Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-9q27g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-22T07:26:06.482Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 October 2014

Vyvyan Evans
Affiliation:
Bangor University
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
The Language Myth
Why Language Is Not an Instinct
, pp. 274 - 300
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2014

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aikhenvald, A., and Dixon, R.. (2013). Possession and Ownership: A Cross-linguistic Typology. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Alonso-Nanclares, L., Gonzalez-Soriano, J., Rodriguez, J. R. and DeFelipe, J.. (2008). Gender differences in human cortical synaptic density. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 105/38: 14615–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ambridge, B., and Lieven, E. V. M.. (2011). Child Language Acquisition: Contrasting Theoretical Approaches. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antunes, R., Schulz, T., Gero, S., Whitehead, H., Gordon, J. and Rendell, L.. (2011). Individually distinctive acoustic features in sperm whale codas. Animal Behaviour 81/4: 723–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (2013). Précis of how the brain got language. Language and Cognition, 5/2–3: 107–32, plus comments 133–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (2012). How the Brain Got Language: The Mirror System Hypothesis. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (2010). Mirror system activity for action and language is embedded in the integration of dorsal and ventral pathways. Brain and Language, 112/1: 12–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arbib, M. A., Liebal, K. and Pika, S.. (2008). Primate vocalization, gesture, and the evolution of human language. Current Anthropology, 49/6: 1053–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnold, K., and Zuberbühler, K.. (2006). The alarm-calling system of adult male putty nosed monkeys, Cercopithecus nictitans martini. Animal Behavior, 72: 643–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Austin, P., and Bresnan, J.. (1996). Non-configurationality in Australian Aboriginal languages. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 14: 215–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Azevedo, F., Carvalho, L., Grinberg, L., Farfel, J., Ferretti, R., Leite, R., Filho, W., Lent, R. et al. (2009). Equal numbers of neuronal and nonneuronal cells make the human brain an isometrically scaled-up primate brain. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 513/5: 532–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barney, A., Martelli, S., Serrurier, A. and Steele, J.. (2012). Articulatory capacity of Neanderthals, a very recent and human-like fossil hominin. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367/1585: 88–102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E., Dale, P. and Thal, D.. (1995). Individual differences and their implications for theories of language development. In Fletcher, P. and MacWhinney, B. (eds.), Handbook of Child Language (pp. 96–151). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Elman, J., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D. and Plunkett, K.. (1999). Innateness and emergentism. In Bechtel, W. and Graham, G. (eds.), A Companion to Cognitive Science (pp. 590–601). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bates, E., and Goodman, J.. (1999). On the emergence of grammar from the lexicon. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), The Emergence of Language (pp. 29–79). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bates, E., and Goodman, J.. (1997). On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: evidence from acquisition, aphasia and real-time processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12/5–6: 507–84.Google Scholar
Bates, E., and MacWhinney, B.. (1989). Functionalism and the competition model. In MacWhinney, B. and Bates, E. (eds.), The Crosslinguistic Study of Sentence Processing (pp. 3–73). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., MacWhinney, B., Caselli, C., Devescovi, A., Natale, F. and Venza, V.. (1984). A cross-linguistic study of the development of sentence interpretation strategies. Child Development, 55: 341–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E., Thal, D., Trauner, D., Aram, D., Eisele, J. and Nass, R.. (1997). From first words to grammar in children with focal brain injury. In D. Thal and J. Reilly (eds.), Special issue on the origins of communication disorders. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13/3: 275–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellugi, U., Bihrle, A., Neville, H., Jernigan, T. and Doherty, S.. (1992). Language, cognition, and brain organization in a neurodevelopmental disorder. In Gunnar, M. and Nelson, C. (eds.), Developmental Behavioral Neuroscience (pp. 201–32). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U., Wang, P. and Jernigan, T. L.. (1994). Williams syndrome: an unusual neuropsychological profile. In Broman, S. and Grafman, J. (eds.,), Atypical Cognitive Deficits in Developmental Disorders: Implications for Brain Function (pp. 23–56). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berlin, B., and Kay, P.. (1969). Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Berwick, R. C., Pietroski, P., Yankama, B. and Chomsky, N.. (2011). Poverty of the stimulus revisited. Cognitive Science, 35: 1207–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bickerton, D. (1984). The language bioprogram hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7: 173–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D. (1981). Roots of Language. Ann Arbor, MI: Karoma Publishers.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. (2006). What causes specific language impairment in children?Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15/5: 217–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. (1997). Uncommon Understanding: Development and Disorders of Language Comprehension in Children. Hove: Psychology Press Limited.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). One Word at a Time. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bloom, P., and Keil, F.. (2001). Thinking through language. Mind and Language, 6: 351–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolinger, D. (1980). Language: the Loaded Weapon. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Bond, A. B., Kamil, A. C. and Balda, R. P.. (2003). Social complexity and transitive inference in corvids. Animal Behaviour, 65: 479–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Börjars, K., Denison, D. and Scott, A.. (2013). Morphosyntactic Categories and the Expression of Possession. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Kessen, W. and Weiskopf, S.. (1976). The categories of hue in infancy. Science, 191: 4223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? English and Mandarin speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43/1: 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boroditsky, L., Fuhrman, O. and McCormick, K.. (2011). Do English and Mandarin speakers think about time differently?Cognition, 118/1: 123–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, L., Schmidt, L. and Phillips, W.. (2003). Sex, syntax, and semantics. In Gentner, D. and Goldin-Meadow, S. (eds.,), Language in Mind: Advances in the study of Language and Cognition (pp. 61–80). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bouckaert, R., Lemey, P., Dunn, M., Greenhill, S. J., Alekseyenko, A. V., Drummond, A. J., Gray, R. D., Suchard, M. A. and Atkinson, Q. D. (2012). Mapping the origins and expansion of the Indo-European language family. Science, 337/6097: 957–960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boutonnet, B., Athanasopoulos, P. and Thierry, G.. (2012). Unconscious effects of grammatical gender during object categorisation. Brain Research, 1479: 72–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boutonnet, B., Dering, B., Viñas-Gusach, N. and Thierry, G.. (2013). Seeing objects through the language glass. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25/10: 1702–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, J. K., and Goldberg, A. E.. (2011). Learning what not to say: the role of statistical preemption and categorization in ‘a’-adjective production. Language, 81/1: 1–29.Google Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1963). The ontogeny of English phrase structure: the first phrase. Language, 39: 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brinton, L. J. (2000). The Structure of Modern English. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broca, P. (1877). Sur la circonvolution limbique et la scissure limbique. Bulletins de la Société d’Anthropologie, 12/2: 646–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, P., and Tomasello, M.. (1999). How children constrain their argument structure constructions. Language, 75: 720–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. W. (1976). Reference: n memorial tribute to Eric Lenneberg. Cognition, 4: 125–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. W. (1958). Words and Things. New York: Macmillian.Google Scholar
Brown, R. W. (1957). Linguistic determinism and the part of speech. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55: 1–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, R. W., and Lenneberg, E. H.. (1954). A study in language and cognition. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49: 454–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. (1976). From communication to language – a psychological perspective. Cognition, 3: 255–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruno, R., Merzenich, M. M. and Nudo, R.. (2012). The fantastic plastic brain. Advances in Mind Body Medicine, 26/2: 30–5.Google ScholarPubMed
Buccino, G., Riggio, L., Melli, G., Binofski, F., Gallese, V. and Rizzolatti, G.. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: a combined TMS and behavioral study. Cognitive Brain Research, 24: 355–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Büchel, C., Price, C. and Friston, K.. (1998). A multimodal language region in the ventral visual pathway. Nature, 394: 274–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bybee, J. (2010). Language, Usage and Cognition. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (2006). Frequency of Use and the Organization of Language. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. (2001). Phonology and Language Use. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W., and Whiten, A.. (1988). Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes, and Humans. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Call, J., and Tomasello, M.. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in the Cognitive Sciences, 30: 187–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, L. (2010). Language isolates and their history, or, what’s weird, anyway? Paper delivered at the 36th annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, Berkeley, CA, 7 February.
Capirici, O., Sabbadini, L. and Volterra, V.. (1996). Language development in Williams syndrome: a case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13/7: 1017–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D. (2006). Aphasic deficits in syntactic processing. Cortex, 42/6: 797–804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N. and Tomasello, M.. (1998). Fourteen- to 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21: 315–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2006). The Architecture of the Mind: Massive Modularity and the Flexibility of Thought. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D. (2013). Gesture and language processing. In Pashler, H., Crane, T., Kinsbourne, M., Ferreira, F. and Zemel, R. (eds.), Encyclopedia of the Mind (pp. 372–4). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Casasanto, D. (2008). Who’s afraid of the Big Bad Whorf? Cross-linguistic differences in temporal language and thought. Language Learning, 58/1: 63–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D., and Dijkstra, K.. (2010). Motor action and emotional memory. Cognition, 115/1: 179–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caselli, M. C., Bates, E., Casadio, P., Fenson, L., Fenson, J., Sanderl, L. and Weir, J. (1995). A cross-linguistic study of early lexical development. Cognitive Development, 10: 159–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casson, R.W. (1997). Color shift: evolution of English color terms from brightness to hue. In Hardin, C. L. and Maffi, L. (eds.), Color Categories in Thought and Language (pp. 224–39). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chance, S. A., and Crow, T. J.. (2007). Distinctively human: Cerebral lateralisation and language in Homo sapiens. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 85: 83–100.Google Scholar
Chemero, A. (2009). Radical Embodied Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M.. (1990). How Monkeys See the World: Inside the Mind of Another Species. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2011). Language and the cognitive science revolution(s). Lecture given at Carleton University, 8 April.
Chomsky, N. (2010). Some simple evo-devo theses: how true might they be for language? In Larson, R., Déprez, V. and Yamakido, H. (eds.), The Evolution of Human Language (pp. 54–62). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2009). Cartesian Linguistics: A Chapter in the History of Rational Thought (Third Edition). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2004). Language and mind: current thoughts on ancient problems. Part I & Part II. In Jenkins, Lyle (ed.), Variation and Universals in Biolinguistics (pp. 379–405). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2002). On Nature and Language. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1980a). Rules and Representations. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1980b). On cognitive structures and their development: a reply to Piaget. In Piatelli-Palmarini, M. (ed.), Language and Learning: The Debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky (pp. 35–54). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1975). Reflections on Language. New York: Parthenon.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N., and Lasnik, H.. (1993). The theory of principles and parameters. In Jacobs, J. et al. (eds.), Syntax: An International Handbook of Contemporary Research, Vol. 1 (pp. 506–69). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1995). The Engine of Reason, the Seat of the Soul: A Philosophical Journey into the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. S. (2002). Brain-Wise: Studies in Neurophilosophy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. (2003). From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1997). Being There: Putting Brain, Body and World Together Again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, H. (1996). Using Language. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, N. S., Dally, J. M. and Emery, N. J.. (2007). Social cognition by food-caching corvids: the western scrub-jay as a natural psychologist. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 362: 507–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clayton, N. S., Griffiths, D. P., Emery, N. J. and Dickinson, A.. (2001). Elements of episodic-like memory in animals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 356: 1483–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31: 187–276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crain, S. (1991). Language acquisition in the absence of experience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14: 597–650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crain, S., and Nakayama, M.. (1986). Structure dependence in children’s language. Language, 62: 522–43.Google Scholar
Crawford, T. D. (1982). Defining ‘Basic Color Term’. Anthropological Linguistics, 25: 338–43.Google Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Mundry, R. and Zuberbühler, K.. (2011). Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology, 22: 142–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Croft, W. (2000). Explaining Language Change. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Crystal, D. (2002). Language Death. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, E. (2004). Language, Mind and Brain: Some Psychological and Neurological Constraints on Theories of Grammar. Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Dąbrowska, E., and Street, J.. (2006). Individual differences in language attainment: Comprehension of passive sentences by native and non-native English speakers. Language Sciences, 28: 604–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahan, D., and Tanenhaus, M. K.. (2004). Continuous mapping from sound to meaning in spoken-language comprehension: evidence from immediate effects of verb-based constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 30: 498–513.Google Scholar
Dale, P. S. (1991). The validity of a parent report measure of vocabulary and syntax at 24 months. Journal of Speech and Hearing Sciences, 34: 565–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, P. S., Bates, E., Reznick, S. and Morisset, C.. (1989). The validity of a parent report instrument of child language at 20 months. Journal of Child Language, 16: 239–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D’Andrade, R. G. (1989). Cultural cognition. In Posner, M. I. (ed.), Foundations of Cognitive Science (pp. 795–830). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Daoutis, C., Franklin, A., Riddett, A., Clifford, A. and Davies, I. R. L.. (2006). Categorical effects in children’s colour search: a cross-linguistic comparison. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23: 1–29.Google Scholar
Davidoff, J., Davies, I. and Roberson, D.. (1999). Colour categories of a stone-age tribe. Nature, 398: 203–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, I., and Corbett, G.. (1998). A cross-cultural study of color-grouping: tests of the perceptual-physiology account of color universals. Ethos, 26/3: 338–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, I., and Corbett, G.. (1995). A practical field method for identifying probable Basic Color Terms. Languages of the World, 9/1: 25–36.Google Scholar
Davies, I., MacDermid, I. C., Corbett, G., Jerrett, D., Jerrett, T., McGurle, H. and Snowden, P. T.. (1992). Color terms in Setswana: a linguistic and perceptual approach. Linguistics, 30: 1065–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R. (2010). The Greatest Show on Earth. London: Black Swan.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1986). The Blind Watchmaker. New York: W. W. Norton and Co.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Deacon, T. (1997). The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain. New York: W. W. Norton and Co.Google Scholar
Dediu, D., and Levinson, S. C.. (2013). On the antiquity of language: the reinterpretation of Neandertal linguistic capacities and its consequences. Frontiers in Psychology, 4: 397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deutscher, G. (2011). Through the Language Glass: Why the World Looks Different in Other Languages. London: Arrow Books.Google Scholar
De Valois, R .L., and De Valois, K. K.. (1993). A multi-stage color model. Vision Research, 3: 1053–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Villiers, J. G., and De Villiers, P. A.. (1973). A cross-sectional study of the acquisition of grammatical morphemes in child speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2/3: 267–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drachman, D. (2005). Do we have brain to spare?Neurology, 64/12: 2004–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dronkers, N. F., Plaisant, O., Iba-Zizen, M. T. and Cabanis, E. A.. (2007). Paul Broca’s historic cases: high resolution MR imaging of the brains of Leborgne and Lelong. Brain, 130/5: 1432–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dryer, M. S. (1998). Why statistical universals are better than absolute universals. Chicago Linguistic Society 33: The Panels: 123–45.
Dryer, M. S., and Haspelmath, M.. (2011). The World Atlas of Language Structures Online ().
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1999). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 6/5: 178–90.3.0.CO;2-8>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996). Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language. London: Faber and Faber.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1992). Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 22/6: 469–984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, M., Greenhill, S. J., Levinson, S. C., and Gray, R. D.. (2011). Evolved structure of language shows lineage-specific trends in word-order universals. Nature, 473: 79–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural Darwinism: The Theory of Neuronal Group Selection. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. (2006). Language acquisition as rational contingency learning. Applied Linguistics, 27/1: 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elman, J., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Bates, E., Johnson, M. H., Parisi, D. and Plunkett, K.. (1996). Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K. (2002). Language, Cognition, and the Brain: Insights from Sign Language Research. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Enard, W., Przeworski, M., Fisher, S. E., Lai, C. S., Wiebe, V., Kitano, T., Monaco, A. P. and Pääbo, S.. (2002). Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature, 418/6900: 869–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enfield, N. (2004). Adjectives in Lao. In Dixon, R. M. W. and Aikhenvald, A. Y. (eds.), Adjective Classes: A Cross-linguistic Typology (pp. 323–47). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Evans, N., and Levinson, S. C.. (2009). The myth of language universals: language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32/5: 429–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, V. (2013). Language and Time: A Cognitive Linguistics Approach. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, V. (2009). How Words Mean: Lexical Concepts, Cognitive Models and Meaning Construction. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, V. (2004). The Structure of Time: Language, Meaning and Temporal Cognition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, V., and Green, M.. (2006). Cognitive Linguistics: An Introduction. Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Everett, D. (2013). 60 years of cognitive science. Lecture given to the British Academy, London, 4 October.
Everett, D. (2012). Language: The Cultural Tool. New York: Vintage.Google Scholar
Everett, D. (2005). Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã: another look at the design features of human language. Current Anthropology, 46: 621–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faarlund, J. T. (2001). From ancient Germanic to modern Germanic languages. In Haspelmath, M., König, E., Österreicher, W. and Reible, W. (eds.,), Language Typology and Language Universals, Vol. II (pp. 1706–19). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., Destro, M. F., Finos, L., Cotillon-Williams, N., Smith, A. T. and Castiello, U.. (2005). Language in shadow. Social Neuroscience, 1/2: 77–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fantz, R. L. (1963). Pattern vision in newborn infants. Science, 140/3564: 296–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Bates, E., Thal, D. and Pethick, S.. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. In Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J., Pethick, S. and Reilly, J.. (1993). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User’s Guide and Technical Manual. San Diego: Singular Press.Google Scholar
Finlay, B. L., and Darlington, R. B.. (1995). Linked regularities in the development and evolution of mammalian brains. Science, 268: 1578–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fishman, J. (2000). Can Threatened Languages be Saved? Reversing Language Shift, Revisited – A 21st Century Perspective. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. (2008). LOT2: The Language of Thought Revisited. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. (1998). Concepts: Where Cognitive Science Went Wrong. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. (1985). Précis of ‘Modularity of Mind’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8: 1–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. (1983). Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. (1975). The Language of Thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Foley, W. A. (1997). Anthropological Linguistics: An Introduction. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Fouts, R. S., Shapiro, G. L. and O’Neil, C.. (1978). Studies of linguistic behavior in apes and children. In Cavalieri, P. and Singer, P. (eds.), Understanding Language Through Sign Language Research (pp. 29–41). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fromkin, V., and Rodman, R.. (1997). An Introduction to Language (sixth edition). New York: Heinle and Heinle Inc.Google Scholar
Fromkin, V., Rodman, R. and Hyams, N.. (2013). An Introduction to Language (tenth international edition). New York: Heinle and Heinle Inc.Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. (2007). Learning organs [English original of L’apprentissage de matières distinctes exige des organes distincts]. In Bricmont, J. and Franc, J. (eds.), Cahier no 88: Noam Chomsky (pp. 181–7). Paris: L’Herne.Google Scholar
Gardner, B. T., and Gardner, R. A.. (1974). Comparing the early utterances of child and chimpanzee. In Pick, A. (ed.), Minnesota Symposium on Use of Signs by Chimpanzees with Humans: Child Psychology, Vol. VIII (pp. 3–23). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1985). The Mind’s New Science: A History of the Cognitive Revolution. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1974). The Shattered Mind. New York: Vintage.Google Scholar
Gardner, R. A., and Gardner, B. T.. (1969). Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science, 165: 664–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentner, D., and Goldin-Meadow, S.. (2003). Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Genter, T. Q., Fenn, K. M., Margoliash, D. and Nusbaum, H. C.. (2006). Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds. Nature 440: 1204–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerhardt, H. C. (1994). The evolution of vocalization in frogs and toads. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 25: 293–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. W., and van Orden, G. (2010). Adaptive cognition without massive modularity. Language and Cognition, 2: 149–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givon, T. (1984). Syntax Volume I. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M., and Kaschak, M. P.. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9: 558–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, E. M. (1967) Language identification in the limit. Information and Control, 10/5: 447–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (2011). Corpus evidence of the viability of statistical preemption. Cognitive Linguistics 22/1: 131–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (2006). Constructions at Work. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Hearing Gesture: How Our Hands Help Us Think. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., and Mylander, C.. (1990). Beyond the input given: the child’s role in the acquisition of language. Language, 66/2: 323–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, C. (2003). Conversation and Brain Damage. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Greenberg, J. (1986). On being a linguistic anthropologist. Annual Review of Anthropology, 15: 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E. and Wallace, C. S.. (1993). Experience and brain development. In Johnson, M. H. (ed.), Brain Development and Cognition: A Reader (pp. 290–322). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the Way of Words. Harvard: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Grodzinsky, Y., and Santi, A.. (2008). The battle for Broca’s region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12/12: 474–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27: 377–442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grush, R. (1997). The architecture of representation. Philosophical Psychology, 10/1: 5–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gumperz, J., and Levinson, S. C.. (1996). Rethinking Linguistic Relativity. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hardcastle, W. (1976). Physiology of Speech Production: Introduction for Speech Scientists. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D, 42: 335–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrington, A. (1987). Medicine, Mind and the Double Brain: A Study in Nineteenth-Century Thought. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N. and Fitch, W. T.. (2002). The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve?Science, 298: 1569–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinrich, B., and Pepper, J.. (1998). Influence of competitors on caching behaviour in the common raven (Corvus corax). Animal Behavior, 56: 1083–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herculano-Houzel, S. (2009). The human brain in numbers: a linearly scaled-up primate brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 09.031.CrossRef
Herman, L. M., Richards, D. G. and Wolz, J. P.. (1984). Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins. Cognition, 16: 129–219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hillix, W. A., and Rumbaugh, D. P.. (2004). Animal Bodies, Human Minds: Ape, Dolphin and Parrot Language Skills. New York: Kluwer Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Himmelmann, N. P. (1997). Deiktikon, Artikel, Nominalphrase: Zur Emergenz syntaktischer Struktur. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobbes, Thomas. ([1651] 2012). Leviathan. Critical edition by Malcolm, N. (3 volumes). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hockett, C. (1966). The problem of universals in language In Greenberg, J. (ed.), Universals of Language (pp. 1–29). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hötting, K., Rösler, F. and Röder, B.. (2004). Altered multisensory interaction in congenitally blind humans: an event-related potential study. Experimental Brain Research, 159: 370–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hull, D. (1989). The Metaphysics of Evolution. Stony Brook: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Hurford, J. (2012). The Origins of Grammar. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hurford, J. (2007). The Origins of Meaning. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hurvich, L. M., and Jameson, D.. (1957). An opponent-process theory of color vision. Psychological Review, 64/1–6: 384–404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M. and Shimpi, P.. (2002). Syntactic priming in young children. Journal of Memory and Language, 50: 182–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ibbotson, P. (2013). The scope of usage-based theory. Frontiers in Psychology, 4: 255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isenberg, N., Silbersweig, D., Engelien, A., Emmerich, S., Malavade, K., Beattie, B., Leon, A. C. and Stern, E.. (1999). Linguistic threat activates the human amygdala. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 96: 10456–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackendoff, R. (2002). Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, Evolution. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (1992). Languages of the Mind: Essays on Mental Representation. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (1987). Consciousness and the Computational Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (1983). Semantics and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jahn-Samilo, J., Goodman, J. C., Bates, E., Appelbaum, M. and Sweet, M.. (2000). Vocabulary Learning in Children from 8 to 30 Months of Age: A Comparison of Parental Report and Laboratory Measures. Project in Cognitive and Neural Development, Technical Report #00–06. La Jolla: University of California, San Diego.Google Scholar
Jakobson, J., and Halle, M. (1956). Fundamentals of Language. Amsterdam: Mouton.Google Scholar
Jelinek, E. (1995). Quantification in Straits Salish. In Bach, E., Jelinek, E., Kratzer, A. and Partee, B. (eds.), Quantification in Natural Languages (pp. 487–540). New York: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Jesperson, O. (1909). A Modern English Grammar on Historical Principles (Volume I). Heidelberg: Carl Winter.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. R. (1994). Cognitive abilities of children with language impairment. In Watkins, R. V. and Rice, M. L. (eds.), Specific Language Impairments in Children (pp. 107–21). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2001). Research into Williams syndrome: the state of the art. In Nelson, C. A. and Luciana, M. (eds.), Handbook of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience (pp. 691–700). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1994). Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Klima, E., Bellugi, U., Grant, J. and Baron-Cohen, S.. (1995). Is there a social module? Language, face processing, and theory of mind in individuals with Williams Syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7/2: 196–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, P. (1999). The emergence of Basic Color Lexicons hypothesis: a comment on ‘The vocabulary of colour with particular reference to ancient Greek and classical Latin’ by John Lyons. In Borg, A. (ed.), The Language of Color in the Mediterranean: An Anthology on Linguistic and Ethnographic Aspects of Color Terms (pp. 76–90). Stockholm: Almqvist and Wiksell.Google Scholar
Kay, P., and Berlin, B.. (1997). Science ≠ imperialism: response to Saunders and van Brakel. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20/2: 196–201.Google Scholar
Kay, P., Berlin, B., Maffi, L., Merrifield, W. R. and Cook, R.. (2009). The World Color Survey. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Kay, P., and Kempton, W.. (1984). What is the Sapir–Whorf hypothesis?American Anthropologist, 86: 65–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, P., and McDaniel, C. K.. (1978). The linguistic significance of the meanings of Basic Color Terms. Language, 54: 610–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, R. (1994). On Language Change: The Invisible Hand in Language. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Kellogg, W. N., and Kellogg, L. A.. (1933). The Ape and the Child: A Study of Environmental Influence Upon Early Behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible Action as Utterance. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klatzky, R. L., Pellegrino, J. W., McCloskey, B. P. and Doherty, S.. (1989). Can you squeeze a tomato? The role of motor representations in semantic sensibility judgments. Journal of Memory and Language, 28: 56–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klima, E. S., and Bellugi, U.. (1979). The Signs of Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (1994). Principles of Linguistic Change, Vol. I: Internal Factors. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Ladefoged, P., and Maddieson, I. (1996). The Sounds of the World’s Languages. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lai, C. S. L., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Vargha-Khadem, F. and Monaco, A. P.. (2001). A fork-head gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature, 413: 519–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. (2004). Don’t Think of an Elephant! Know Your Values and Frame the Debate – The Essential Guide for Progressives. New York: Chelsea Green Publishing.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M.. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and its Challenge to Western Thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M.. (1980). Metaphors We Live By. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (2008). Cognitive Grammar: A Basic Introduction. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (2002). Concept, Image and Symbol. Second Edition. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (2000). A dynamic usage-based model. In Barlow, M. and Kemmer, S. (eds.), Usage-based Models of Language (pp. 1–63). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Ledgeway, A. (2012). From Latin to Romance: Morphosyntactic Typology and Change. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ledgeway, A. (2011). Syntactic and morphosyntactic typology and change. In Ledgeway, A., Maiden, M. and Smith, J.-C. (eds.), The Cambridge History of the Romance Languages (pp. 382–471). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (1995). Emotion: clues from the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 46: 209–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, P. (1996). The Whorf Theory Complex: A Critical Reconstruction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M. (1991). The role of person and object in eliciting early imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51:423–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenneberg, E. H. (1953). Cognition in ethnolinguistics. Language, 29: 463–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with Specific Language Impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google ScholarPubMed
LePage, R. B., and Tabouret-Keller, A.. (1985). Acts of Identity: Creole-based Approaches to Ethnicity. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (2006). On the human ‘interaction engine’. In Enfield, N. J. and Levinson, S. C. (eds.), Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and Interaction (pp. 39–69). Oxford: Berg.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (2003). Space in Language and Cognition: Explorations in Cultural Diversity. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (2000). Yélî Dnye and the theory of basic color terms. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 10/1: 3–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (1996). Frames of reference and Molyneux’s question: cross-linguistic evidence. In Bloom, P., Peterson, M., Nadel, L. and Garrett, M. (eds.), Language and Space (pp. 109–69). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C., and Gray, R. D.. (2012). Tools from evolutionary biology shed new light on the diversification of languages. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16/3: 167–173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, P., and Gleitman, L.. (2002). Turning the tables: language and spatial reasoning. Cognition, 83/3: 265–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, P. (1984). The Biology and Evolution of Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P., Crelin, E. S. and Klatt, D. H.. (1972). Phonetic ability and related anatomy of the newborn and adult human, Neanderthal man, and the chimpanzee. In Quiatt, D. and Itani, J. (eds.), Hominid Culture in Primate Perspective (pp. 233–51). Boulder: University Press of Colorado.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P., Klatt, D. and Wilson, W.. (1969). Vocal tract limitations on the vowel repertoires of rhesus monkeys and other nonhuman primates. Science, 64/3884: 1185–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E. M., Cameron-Faulkner, T. and Tomasello, M.. (2003). A construction based analysis of child directed speech. Cognitive Science, 27: 843–73.Google Scholar
Lovgren, S. (2005). Chimps, humans 96 percent the same, gene study finds. National Geographic News, 31 Aug.
Lucy, J. A. (1997). The linguistics of color. In Hardin, C. L. and Maffi, L. (eds.), Color Categories in Language and Thought (pp. 320–46). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucy, J. A. (1992a). Language Diversity and Thought: A Reformulation of the Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucy, J. A. (1992b). Grammatical Categories and Cognition: A Case Study of the Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucy, J. A., and Schweder, R.. (1979). Whorf and his critics. American Anthropologist, 81: 581–615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lumsden, C., and Wilson, E. O.. (1981). Genes, Mind and Culture: The Co-evolutionary Process. London: World Scientific Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G. (2012a). Linguistically modulated perception and cognition: the label feedback hypothesis. Frontiers in Cognition, 3:54.Google ScholarPubMed
Lupyan, G. (2012b). What do words do? Toward a theory of language-augmented thought. In Ross, B. H. (ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. LVII (pp. 255–97). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luraghi, S. (2010). The rise (and possible downfall) of configurationality. In Luraghi, S. and Bubenik, V. (eds.), A Companion to Historical Linguistics (pp. 212–29). London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Lurz, R. W. (2011). Mindreading Animals: The Debate over What Animals Know about Other Minds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyons, J. (1995). Colour in language. In Lamb, T. and Bourriau, J. (eds.), Colour: Art and Science (pp. 194–224). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Macario, J. F. (1991). Young children’s use of colour in classification: foods and canonically coloured objects. Cognitive Development, 6: 17–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machery, E. (2007). Massive modularity and brain evolution. Philosophy of Science, 74: 825–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacLaury, R. (1992). From brightness to hue. Current Anthropology, 33/2: 137–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacNeilage, P. (2008). The Origin of Speech. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Maess, B., Koelsch, S., Gunter, T. C. and Friederici, A. D.. (2001). Musical syntax is processed in Broca’s area: an MEG study. Nature Neuroscience, 4: 540–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maffi, L. (1990). Somali color term evolution: grammatical and semantic evidence. Anthropological Linguistics, 33/3–4: 316–34.Google Scholar
Mahmoudzadeha, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Fourniera, M., Kongoloa, G., Goudjila, S., Dubois, J., Grebea, R. and Walloisa, F.. (2013). Syllabic discrimination in premature human infants prior to complete formation of cortical layers. PNAS, 19/110: 4846–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maratsos, M. (1984). How degenerate is the input to Creoles and where do its biases comes from?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7: 200–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marchman, V., and Bates, E.. (1994). Continuity in lexical and morphological development: a test of the critical mass hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 21: 339–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, G. F. (2006). Language: startling starlings. Nature, 440: 1117–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsaja, I. G. (2008). Desa Kolok - A Deaf Village and its Sign Language in Bali, Indonesia. Nijmegen: Ishara Press.Google Scholar
Martin, A., and Chao, L. L.. (2001). Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11: 194–201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maslin, K. T. (2001). An Introduction to the Philosophy of Mind. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Matthews, P. H. (1981). Syntax. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McGonigle, B. O., and Chalmers, M.. (1977). Are monkeys logical?Nature, 267/5613: 694–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGurk, H., and MacDonald, J.. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264/5588: 746–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal About Thought. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1966). Developmental psycholinguistics. In Smith, F. and Miller, G. A. (eds.), The Genesis of Language (pp. 15–84). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31: 838–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A., and Moore, M.. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198: 75–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mervis, C. B., and Bertrand, J.. (1993). Acquisition of early object labels: the roles of operating principles and input. In Kaiser, A. and Gray, D. B. (eds.), Enhancing Children’s Communication: Research Foundations for Intervention (pp. 287–316). Baltimore: Brookes.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B., Bertrand, J. and Pani, J. R.. (1975). Transaction of cognitive linguistic abilities and adult input: a case study of the acquisition of color terms and color-based subordinate object categories. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13:285–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merzenich, M. M. (1995). Cortical plasticity: shaped, distributed representations of learned behaviors. In Julesz, B. and Kovacs, I. (eds.), Maturational Windows and Cortical Plasticity in Human Development: Is There a Reason for an Optimistic View? (pp. 73–86). Reading, MA: Addison Wesley.Google Scholar
Mithen, S. (1996). The Prehistory of the Mind: A Search for the Origins of Art, Science and Religion. London: Phoenix.Google Scholar
Mody, M., Studdert-Kennedy, M. and Brady, S.. (1997). Speech perception deficits in poor readers: auditory processing or phonological coding?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64: 199–231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moss, A. E. (1989). Basic Color Terms. Lingua, 78: 313–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mukamel, R., Ekstrom, A. D., Kaplan, J., Iacoboni, M. and Fried, I.. (2010). Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Current Biology, 20: 750–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulcahy, N. J., and Call, J.. (2006). Apes save tools for future use. Science, 312/5776: 1038–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naigles, L. R., and Hoff-Ginsberg, E.. (1998). Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children’s early verb use. Journal of Child Language, 25: 95–120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nettle, D., and Romaine, S.. (2002). Vanishing Voices. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nichols, J., and Bickel, B.. (2005). Possessive classification and obligatory possessive inflection. In Haspelmath, M., Dryer, M. S., Gil, D. and Comrie, B. (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures (pp. 242–5). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nishimura, T., Mikami, A., Suzuki, J. and Matsuzawa, T.. (2003). Descent of the larynx in chimpanzee infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 100/12: 6930–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norris, T., Jacobsen, J. and Cerchio, S.. (2000). A Comparative Analysis of Humpback Whale Songs Recorded in Pelagic Waters of the Eastern North Pacific: Preliminary Findings and Implications for Discerning Migratory Routes and Assessing Breeding Stock Identity. NOAA Technical Memorandum, N0 AA-TM-NMFS-S W FSC-295. US Department of Commerce, National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration, National Marine Fisheries Service, Southwest Fisheries Science Center.Google Scholar
Ouattara, K., Lemasson, A. and Zuberbühler, K.. (2009). Campbell’s monkeys use affixation to alter call meaning. PLoS ONE, 4/11: e7808.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, F., and Linden, E.. (1985). The Education of Koko. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Payne, R. S., and McVay, S. (1971). Songs of humpback whales. Science, 173/ 3997: 585–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pederson, E. (1993). Geographic and manipulable space in two Tamil linguistic systems. In Frank, A. U. and Campari, I. (eds.), Spatial Information Theory (pp. 294–311). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (2000). The Alex Studies: Cognitive and Communicative Abilities of Grey Parrots. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1983). Cognition in the African Grey parrot: preliminary evidence for auditory/vocal comprehension of the class concept. Animal Learning Behavior, 11: 179–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perridon, H., and Sleeman, P.. (2011). The noun phrase in Germanic and Romance: common developments and differences. In Sleeman, P. and Perridon, H. (eds.), The Noun Phrase in Romance and Germanic: Structure, Variation, and Change (pp. 1–21). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, J. (2000). The phonetic grounding of phonology. Bulletin de la Communication Parlée, 5: 7–23.Google Scholar
Pika, S., Liebal, K., Call, J. and Tomasello, M.. (2005). Gestural communication of apes. Gesture, 5/1–2: 41–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (2007). The Stuff of Thought. New York: Viking Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2002). The Blank Slate. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2001). Words and Rules. London: Phoenix.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1997). How the Mind Works. New York: W. W. Norton and Co.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. New York: William Morrow.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pitchford, N. J., and Mullen, K. T.. (2002). Is the acquisition of Basic Colour Terms in young children constrained?Perception, 31: 1349–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plaza, M., Gatignol, P., Leroy, M. and Duffau, H.. (2009). Speaking without Broca’s area after tumor resection. Neurocase, 15/4: 294–310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pons, T. P., Garraghty, P. E., Ommaya, A. K., Kaas, J. H., Taub, E. and Mishkin, M. (1991). Massive cortical reorganization after sensory deafferentation in adult macaques. Science, 252/5014: 1857–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Popper, K. (1959). The Logic of Scientific Discovery. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Pourcel, S. (2005). Relativism in the linguistic and cognitive conceptualisation of motion events across verb-framed and satellite-framed languages. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis: Department of Linguistics, University of Durham.
Premack, D. (1986). Gavagai! Or the Future History of the Animal Language Controversy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Premack, D. (1974). Intelligence in Ape and Man. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Premack, D., and Premack, A. J.. (1983). The Mind of an Ape. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Prinz, J. (2006). Is the mind really modular? In Stainton, R. J. (ed.), Contemporary Debates in Cognitive Science (pp. 22–36). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Prinz, J. (2002). Furnishing the Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pullum, G. K., and Scholz, B. C.. (2002). Empirical assessment of stimulus of poverty arguments. The Linguistic Review, 19: 9–50.Google Scholar
Pulvermüller, F. (1999). Words in the brain’s languages. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22: 253–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pustejovsky, J. (1995). The Generative Lexicon. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Quartz, S. R. (2002). Toward a developmental evolutionary psychology: genes, development, and the evolution of the human cognitive architecture. In Scher, S. and Rauscher, M. (eds.), Evolutionary Psychology: Alternative Approaches (pp. 1–35). Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Radford, A. (1990). Syntactic Theory and the Acquisition of English Syntax: The Nature of Early Child Grammars of English. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Rakic, P. (1995). A small step for the cell, a giant leap for mankind: a hypothesis of neocortical expansion during evolution. Trends in Neuroscience, 18: 383–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Regier, T., and Kay, P.. (2009). Language, thought and color: Whorf was half right. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13/10: 439–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rendell, L. E., and Whitehead, H. (2003a). Vocal clans in sperm whales. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 270: 225–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rendell, L. E., and Whitehead, H. (2003b). Comparing repertoires of sperm whale codas: a multiple methods approach. Bioacoustics, 14: 61–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renfrew, C. (1990). Archaeology and Language: The Puzzle of Indo-European Origins. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L.. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27: 169–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberson, D., Davidoff, J., Davies, I. R. L. and Shapiro, L. R.. (2004). The development of color categories in two languages: a longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133: 554–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberson, D., Davies, I. R. L., Corbett, G. and Vandervyver, M.. (2005). Freesorting of colors across cultures: are there universal grounds for grouping?Journal of Cognition and Culture, 5: 349–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberson, D., Pak, H. S. and Hanley, J. R.. (2008). Categorical perception of colour in the left and right visual field is verbally mediated: evidence from Korean. Cognition, 107: 752–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosch, E. (1977). Human categorization. In Warren, N. (ed.), Advances in Cross-cultural Psychology, Vol. I (pp. 1–49). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rosch Heider, E. (1972). Probabilities, sampling, and ethnographic method: the case of Dani colour names. Man, 7: 448–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, H., and Rose, S.. (2001). Alas Poor Darwin: Arguments Against Evolutionary Psychology. New York: Vintage.Google Scholar
Rumbaugh, D. M. (1977). Language Learning by a Chimpanzee: The Lana Project. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Saffran, J., Aslin, R. and Newport, E. L.. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274/5294: 1926–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samarin, W. (1984). Socioprogrammed linguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7: 206–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sampson, G. (2001). The ‘Language Instinct’ Debate (revised edition). London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Sandler, W., Aronoff, M., Meir, I. and Padden, C.. (2011). The gradual emergence of phonological form in a new language. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 29/2: 503–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandler, W., Meir, I., Padden, C. and Aronoff, M.. (2005). The Emergence of Grammar in a New Sign Language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102/7: 2661–2665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapir, E. (1985). Selected Writings in Language, Culture, and Personality. Edited by Mandelbaum, D. G.. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Saunders, B. (2000). Revisiting basic color terms. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 6: 81–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saunders, B., and von Brakel, J.. (1997). Are there nontrivial constraints on colour categorization?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20: 167–228.Google ScholarPubMed
Saussure, F.. (2013). Course in General Linguistics. (New edition.) Translated and annotated by Harris, R.. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., and Lewin, R.. (1996). Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., McDonald, K., Sevcik, R. A., Hopkins, W. D. and Rubert, E.. (1986). Spontaneous symbol acquisition and communicative use by pygmy chimpanzees (pan paniscus). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115/3: 211–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmelz, M., Call, J. and Tomasello, M.. (2012). Chimpanzees predict that a competitor’s preference will match their own. Biology Letters, 9: 20120829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scholz, B. C., and Pullum, G. K.. (2006). Irrational nativist exuberance. In Stainton, R. (ed.), Contemporary Debates in Cognitive Science (pp. 59–80). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (1969). Speech Acts: An Essay in the Philosophy of Language. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Senghas, A. (2005). Language emergence: clues from a new Bedouin sign language. Current Biology, 15/12: 463–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seuren, P. (1984). The bioprogam hypothesis: fact and fancy. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 7: 208–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. and Marler, P.. (1980). Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science, 210: 801–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shatz, M., Behrend, D., Gelman, S. A. and Ebeling, K. S.. (1996). Colour term knowledge in two-year olds: evidence for early competence. Journal of Child Language, 23: 177–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shepherd, R. H. (1991). The perceptual organization of colors: an adaptation to regularities of the terrestrial world? In Barkow, J., Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (eds.), The Adapted Mind (pp. 495–532). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shinohara, K. (2000). Up–down orientation in time metaphors: analysis of English and Japanese. Manuscript, Tokyo University of Agriculture and Technology.Google Scholar
Shinohara, K., and Matsunaka, Y.. (2010). Frames of reference, effects of motion, and lexical meanings of Japanese front/back terms. In Evans, V. and Chilton, P. (eds.), Language, Cognition and Space: The State of the Art and New Directions (pp. 293–315). London: Equinox.Google Scholar
Singer Harris, N. G., Bellugi, U., Bates, E., Jones, W. and Rosen, M. J.. (1997). Contrasting profiles of language development in children with Williams and Down syndromes. In D. J. Thal and J. S. Reilly (eds.), Special issue on origins of language disorders. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13/3: 345–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinha, C. (1988). Language and Representation: A Socio-Naturalistic Approach to Human Development. Brighton: Harvester Press.Google Scholar
Sinha, C., da Silva Sinha, V., Zinken, J. and Sampaio, W.. (2011). When time is not space: the social and linguistic construction of time intervals and temporal event relations in an Amazonian culture. Language and Cognition, 3/1: 137–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skipper, J. I., Goldin-Meadow, S., Nusbaum, H. C. and Small, S. L.. (2007). Speech-associated gestures, Broca’s Area, and the human mirror system. Brain and Language, 101/3: 260–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C. (1997). Sign Language Companion: A Handbook of British Signs. London: Human Horizons.Google Scholar
Smith, N. (1999). Chomsky: Ideas and Ideals. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, N., and Tsimpli, I.-M.. (1995). The Mind of a Savant. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Spivey, M. J., Tyler, M., Richardson, D. C. and Young, E.. (2000). Eye movements during comprehension of spoken scene descriptions. In Gleitman, L. R. and Joshi, K. R. (eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-second Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 487–92). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stanfield, B. B., and O’Leary, D. D.. (1985). Fetal occipital cortical neurones transplanted to the rostral cortex can extend and maintain a pyramidal tract axon. Nature, 313/5998: 135–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanfield, R. A., and Zwaan, R. A.. (2001). The effect of implied orientation derived from verbal context on picture recognition. Psychological Science, 12: 153–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stone, V. E., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Kroll, N. and Knight, R. T.. (2002). Selective impairment of reasoning about social exchange in a patient with bilateral limbic system damage. PNAS, 99/17: 11531–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sur, M., Pallas, S. L. and Roe, A. W.. (1990). Cross-modal plasticity in cortical development: differentiation and specification of sensory neocortex. Trends in Neuroscience, 13: 227–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suttle, L., and Goldberg, E.. (2011). Partial productivity of constructions as induction. Linguistics, 49/6: 1237–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suzuki, R., Buck, J. R. and Tyack, P. L.. (2006). Information entropy of humpback whale songs. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119/3: 1849–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P., Miller, S., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Nagarajan, S., Schreiner, C., Jenkins, W. and Merzenich, M.. (1996). Language comprehension in learning impaired children improved with acoustically modified speech. Science, 271: 81–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talmy, Leonard. (2000). Toward a Cognitive Semantics (2 volumes). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, J. A. (2012). The Mental Corpus: How Language is Represented in the Mind. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, L. J., and Zwaan, R. A.. (2009). Action in cognition: the case of language. Language and Cognition, 1: 45–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terrace, H. (1979). Nim. New York: Kopf.Google Scholar
Terrace, H., Petitto, L. A., Sanders, R. J. and Bever, T. G.. (1979). Can an ape create a sentence?Science, 206/4421: 891–902.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thal, D. J., and Katich, J.. (1996). Predicaments in early identification of specific language impairment: does the early bird always catch the worm? In Thal, D. J., Cole, K. N. and Phillip, D. (eds.), Assessment of Communication and Language (pp. 1–28). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V., Pine, J. and Rowland, C.. (2004). Semantic generality, input frequency and the acquisition of syntax. Journal of Child Language, 31: 61–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E. (1995). Motor development: a new synthesis. The American Psychologist, 50/2: 79–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E., and Smith, L. B.. (1994). A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thierry, G., Athanasopoulos, P., Wiggett, A., Dering, B. and Kuipers, J.-R.. (2009). Unconscious effects of language-specific terminology on preattentive color perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 106/11: 4567–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, E. (1995). Colour Vision. A Study in Cognitive Science and the Philosophy of Perception. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2011). Human culture in evolutionary perspective. In Gelfand, M. (ed.), Advances in Culture and Psychology (pp. 5–51). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2008). The Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1992). First Verbs: A Case Study of Early Grammatical Development. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., and Call, J.. (1997). Primate Cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., and Herrmann, E.. (2010). Ape and human cognition: what’s the difference?Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19/3: 3–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., and Slobin, D. I.. (2004). Beyond Nature–Nurture: Essays in Honor of Elizabeth Bates. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Townsend, J., Wulfeck, B., Nichols, S. and Koch, L.. (1995). Attentional deficits in children with developmental language disorder. Technical report CND-9513. Center for Research in Language, University of California at San Diego.Google Scholar
Trask, R. L. (1996). Historical Linguistics. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity. In Bullowa, M. (ed.), Before Speech: The Beginning of Human Communication (pp. 321–47). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tyler, A., and Evans, V.. (2003). The Semantics of English Prepositions: Spatial Scenes, Embodied Experience and Cognition. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uttal, W. R. (2001). The New Phrenology: The Limits of Localizing Cognitive Processes in the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Van de Velde, F. (2011). Left-peripheral expansion of the English NP. English Language and Linguistics, 15: 387–415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van de Velde, F. (2010). The emergence of the determiner in the Dutch NP. Linguistics, 48: 263–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van de Velde, F. (2009a). De Nominale Constituent: Structuur en Geschiedenis. Leuven University Press.Google Scholar
Van de Velde, F. (2009b). The emergence of modification patterns in the Dutch noun phrase. Linguistics, 47: 1021–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Valin, R. D., and LaPolla, R. J. (1997). Syntax: Structure, Meaning, and Function. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varela, F., Thompson, E. and Rosch, E.. (1991). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K., Alcock, K., Fletcher, P. and Passingham, R.. (1995). Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large family with a genetically transmitted speech and language disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 92: 930–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Von Frisch, K. (1976). Bees: Their Vision, Chemical Senses, and Language (revised edition). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Von Frisch, K. (1953). The Dancing Bees: An Account of the Life and Senses of the Honey Bee. New York: Harvest Books.Google Scholar
Wallace, B. (2010). Getting Darwin Wrong: Why Evolutionary Psychology Won’t Work. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Whorf, B.-L. (1956). Language, Thought and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Edited by Carroll, John B.. Cambridge, MA.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (1996). Semantics: Primes and Universals. Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Wills, C. (1991). Exons, Introns, and Talking Genes: The Science Behind the Human Genome Project. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Winawer, J., Witthoft, N., Frank, M. C., Wu, L., Wade, A. R. and Boroditsky, L.. (2007). Russian blues reveal effects of language on color discrimination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 104/19: 7780–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wynn, K. (1995). Origins of numerical knowledge. Mathematical Cognition, 1: 35–60.Google Scholar
Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358: 749–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yip, M. (2006). The search for phonology in other species. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10/10: 442–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zwaan, R. A. (2004). The immersed experiencer: toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In Ross, B. H. (ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (pp. 35–62). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., and Yaxley, R. H.. (2003). Hemispheric differences in semantic-relatedness judgments. Cognition, 87: B79–B86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Vyvyan Evans, Bangor University
  • Book: The Language Myth
  • Online publication: 05 October 2014
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107358300.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Vyvyan Evans, Bangor University
  • Book: The Language Myth
  • Online publication: 05 October 2014
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107358300.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Vyvyan Evans, Bangor University
  • Book: The Language Myth
  • Online publication: 05 October 2014
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107358300.010
Available formats
×