvard University, Massachusetts
Jane Holmes Bernstein
Affiliation:
The Children's Hospital, Boston
Mary Helen Immordino-Yang
Affiliation:
University of Southern California
Get access

Summary

Overview: The new era of learning disorders research and practice is marked by cross-disciplinary discussion and collaborative thinking about children's development across mind, brain, and education. A major issue in this effort is the practical question of where to begin discussion, so that researchers from psychology, neuroscience, cognitive science, and education can communicate across their perspectives and settings, ranging across the experimental laboratory, the hospital, the special-needs classroom, and many others. Bernstein suggests that a fruitful approach to collaboration is to step back from disciplinary perspectives and reframe the discussion in terms of the whole child in a real context who is actively working to adapt to the world with his or her skills and limitations. A helpful tool for promoting collaborative insight is to share and interpret common observations, as with the chapters in this section: each author addresses the behaviors of four boys performing various activities related to reading. In her own observations, Bernstein asserts that understanding each child's performance requires analyzing not just the child as observed (looking at the child) but the child as an interactive partner in context (being with the child). Each child acts in a social relationship with the interviewer or teacher and responds cognitively and emotionally to the activities, the physical surroundings, and the emotional climate of the situation, adapting his or her skills and limitations to cope.

The Editors

What is a good way to promote dialogue among researchers and practitioners from different perspectives in order to improve understanding of learning disorders, especially with respect to their brain bases? Work on learning disorders requires input from multiple disciplines, but despite this need, interdisciplinary cooperation on learning disorders and their brain bases has not been notably successful until relatively recently.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bakker, D. (1984). The brain as a dependent variable. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 6, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, J. H. (2000). Developmental neuropsychological assessment. In Yeates, K. O., Ris, M. D. & Taylor, H. G. (eds), Pediatric neuropsychology: Theory, research and practice. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Bernstein, J. H. & Weiler, M. D. (2000). “Pediatric neuropsychological assessment” examined. In Goldstein, G. & Hersen, M. (eds), Handbook of psychological assessment 3rd edn. New York: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Buller, D. J. & Hardcastle, V. G. (2000). Evolutionary psychology, meet developmental neurobiology: Against promiscuous modularity. Brain and Mind, 1, 307–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denckla, M. B. & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid automatized naming (R. A. N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, K. W., Ayoub, C. C., Noam, G. G., Singh, I., Maraganore, A. & Raya, P. (1997). Psychopathology as adaptive development along distinctive pathways. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 751–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, K. W., Rotenberg, E. J., Bullock, D. H. & Raya, P. (1993). The dynamics of competence: How context contributes directly to skill. In Wozniak, R. H. & Fischer, K. W. (eds). Development in context: Acting and thinking in specific environments, 93–117. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ginsburg, H. P. (1997). Entering the child's mind: The clinical interview in psychological research and practice. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Globus, G. G. (1973). Consciousness and brain, I. The identity thesis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 29, 153–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harter, S. (2000). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Holmes Bernstein, J. & Waber, D. P. (1990). Developmental neuropsychological assessment: The systemic approach. In Boulton, A. A., Baker, G. B. & Hiscock, M. (eds), Neuromethods: Volume 17, Neuropsychology. Clifton, NJ: Humana Press.Google Scholar
Matarazzo, J. D. (1990). Psychological assessment versus psychological testing. American Psychologist, 45, 999–1017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quartz, S. R. & Sejnowski, T. J. (1997). The neural basis of cognitive development: A constructivist manifesto. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 537–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test de copie d'une figure complexe. Archives de Psychologie, 30, 206–356.Google Scholar
Rey, A. (1941). L'examen psychologique dans le cas d'encéphalopathie traumatique. Archives de Psychologie, 28, 286–340.Google Scholar
Reynolds, C. R. & Kamphaus, R. W. (1992). Behavior assessment system for children. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Siegler, R. S. (1988). Strategy choice procedures and the development of multiplication skill, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 258–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegler, R. S. (1996). Emerging minds. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sorensen, L. G., Forbes, P. W., Bernstein, J. H., Weiler, M. D., Mitchell, W. M. & Waber, D. P. (2003). Psychosocial adjustment over a two-year period in children referred for learning problems: Risk, resilience, and adaptation. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 18(1), 10–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and garden-variety poor reader: the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21, 590–612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torgesen, J. K. & Mathes, P. (2000). A basic guide to understanding, assessing, and teaching phonological awareness. Austin, TX: PRO-ED Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
Vanderploeg, R. D. (1994). Interview and testing: the data-collection phase of neuro psychological evaluations. In Vanderploeg, R. D. (ed.) Clinician's guide to neuro psychological assessment. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Waber, D. P., Wolff, P. H., Weiler, M. D., Bellinger, D., Marcus, D. H., Forbes, P. & Wypij, D. (2001). Processing of rapid auditory stimuli in school-age children referred for evaluation of learning disorder. Child Development, 72, 37–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K. & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processes. Austin, TX: PRO-ED Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
Wakefield, J. C. (1992). The concept of mental disorder: On the boundary between biological facts and social values. American Psychologist, 47, 373–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiderholt, J. L. & Bryant, B. R. (1992). Gray oral reading tests, 3rd edn. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.Google Scholar
Wolf, M. & Katzir-Cohen, T. (2001). Reading fluency and its intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 211–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolff, P. H., Michel, G. F., Ovrut, M. & Drake, C. (1990). Rate and timing precision of motor coordination in developmental dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 26, 349–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×