Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T22:58:55.869Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

18 - Teachers' models of children's minds and learning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Lawrence A. Hirschfeld
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Susan A. Gelman
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Get access

Summary

Laypersons, teachers, and psychologists believe that learning takes place in people's minds. They all believe that learning takes place as a consequence of instruction, if we interpret instruction in its widest sense (Atran & Sperber, 1991).

Western laypersons have folk psychology notions of the mind and its functioning. But their notions are not all that naive because Western adults have considerable practice at learning and, even more important, thinking about learning. Entry into school, the culturally designated place for learning to take place, generally begins at age 6. Thus students who have finished their high school education have been in formal learning situations for 12 years. Over a considerable period of time, then, they have practiced what they and others think it takes to learn. In the sense that they have not studied formally about the mind and learning in, say, psychology courses, they can be considered laypersons. But it is a big stretch to call them laypersons and holders of a naive psychology after they have been in places where learning is the main goal and where they have been reflective about what that learning is.

Teachers are professionals who have been educated to teach in such a way as to cause change in others’ minds. That change is learning. The cause of that change is thought to be teaching. The differentiation between teaching and learning is often unclear, even in the educational literature. Some of our best friends refer to it as the teaching/learning process.

Type
Chapter
Information
Mapping the Mind
Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture
, pp. 455 - 473
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×