Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-68ccn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-11T09:16:46.575Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

14 - Anaphors and logophors: an argument structure perspective

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 June 2011

Tanya Reinhart
Affiliation:
Tel Aviv University
Eric Reuland
Affiliation:
University of Groningen
Jan Koster
Affiliation:
Rijksuniversiteit Groningen, The Netherlands
Get access

Summary

Introduction

It has been known for quite some time that the binding theory developed in Chomsky (1981) and subsequent work does not account for the full range of binding facts (cf. Maling (1982), Giorgi (1984), Chomsky (1986a), Everaert (1986a), and others): The scope of conditions A and B is limited to binding within the domain of the first accessible subject, the local domain. Therefore, non-clausebounded reflexives, which are commonly referred to as long-distance (LD-) anaphors in languages as diverse as Icelandic, Finnish, Polish, Latin, Chinese, Japanese, etc., cannot be captured (see the various contributions in this volume for discussion and references). Even within the domain to which conditions A and B apply, language does not fully live up to their predictions. In a number of environments pronouns can be found where condition B excludes them. Well-known examples are English sentences of the type John saw the snake near him, reflexive verbs in Dutch and French which allow locally bound first and second person pronouns in object position, and Frisian, where this latitude also obtains with third person object pronouns (see Everaert (chapter 4), and Bouchard (1984)).

For years, a unified theory of the various anaphors across languages seemed somewhat unfeasible, in view of the massive differences reported concerning their distribution, particularly in the case of LD-anaphors. A major breakthrough, however, has been the discovery that a distinction is needed between logophoric processes and structural binding relations.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×